Psychology Wiki
(New page: {{CompPsy}} thumb|Tool use by a [[Gorilla]] thumb|An [[adult female gorilla using a stick to gage the depth of water]]...)
 
(Copied from wikipedia entry)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{CompPsy}}
 
{{CompPsy}}
  +
{{About|the use of tools by non-human animals|tool use by humans|Technology|and|Tools}}
[[Image:Gorrila tool use-Efi.jpg|thumb|Tool use by a [[Gorilla]]]]
 
  +
{{Ethology}}
[[Image:Gorilla tool use.png|thumb|An [[adult]] [[female]] gorilla using a stick to gage the depth of water]]
 
[[Image:Chimpanzee and stick.jpg|thumb|A [[chimpanzee]] gathering food with a stick]]
 
   
  +
[[Tool]]s are used by some animals to perform behaviours including the acquisition of food and water, [[Personal grooming|grooming]], defence, [[Play (activity)|recreation]] or [[animal architecture|construction]].<ref name="Hansell2005">{{cite book|author=Michael Henry Hansell|title=Animal architecture|url=http://books.google.com/books?id=WdIIbokXxDcC|accessdate=29 June 2011|year=2005|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-850752-9|page=1}}</ref> Originally thought to be a skill only possessed by [[human]]s, some tool use requires a sophisticated level of cognition. There is considerable discussion about the definition of what constitutes a tool and therefore which behaviours can be considered as true examples of tool use. A wide range of animals is considered to use tools including mammals, birds, fish, cephalopods and insects.
Some animals, especially [[primates]], use tools to perform simple tasks such as getting food or [[grooming]].
 
   
  +
Rarely, animals have been observed making their own tools, e.g. primates sharpening a stick to use as a weapon,<ref name="Roach" /> or removing leaves and twigs from a branch and fishing for termites with a stem frayed by chewing.<ref>{{cite web|title=Tool use in chimpanzees|url=http://www.onekind.org/be_inspired/animal_sentience/tool_use/tool_use_in_chimpanzees/|work=OneKind|accessdate=February 1, 2014|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20130820180455/http://www.onekind.org/be_inspired/animal_sentience/tool_use/tool_use_in_chimpanzees/|archivedate=20 August 2013}}</ref>
== Types of tools ==
 
   
  +
== Definitions and terminology ==
There are some different definitions of a tool, such as:
 
  +
The key to identifying tool use is defining what constitutes a tool. Researchers of animal behavior have arrived at different formulations.
   
  +
In 1980, Beck published a widely used definition of tool use.<ref>Beck, B., (1980). ''Animal Tool Behaviour: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals'' Garland STPM Pub.</ref> More recently, this has been modified to -
{{quote|''An object that has been modified to fit a purpose' or 'An inanimate object that one uses or modifies in some way to cause a change in the environment, thereby facilitating ones achievement of a target goal'.|Hauser, 2000<ref>Hauser, 2000</ref>}}
 
   
  +
{{quote|"The external employment of an unattached or manipulable attached environmental object to alter more efficiently the form, position, or condition of another object, another organism, or the user itself, when the user holds and directly manipulates the tool during or prior to use and is responsible for the proper and effective orientation of the tool.<ref name="Shumaker">Shumaker, R.W., Walkup, K.R. and Beck, B.B., (2011). ''Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals'' Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore</ref>}}
{{quote|''the use of physical objects other than the animal's own body or appendages as a means to extend the physical influence realized by the animal''|Jones and Kamil, 1973<ref name="Jones and Kamil">Jones, T. B. & Kamil, A. C. 1973 Tool-making and tool-using in the northern blue jay. Science 180, 1076–1078.</ref>}}
 
   
  +
Other, briefer definitions have been proposed -
The most common are sticks or bits of stone.
 
   
  +
{{quote|"An object carried or maintained for future use"|Finn, Tregenza, and Norman, 2009.<ref>{{Cite journal|first1=Julian K. |last1 = Finn|first2=Tom|last2=Tregenza|first3=Mark D.|last3=Norman|year=2009|title=Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus|journal=Curr. Biol.|volume=19|issue=23|pages=R1069–R1070|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.052}}</ref>}}
== Uses ==
 
   
  +
{{quote|"The use of physical objects other than the animal's own body or appendages as a means to extend the physical influence realized by the animal"|Jones and Kamil, 1973<ref name="Jones and Kamil">Jones, T. B. & Kamil, A. C. 1973 Tool-making and tool-using in the northern blue jay. ''Science'' 180, 1076–1078.</ref>}}
Tool use implies an animal has knowledge of the relationship between objects and their effects.
 
   
  +
{{quote|"An object that has been modified to fit a purpose" or "An inanimate object that one uses or modifies in some way to cause a change in the environment, thereby facilitating one's achievement of a target goal".|Hauser, 2000<ref>{{cite book|title=The Oxford Handbook of Animal Ethics|page=232|editors=Tom L. Beauchamp and R.G. Frey|publisher=Oxford University Press|year=2011|isbn=9780195371963}}</ref>}}
If an object is placed out of reach on a towel that itself is in reach, dogs and children will pull the towel to bring the object closer to them. But does this show knowledge about the nature of the world (declarative memory) or recall of rules already learnt (procedural)?
 
   
  +
Others, for example Lawick-Goodall,<ref>Lawick-Goodall, J.V., (1970). Tool using in primates and other vertebrates in Lehrman, D.S, Hinde, R.A. and Shaw, E. (Eds) ''Advances in the Study of Behaviour, Vol. 3.'' Academic Press.</ref> distinguish between "tool use" and "object use".
* Sticks can be used to break into termite nests for food or even to fight rivals. They are sometimes used for grooming.
 
* Stones can be used, again, to fight rivals. However, they may also be used to carve bits of wood by more intelligent animals.
 
   
  +
Different terms have been given to the tool according to whether the tool is altered by the animal. If the "tool" is not held or manipulated by the animal in any way, such as an immobile [[anvil]], objects in a [[Bowerbird]]'s bower, or a [[heron]] using discarded crackers as bait to catch [[fish]], it is sometimes referred to as a '''"proto-tool"'''. Several studies in [[primates]] and [[birds]] have found that tool use is correlated with an enlargement of the brain as a whole or of particular regions. For example, true tool-using birds have relatively larger brains than proto-tool users.<ref name="Map Of Life" />
Some species, such as the [[Woodpecker Finch]] of the [[Galapagos Islands]], use particular tools as an essential part of their [[foraging]] behavior. However, these behaviors are often quite inflexible and cannot be applied effectively in new situations. Several species have now been shown to be capable of more flexible tool use. A well known example is Jane Goodall's observation of chimpanzees "fishing" for [[termite]]s in their natural environment, and captive great apes are often observed to use tools effectively; several species of [[corvid]]s have also been trained to use tools in controlled experiments, or use bread crumbs for bait-fishing.<ref>[http://www.orenhasson.com/EN/bait-fishing.htm Bait fishing]</ref>
 
   
  +
When an animal uses a tool that acts on another tool, this has been termed use of a '''"meta-tool"'''. For example, [[New Caledonian Crow]]s will spontaneously use a short tool to obtain an otherwise inaccessible longer tool that then allows them to extract food from a hole.<ref name="Map Of Life" /> Similarly, [[Black-striped capuchin|bearded capuchin]] monkeys will use smaller stones to loosen bigger quartz pebbles embedded in conglomerate rock, which they subsequently use as tools.<ref name="Mannu" />
=== Tool use by specific groups of animals ===
 
====Primates====
 
Research in [[2007]] shows that [[chimpanzee]]s in the [[Fongoli]] [[savannah]] sharpen sticks to use as [[spear]]s when hunting, considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans.<ref>http://desmoinesregister.com/apps/pbcs.dll/article?AID=/20070223/NEWS/702230385/-1/NEWS04</ref><ref>[http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2007/02/070222-chimps-spears.html Chimps Use "Spears" to Hunt Mammals, Study Says<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> It has also been observed in the 1970s that some chimpanzees/bonobos use sticks as probes to collect ants and termites. Also they have been observed cutting down the stick with their fingers and teeth so that it can fit into a hole in the ants' nest. They have even been observed using two tools, a stick to dig into the ant nest and a 'brush' made from grass stems with their teeth to collect the ants.<ref>The Human Ape</ref> In west Africa chimpanzees have been observed banging nuts against a stone in order to crack it.<ref>http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5Cp7_In7f88&feature=related</ref> Some troops use another stone whilst others use wooden clubs (heavy sticks). In one troop of chimpanzees it was observed that a female was using a stick to break into a bee hive to acquire honey.<ref>The Human Ape</ref> In an experiment a group of chimpanzees were presented with a model leopard with a moving head. There was soon commotion as leopards are one of the chimpanzees' predators. They were then observed clubbing the model with heavy quarterstaffs (fallen trees and/or branches). They continued doing this until the moving head had fallen off.<ref>http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bKpZUsRJWBg&feature=related</ref>
 
   
  +
Rarely, animals may use one tool followed by another, for example, bearded capuchins use stones and sticks, or two stones.<ref name="Mannu" /> This is called '''"associative"''', '''"secondary"''' or '''"sequential"''' tool use.<ref>Mulcahy, N.J., Call, J. and Dunbar, R.I.B., (2005). Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. ''Journal of Comparative Psychology'', 119: 23–32</ref>
Gorillas have been observed to (as shown above) use sticks to measure the depth of water.
 
   
  +
Some animals use other individuals in a way which could be interpreted as tool use, for example, ants crossing water over a bridge of other ants, or [[weaver ants]] using conspecifics to glue leaves together. These have been termed '''"social tools"'''.<ref>Pierce, J.D., (1986). A review of tool use in insects. The Florida Entomologist, 69: 95-104</ref>
Orangutans have also been observed to use sticks to measure the depth of water. It has also been observed that Orangutans in Sumatra use sticks to acquire seeds from a certain fruit. This is because the lining of the inside of the fruit has hairs that sting. On the island of Kaja a male Orangutan was observed using a pole to acquire fish from a net after observing local humans spear fishing.<ref>{{cite news | publiser=The Daily Mail1 | url=http://www.dailymail.co.uk/pages/live/articles/technology/technology.html?in_article_id=562236&in_page_id=1766&ito=1490 | title=Orangutan attempts to hunt fish with spear | date=[[2008-04-26]]}}</ref>.
 
   
  +
===Borderline examples===
Capuchins have also been observed banging nuts between two stones in order to crack it.<ref>{{cite journal |title=Substrate and Tool Use by Brown Capuchins in Suriname: Ecological Contexts and Cognitive Bases |author=Boinski, S., Quatrone, R. P. & Swartz, H. |year=2008 |volume=102 |issue=4 |pages=741–761 |doi=10.1525/aa.2000.102.4.741 |journal=American Anthropologist}}</ref> They have also been observed using stones to crack into shells.{{Fact|date=October 2008}} In captivity Capuchins have been observed during experiment to make flint knives after banging a piece of flint against the floor until it broke and using the smaller, sharper pieces to cut into a box with fruit. Some scientists believe that this (the above) isn't because of some higher intelligence in Capuchins but an interesting side effect of their aggressive and destructive nature.{{Fact|date=October 2008}}
 
  +
'''Play:''' Play has been defined as “activity having no immediate benefits and structurally including repetitive or exaggerated actions that may be out of sequence or disordered”.<ref name="Oinuma">Oinuma, C., (2008). ''Octopus mercatoris'' response behavior to novel objects in a laboratory setting: Evidence of play and tool use behavior? In, ''Octopus Tool Use and Play Behavior.''[http://nature.berkeley.edu/classes/es196/projects/2008final/Oinuma_2008.pdf]</ref> When play is discussed in relation to manipulating objects, it is often used in association with the word "tool".<ref>{{cite book|title=The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play|last1=Bjorklung|first1=David F.|last2=Gardiner|first2=Amy K.|chapter=Object Play and Tool Use: Developmental and Evolutionary Perspectives|page=153|isbn=9780195393002|editor=Anthony D. Pellegrini|publisher=Oxford University Press|year=2011}}</ref> Some birds, notably crows, parrots and birds of prey "play" with objects, many of them playing in flight with such items as stones, sticks, leaves, by letting them go and catching them again before they reach the ground. A few species repeatedly drop stones, apparently for the enjoyment of the sound effects.<ref name="Boswall" /> Many other species of animals, both avian and non-avian, play with objects in a similar manner and this appears to be consistent with the definition of tool use by Shumaker et al. given above.<ref name="Shumaker"/> However, if this is the case, there appears no logical reason to exclude play behaviour and object manipulation by domestic pets such as cats, dogs and horses, making them potentially, tool-users.{{or|date=November 2013}}
   
  +
'''Fixed "devices:"''' The impaling of prey on thorns by many of the [[Shrikes]] (''Laniidae'') is well known.<ref>{{cite book|title=Birds of Southern California's Deep Canyon|last=Weathers|first=Wesley|page=189|year=1983|publisher=University of California Press|isbn=9780520047549}}</ref> Several other birds may use spines or forked sticks to anchor a carcass while they flay it with the bill. It has been concluded that "This is an example of a fixed device which serves as an extension of the body, in this case, talons" and is thus a true form of tool use. On the other hand, the use of fixed skewers may not be true tool-use because the thorn (or whatever) is not manipulated by the bird.<ref name="Boswall" /> [[Leopard]]s perform a similar behaviour by dragging carcasses up trees and caching them in the forks of branches.
<gallery>
 
Image:BonoboFishing01.jpeg|A [[Bonobo]] using a stick to 'fish' for termites in [[San Diego Zoo]]
 
Image:BonoboFishing04.jpeg|Stage 2
 
Image:BonoboFishing02.jpeg|Stage 3
 
Image:BonoboFishing05.jpeg|Extracting the insects
 
</gallery>
 
{{clear}}
 
   
  +
'''Use of baits:''' Several species of [[heron]]s such as the [[Striated Heron]] (''Butorides striatus'') will place bread in the water to attract fish.<ref name="Boswall" /><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.stanford.edu/group/stanfordbirds/text/essays/Tool_Using.html|title=Birds of Stanford: Tool Using|publisher=Stanford University|accessdate=2013-205-23}}</ref> Whether this is tool use is disputed because the bread is not manipulated or held by the bird.
====Birds====
 
[[Image:Egyptian vulture in flight.jpg|thumb|An [[Egyptian vulture]] in flight]]
 
Many [[bird]]s have been shown as capable of using tools.
 
By Jones and Kamil's definition above, an [[Egyptian vulture]] dropping a bone on a rock would not be using a tool since the rock cannot be seen as an extension of the body. However the use of a rock manipulated using the beak to crack an [[ostrich]] egg would qualify the Egyptian vulture as a tool user. [[:Category:Tool-using species|Many other species]], including [[parrot]]s, [[corvid]]s and a range of [[passerine]]s, have been noted as tool users.<ref name=emery>Nathan J. Emery (2006) Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B (2006) 361, 23–43 [http://www.princeton.edu/~asifg/braindiversity/Emery%20-%20Cognitive%20Ornithology.pdf]</ref>
 
   
  +
== Learning and cognition ==
[[New Caledonian Crow]]s have been observed in the wild to use stick tools with their beaks to extract [[insect]]s from logs. While young birds in the wild normally learn this technique from elders, a laboratory [[crow]] named "Betty" improvised a hooked tool from a wire with no prior experience.<ref>[http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2002/08/0808_020808_crow.html Crow making tools]</ref> The [[Woodpecker Finch]] from the [[Galapagos Islands]] also uses simple stick tools to assist it in obtaining food. In [[captivity]], a young [[Cactus Finch]] learned to imitate this behaviour by watching a Woodpecker Finch in an adjacent [[Birdcage|cage]]. Crows in urban [[Japan]] have innovated a technique to crack hard-shelled [[Nut (fruit)|nut]]s by dropping them onto cross walks and letting them be run over and cracked by cars. They then retrieve the cracked nuts when the cars are stopped at the red light. [[Striated Heron]]s (''Butorides striatus'') and [[Hooded Crow]]s (''Corvus cornix'') use bait to catch fish<ref name-Norris>Norris (1975), Boswall (1983), Walsh et al. (1985), Robinson (1994)</ref><ref>{{cite web | url=http://www.orenhasson.com/EN/bait-fishing.htm | title=Bait-Fishing in Crows | accessdate=2009-2-7|}}</ref>.
 
  +
Tool use by animals may indicate different levels of [[learning]] and [[Animal cognition|cognition]].
{{clear}}
 
   
  +
For some animals, tool use is largely instinctive and inflexible. For example, the [[Woodpecker Finch]] of the [[Galápagos Islands]] use twigs or spines as an essential and regular part of its [[foraging]] behaviour, however, these behaviours are often quite inflexible and are not applied effectively in different situations. Other tool use, e.g. [[chimpanzees]] using twigs to "fish" for [[termites]], may be developed by [[Observational learning|watching others use tools]] and may even be a true example of animal teaching. Tools may even be used in solving puzzles in which the animal appears to experience a [[Eureka effect|"Eureka moment"]].
[[Image:Bottlenose dolphin mother and juvenile.jpg|thumb|left|A [[mother]] and [[juvenile]] [[Bottlenose Dolphin]] head to the sea floor]]
 
   
====Cetaceans====
+
==In mammals==
{{As of|2005}}, scientists have observed limited groups of [[Bottlenose Dolphins]] around the [[Australian Pacific]] using a basic tool. When searching for food on the [[sea]] floor, many of these dolphins were seen tearing off pieces of [[sea sponge|sponge]] and wrapping them around their "bottle nose" to prevent abrasions.<ref>{{cite web | url=http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/102/25/8939 | title=Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins | accessdate=2006-10-24|}}</ref>
 
   
====Elephants====
+
===Primates===
  +
[[File:Gorilla tool use.png|thumb|right|250px|A gorilla using a stick possibly to gauge the depth of water]]
[[Image:Anakotta1.jpg|thumb|[[Elephant]]s in a reserve]]
 
  +
'''Tool use''' has been reported many times in both wild and captive primates, particularly the great apes. The use of tools by primates is varied and includes hunting (mammals, invertebrates, fish), collecting honey, processing food (nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds), collecting water, weapons and shelter.
[[Elephant]]s show a remarkable ability to use [[tools]], despite having no hands. Instead, they use their trunk like an arm. Elephants have been observed digging holes to drink water and then ripping bark from a tree, chewing it into the shape of a ball, filling in the hole and covering over it with sand to avoid [[evaporation]]. The elephant later went back to this spot for a drink. They also often use branches to swat flies or scratch themselves.<ref>{{cite journal
 
| last = Holdrege
 
| first = Craig
 
| title = Elephantine Intelligence
 
| journal = In Context
 
| issue = 5
 
| publisher = The Nature Institute
 
| date = Spring 2001
 
| url = http://www.natureinstitute.org/pub/ic/ic5/elephant.htm
 
| accessdate = 2007-10-30 }}</ref>.
 
Elephants have also been known to drop very large rocks onto an [[electric fence]] to either ruin the fence or cut off the [[electricity]]<ref name=Poole>{{cite book
 
| last = Poole
 
| first = Joyce
 
| title = Coming of Age with Elephants
 
| publisher = Trafalgar Square
 
| date = 1996
 
| location = [[Chicago, Illinois]]
 
| pages = 131-133, 143-144, 155-157
 
| isbn = 034059179X}}</ref>
 
   
  +
In 1960, [[Jane Goodall]] observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a [[termite]] mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to “fish” or "dip" for termites.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute1">{{cite web|url=http://www.janegoodall.org/chimpanzees/tool-use-hunting-other-discoveries|title=Tool use, hunting & other discoveries|publisher=The Jane Goodall Institute|accessdate=August 1, 2013}}</ref>
====Mustelids====
 
  +
Sea otters have been observed using stones to hammer abalone shells off the rocks. They hammer at a rate of 180 blows per minute and do it in three dives.
 
  +
'''Tool manufacture''' is much rarer than simple tool use and probably represents higher cognitive functioning. Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute1" /> In 1990, it was claimed the only primate to manufacture tools in the wild was the [[chimpanzee]].<ref name="Boesch">{{cite journal|author=Boesch, C. and Boesch, H.|year=1990 |title=Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. |journal=Folia Primatol., 54: 86-99 |url=http://www.eva.mpg.de/primat/staff/boesch/pdf/fol_prim_tool_use_making.pdf}}</ref> However, since then, several primates have been reported as tool makers in the wild.<ref>{{cite book|title=The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution|last1=Boesch|first1=Christophe|last2=Boesch-Achermann|first2=Hedwige|page=192|year=2000|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=9780198505082}}</ref>
{{clear}}
 
  +
  +
Both bonobos and chimpanzees have been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water and using these for grooming. Sumatran orangutans will take a live branch, remove twigs and leaves and sometimes the bark, before fraying or flattening the tip for use on ant or bees.<ref name=van/> In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at [[Chester Zoo]] (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.<ref name="Gill2011">{{cite web | url = http://www.bbc.co.uk/nature/14227783 | title = Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool | first = Victoria | last = Gill | publisher = BBC | date = July 22, 2011 | accessdate = August 11, 2013}}</ref> Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.<ref name="Vancatova">{{cite web|url=http://www.rozhlas.cz/therevealed/comments/_zprava/488947|author=Vancatova, M.|title=Gorillas and Tools - Part I|year=2008|accessdate=August 4, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Chimpanzees====
  +
Common [[chimpanzees]] (''Pan troglodytes'') are sophisticated tool users with behaviors including cracking nuts with stone tools and fishing for ants or termites with sticks. There are more limited reports of the closely related [[bonobo]] (''Pan paniscus'') using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity,<ref name="ApeTag">{{cite web|url=http://www.clemetzoo.com/apetag/Bonobos.html|title=Bonobos|publisher=ApeTag|year=2010|accessdate=August 3, 2013}}</ref> It has been reported that both female chimpanzees and bonobos use tools more avidly than males.<ref>{{cite journal|authors=Gruber, T., Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. |date=2010 |title=A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the ''Pan'' lineage |journal=Animal Behaviour, 80: 1023-1033 |url=http://www.emory.edu/LIVING_LINKS/publications/articles/Gruber_etal_2010.pdf}}</ref> Wild chimpanzees predominantly use tools in the context of food acquisition while wild bonobos appear to use tools mainly for personal care (cleaning, protection from rain) and social purposes.
  +
  +
====Orangutans====
  +
[[Orangutans]] (genus ''Pongo'') were first observed using tools in the wild in 1994 in the northwest corner of Sumatra.<ref>{{cite web|url=
  +
http://www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/episodes/orangutans-just-hangin-on/orangutan-tools/2263/|title=Just Hangin' on: Orangutan tools|publisher=Nature|accessdate=August 5, 2013}}</ref> As with the chimpanzees, orangutans use tools made from branches and leaves to scratch, scrape, wipe, sponge, swat, fan, hook, probe, scoop, pry, chisel, hammer, cover, cushion and amplify. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30&nbsp;cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites.<ref name=van>van Schaik, C., Fox, E. and Sitompul, A., (1996). Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. ''Naturwissenschaften'', 83: 186–188</ref><ref name="OrangutanIslands">{{cite web|url=http://orangutanislands.com/sumatra-orangutans.htm|title=Sumatran orangutans|publisher=OrangutanIslands.com|accessdate=August 2, 2013}}</ref> Sumatran orangutans use a variety of tools - up to 54 types for extracting insects or honey, and as many as 20 types for opening or preparing fruits such as the hard to access ''Neesia Malayana''.<ref name=sumatran>{{cite book|title=World Atlas of Great Apes and Their Conservation|editors=Julian Oliver Caldecott, Lera Miles|page=189|publisher=University of California Press|year=2005|isbn=9780520246331}}</ref>
  +
They also use an 'autoerotic tool' - a stick which they use to stimulate the
  +
genitals and masturbate (both male and female).<ref>http://www.aim.uzh.ch/Research/orangutannetwork/CultureList.html</ref><ref>
  +
Anne E. Russon, Carel P. van Schaik, Purwo Kuncoro, Agnes
  +
Ferisa, Dwi P. Handayani and Maria A. van Noordwijk
  +
''Innovation and intelligence in orangutans,'' Chapter 20 in
  +
Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation, ed.
  +
Wich, Serge A.,
  +
Oxford University Press, 2009
  +
ISBN 9780199213276, p.293</ref> In parts of Borneo, orangutans use handfuls of leaves as napkins to wipe their chins while orangutans in parts of Sumatra use leaves as gloves, helping them handle spiny fruits and branches, or as seat cushions in spiny trees.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.orangutan.org/orangutan-facts/orangutan-behavior|title=Orangutan behavior|publisher=Orangutan International Foundation|accessdate=August 5, 2013}}</ref> There have been reports that individuals in both captivity and in the wild use tools held between the lips or teeth, rather than in the hands.<ref>O’Malley R.C. and McGrew W.C., (2000). Oral tool use by captive orangutans (''Pongo pygmaeus''). Folia Primatol., 71: 334–341. {{DOI|10.1159/000021756}}</ref> In captivity, orangutans have been taught to chip stone handaxes.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://orangutan.org/OFI/orangutans/orangutan-behaviour/|title=Orangutan behavior|accessdate=March 30, 2014|publisher=Orangutan Foundation International}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Gorillas====
  +
There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild.<ref>{{cite book|title=Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees|editors=Tetsurō Matsuzawa and Masaki Tomonaga|page=398|year=2006|publisher=2006|isbn=9784431302483}}</ref> An adult female [[Western lowland gorilla]] (''Gorilla gorilla gorilla'') used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge. One possible explanation for the absence of observed tool use in wild gorillas is that they are less dependent on foraging techniques that require the use of tools, since they exploit food resources differently from chimpanzees. Whereas chimpanzees and orangutans feeding involves tools such as hammers to crack open nuts and sticks to fish for termites, gorillas access these foods by breaking nuts with their teeth and smashing termite mounds with their hands.<ref>Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M. and Fishlock, V., (2005). First observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLoS Biol. Nov;3(11):e380 [http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.0030380]</ref>
  +
  +
Among the remaining primates, capuchin monkeys, baboons, mandrills and crab-eating macaques are known to use tools, either in captivity or in the wild.
  +
  +
====Capuchin monkeys====
  +
The [[black-striped capuchin]] (''Sapajus libidinosus'') was the first non-ape primate for which tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone.<ref name="Fragaszy" /> [[Robust capuchin monkey]]s (genus Sapajus) use stone tools in the wild.<ref name="Fragaszy">Fragaszy, D., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E.B., de Oliveira, M.G., (2004). Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. ''American Journal of Primatology'', 64: 359–366. {{DOI|10.1002/ajp.20085}}</ref><ref name="Alfaro">Lynch Alfaro, J.W., Silva, J.S. and Rylands, A.B., (2012). How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? an argument for the use of ''Sapajus'' and ''Cebus''. ''American Journal of Primatology'', 74(4): 273-286. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.222007 [http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajp.22007/abstract;jsessionid=397B42DDD36E4F654BAB381E3104ABB3.d02t04]</ref><ref>Ottoni, E.B., Izar, P., (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17: 171–178. {{DOI|10.1002/evan.20185}}</ref><ref>Ottoni, E.B. and Mannu, M., (2001). Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. International Journal of Primatology, 22: 347–358. doi=10.1023/A:1010747426841 [http://stoa.usp.br/ebottoni/files/435/2193/2001_IJP_Ottoni%2526Mannu.pdf]</ref><ref>{{cite journal |title=Substrate and tool use by brown capuchins in Suriname: Ecological contexts and cognitive bases |author=Boinski, S., Quatrone, R. P. & Swartz, H. |year=2008 |volume=102 |issue=4 |pages=741–761 |doi=10.1525/aa.2000.102.4.741 |journal=American Anthropologist}}</ref> It may take a capuchin up to 8 years to master this skill.<ref>''Life'' series. 2009. Episode 1. [[BBC]].</ref> These are used to pound open hard shelled fruits, seeds and even [[oyster]]s. They have also been known to use tools for digging and probing in the dirt. Robust capuchins are also known at times to rub [[Chemical defense|defensive secretions]] from [[arthropod]]s over their bodies before eating them;<ref name="Alfaro"/> such secretions are believed to act as natural [[insecticide]]s.
  +
  +
====Baboons====
  +
Tool use by [[baboons]] in the wild was mentioned by [[Charles Darwin]] in his book ''[[The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex]]''.<ref>Darwin, C., (1871). ''The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex''. John Murray, London, pp. 51-52</ref>
  +
  +
{{quote|Brehm states, on the authority of the well-known traveller Schimper, that in Abyssinia when the baboons belonging to one species (''C. gelada'') descend in troops from the mountains to plunder the fields, they sometimes encounter troops of another species (''C. hamadryas''), and then a fight ensues. The Geladas roll down great stones, which the Hamadryas try to avoid...}}
  +
  +
Darwin continued:
  +
  +
{{quote|Brehm, when accompanying the Duke of Coburg-Gotha, aided in an attack with fire-arms on a troop of baboons in the pass of Mensa in Abyssinia. The baboons in return rolled so many stones down the mountain, some as large as a man's head, that the attackers had to beat a hasty retreat; and the pass was actually for a time closed against the caravan.}}
  +
  +
These rather anecdotal reports of stone throwing by baboons have been corroborated by more recent research on [[chacma baboon]] (''Papio ursinus'') troops living on the desert floor of the Kuiseb Canyon in South West Africa.
  +
Stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side. The stones tumble down the side of the cliff or falls directly to the canyon floor. The researchers recorded 23 such incidents involving the voluntary release of 124 stones.<ref>Hamilton, W.J., Buskirk, R.E. and Buskirk, W.H., (1975). Defensive stoning by baboons. ''Nature'', 256: 488-489. {{DOI|10.1038/256488a0}}</ref>
  +
  +
A subadult male from a captive group of [[Guinea baboon]]s (''Papio papio'') learned, by trial-and-error, to use a tool to rake in food. He then used the tool 104 times over 26 days, thereby providing the group with most of its food.<ref>Beck, B., (1973). Observation learning of tool use by captive Guinea baboons (Papio papio). ''American Journal of Physical Anthropology'', 38: 579–582</ref>
  +
  +
====Mandrills====
  +
In the wild mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at [[Chester Zoo]] (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.<ref name="Gill2011"/>
  +
  +
====Crab-eating macaques====
  +
In Thailand and Myanmar, [[crab-eating macaque]]s use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails (nerites, muricids, trochids, etc.) along the Andaman sea coast and offshore islands.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Gumert|first=MD|author2=Kluck, M. |author3=Malaivijitnond, S. |title=The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand|journal=American Journal of Primatology|year=2009|volume=71|pages=594–608|doi=10.1002/ajp.20694}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Hunting mammals====
  +
Research in 2007 showed that common chimpanzees sharpen sticks to use as weapons when hunting mammals. This is considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans. Researchers documented 22 occasions where wild chimpanzees on a savanna in [[Senegal]] fashioned sticks into "spears" to hunt [[Senegal bushbaby|lesser bush babies]] (''Galago senegalensis'').<ref>{{cite book|title=Cognitive Archaeology and Human Evolution|editors=Sophie A. de Beaune, Frederick L. Coolidge, Thomas Wynn|page=66|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2009|isbn=9780521769778}}</ref> In each case, a chimpanzee modified a branch by breaking off one or two ends and, frequently using its teeth, sharpened the stick. The tools, on average, were about 60&nbsp;cm (24&nbsp;in) long and 1.1&nbsp;cm (0.4&nbsp;in) in circumference. The chimpanzee then jabbed the spear into hollows in tree trunks where bush babies sleep.<ref>{{cite book|title=Among the Great Apes: Adventures on the Trail of Our Closest Relatives|last=Raffaele|first=Paul|page=109|publisher=Harper|year=2011|isbn=9780061671845}}</ref> There was a single case in which a chimpanzee successfully extracted a bush baby with the tool. The tools are referred to as "spears", however, they differ from throwing spears in the sense that they are jabbed into tree trunks and branches, not thrown. It has been suggested the word "spear" is an overstatement that makes the chimpanzees seem too much like early humans and the term "bludgeon" is more accurate.<ref name="Roach">{{cite web|url=http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2007/02/070222-chimps-spears.html|author=Roach, J.|title=Chimps use "spears" to hunt mammals, study says|publisher=National Geographic News|year=2007|accessdate=August 1, 2013}}</ref> Females, particularly adolescent females, and young chimps in general were seen exhibiting this behaviour more frequently than adult males.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/6387611.stm|title=Chimpanzees 'hunt using spears'|publisher=BBC|accessdate= August 11, 2013|date=February 22, 2007}}</ref>
  +
  +
Chimpanzees regularly eat the marrow of long bones of [[colobus monkeys]] with the help of small sticks, after opening the ends of the bones with their teeth.<ref>{{cite book|last=Shipman|first=Pat|title=The Animal Connection: A New Perspective on What Makes Us Human|page=95|publisher=W. W. Norton & Company|year=2011|isbn=9780393082227}}</ref> A juvenile female was observed to eat small parts of the brain of an intact skull that she could not break open by inserting a small stick through the [[foramen magnum]]. On another occasion, an adult female used three sticks to clean the orbits of a colobus monkey skull after she had just eaten the eyes.<ref name="Boesch" />
  +
  +
====Hunting invertebrates====
  +
  +
In the [[Gombe National Park]] in 1960, [[Jane Goodall]] observed a chimpanzee, David Greybeard, poking pieces of grass into a [[termite]] mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what David was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. David had been using the grass as a tool to “fish” or "dip" for termites.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute1"/> Soon after this initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed David and other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves, and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans made and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute1" />
  +
  +
More recent studies of the Gombe chimps show that young females and males learn to fish for termites differently. Female chimps learn to fish for termites earlier and better than the young males.<ref>{{cite book|title=Among the Great Apes: Adventures on the Trail of Our Closest Relatives|last=Raffaele|first=Paul|page=83|publisher=Harper|year=2011|isbn=9780061671845}}</ref> Females also spend more time fishing while at the mounds with their mothers - males spend more time playing. When they are adults, females need more termite [[protein]] because with young to care for, they can't hunt the way males can.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute2">{{cite web|url=http://www.janegoodall.org/study-corner-tool-use|title=Study corner - tool use|publisher=The Jane Goodall Institute|accessdate=August 1, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
Populations differ in the prevalence of tool use for fishing for invertebrates. Chimpanzees in the [[Tai National Park]] only sometimes use tools, whereas Gombe chimpanzees rely almost exclusively on tools for their intake of [[Dorylus|driver ants]]. This may be due to difference in the rewards gained by tool use; Gombe chimpanzees collect 760 ants/min compared to 180 ants/min for the Tai chimpanzees.<ref name="Boesch" />
  +
  +
Some chimpanzees use tools to hunt large bees (''Xylocopa'' sp.) which make nests in dead branches on the ground or in trees. To get to the grubs and the honey, the chimpanzee first tests for the presence of adults by probing the nest entrance with a stick. If present, adult bees block the entrance with their abdomens, ready to sting. The chimpanzee then disables them with the stick to make them fall out and eats them rapidly. Afterwards, the chimpanzee opens the branch with its teeth to obtain the grubs and the honey.<ref name="Boesch" />
  +
  +
Chimpanzees have even been observed using two tools, a stick to dig into an ant nest and a "brush" made from grass stems with their teeth to collect the ants.<ref name="Boesch" />
  +
  +
Wild [[Black-striped capuchin|bearded capuchins]] use sticks to flush prey from inside rock crevices.<ref name="Mannu" />
  +
  +
====Hunting fish====
  +
Orangutans living in [[Borneo]] scavenge fish that wash up along the shore and scoop catfish out of small ponds for fresh meals. Over two years, anthropologist Anne Russon saw several animals on these forested islands learn on their own to jab at catfish with sticks, so that the panicked prey would flop out of ponds and into the orangutan's waiting hands.<ref>{{cite journal|title=Borneo Orangs Fish for Their Dinner: Behavior Suggests Early Human Ancestors Were Piscivores|last=Bower|first=Bruce|journal=Science News|volume=179|issue=10|page=16|date=May 7, 2011|url=http://www.questiaschool.com/read/1G1-256365262/borneo-orangs-fish-for-their-dinner-behavior-suggests {{subscription required}}|doi=10.1002/scin.5591791014}}</ref> Although orangutans usually fished alone, Russon observed pairs of apes catching catfish on a few occasions.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2011/04/orangutan-tools-fishing/|title=Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish|author=Bower, B.|accessdate=August 5, 2013|work=Wired|date=April 18, 2011}}</ref> On the island of Kaja in Borneo, a male orangutan was observed using a pole apparently trying to spear or bludgeon fish. This individual had seen humans fishing with spears. Although not successful, he was later able to improvise by using the pole to catch fish already trapped in the locals' fishing lines.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-562236/Orangutan-attempts-hunt-fish-spear.html|title=Orangutan attempts to hunt fish with spear|publisher=MailOnline|accessdate=August 1, 2013|location=London|date=April 26, 2008}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Collecting honey====
  +
Honey of four bee species is eaten by chimpanzees. Groups of chimpanzees fish with sticks for the honey after having tried to remove what they can with their hands. They usually extract with their hands honeycombs from undisturbed hives of honey bees and run away from the bees to quietly eat their catch. By contrast, hives that have already been disturbed either through the falling of the tree or because of the intervention of other predators, are cleaned of the remaining honey with fishing tools.<ref name="Boesch" />
  +
  +
Sumatran orangutans will use a stick to poke a bees' nest wall, move it around and catch the honey.<ref name="OrangutanIslands" />
  +
  +
====Processing nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds====
  +
[[Image:Chimpanzee and stick.jpg|A [[chimpanzee]] gathering food with a stick|thumb|right|250px]]
  +
Tai chimpanzees crack open nuts with rocks while there is no record of Gombe chimpanzees using rocks in this manner.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute1" /> After opening nuts by pounding with a hammer, parts of the [[kernel]]s may be too difficult to be reached with the teeth or fingernails and some individuals use sticks to remove these remains, instead of pounding the nut further with the hammer as other individuals do;<ref name="Boesch" /> a relatively rare combination of using two different tools. Hammers for opening nuts may be either wood or stone.
  +
  +
Chimpanzees in the Nimba Mountains of [[Guinea]], Africa, use both stone and wooden [[cleaver]]s, as well as stone anvils, to chop up and reduce ''[[Treculia]]'' fruits into smaller bite-sized portions. These fruits, which can be the size of a [[Volleyball (ball)|volleyball]] and weigh up to 8.5&nbsp;kg, are hard and fibrous. But, despite lacking a hard outer shell, they are too large for a chimpanzee to get its jaws around and bite into. Instead, the chimpanzees use a range of tools to chop them into smaller pieces. This is the first account of chimpanzees using a pounding tool technology to break down large food items into bite-sized chunks rather than just extract it from other unobtainable sources such as [[baobab]] nuts. It is also the first time wild chimpanzees have been found to use two distinct types of percussive technology, i.e. movable cleavers against a non-movable anvil, to achieve the same goal. Neighbouring chimpanzees in the nearby region of Seringbara do not process their food in this way, indicating how tool use among apes is culturally learnt.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8427000/8427974.stm|title=Chimps use cleavers and anvils as tools to chop food|author=Walker, M.|publisher=BBC|accessdate=August 11, 2013|date=December 24, 2009}}</ref>
  +
  +
Sumatran orangutans use sticks to acquire seeds from a particular fruit.<ref>{{cite book|title=The Evolution of Primate Societies|page=685|editors=John C. Mitani, Josep Call, Peter M. Kappeler, Ryne A. Palombit, and Joan B. Silk|publisher=University of Chicago Press|year=2012|isbn=9780226531731}}</ref> When the fruit of the ''Neesia'' tree ripens, its hard, ridged husk softens until it falls open. Inside are seeds that are highly desirable to the orangutans, but they are surrounded by fibreglass-like hairs that are painful if eaten. A ''Neesia''-eating orangutan will select a 12&nbsp;cm stick, strip off the bark, and then carefully collect the hairs with it. Once the fruit is safe, the ape will eat the seeds using the stick or its fingers.<ref name="OrangutanIslands"/>
  +
  +
Wild capuchin monkeys in many areas use stone hammers and anvils to crack nuts and encased seeds.<ref>Fragaszy, D. et al., (2004). Wild capuchin monkeys (''Cebus libidinosus'') use anvils and stone pounding tools. ''American Journal of Primatology'', 64: 359-366</ref><ref>Ottoni, E.B. and Izar, P., (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17: 171-187</ref> They transport stones and nuts to an anvil for this purpose.<ref>Visalberghi, E. et al., (2009). Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded capuchin monkeys. ''Current Biology'', 3: 213-217</ref> Capuchins also use stones to excavate tubers.<ref name="Mannu">Mannu, M. and Ottoni, E.B., (2009). The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. ''American Journal of Primatology'', 3: 242-251</ref>
  +
  +
====Collecting water====
  +
When chimpanzees can't reach water that has formed in hollows high up inside trees, they have been observed taking a handful of leaves, chewing them, and dipping this “sponge” into the pool to suck out the water.<ref name="The Jane Goodall Institute2" />
  +
  +
Both bonobos and chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water and are used as grooming tools.<ref>{{cite book|author=Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology, Harvard University. Dept. of Anthropology|title=Symbols|publisher=Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology|year=1995|page=5}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Other uses====
  +
[[Image:Gorrila tool use-Efi.jpg|thumb|A gorilla pushing a stick into the ground and using it as a stabilizing stick while dredging aquatic herbs]]
  +
Western lowland gorillas have been observed using sticks to apparently measure the depth of water and as "walking sticks" to support their posture when crossing deeper water.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030385|title=Wild gorillas handy with a stick|author=Gross, L.|year=2005|accessdate=August 2, 2013}}</ref> Orangutans have also been observed using sticks to apparently measure the depth of water.
  +
  +
It has been reported that orangutans use tools for a wide range of purposes including using leaves as protective gloves or napkins, using leafy branches to swat insects or gather water, and building sun or rain covers above the nests used for resting.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/01/030107073934.htm|title=Evidence for orangutan culture|publisher=DailyNews|accessdate=August 2, 2013}}</ref> It has been reported that a Sumatran orangutan used a large leaf as an umbrella in a tropical rainstorm.<ref name="OrangutanIslands" />
  +
  +
Orangutans produce an alarm call known as a “kiss squeak” when they encounter a predator like a snake or a human. Sometimes, orangutans will strip leaves from a branch and hold them in front of their mouth when making the sound. It has been found this lowers the maximum frequency of the sound i.e. makes it deeper, and in addition, smaller orangutans are more likely to use the leaves. It has been suggested they use the leaves to make themselves sound bigger than they really are, the first case of an animal using a tool to manipulate sound.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://blogs.smithsonianmag.com/science/2009/08/orangutans-use-leaves-to-sound-bigger/|title=Orangutans use leaves to sound bigger|author=Zielinski, S.|year=2009|accessdate=August 5, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
Some examples of tool use in wild bonobos include using leaves as cover for rain, or the use of branches in social displays.<ref name="ApeTag" />
  +
  +
A troop of wild macaques which regularly interact with humans have learnt to remove hairs from the human's heads, and use the hair to floss their teeth.<ref>The One Show. Television programme broadcast by the BBC on March 26, 2014</ref>
  +
  +
Captive western lowland gorillas have been observed to threaten each other with sticks and larger pieces of wood, while others use sticks for hygienic purposes. Some females have attempted to use logs as ladders.<ref name="Fontaine">{{cite journal|author=Fontaine, B., Moisson, P.Y. and Wickings, E.J. |date=1995 |title=Observations of spontaneous tool making and tool use in a captive group of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) |url=http://www.researchgate.net/publication/232593480_Observations_of_spontaneous_tool_making_and_tool_use_in_a_captive_group_of_western_lowland_gorillas_%28Gorilla_gorilla_gorilla%29 |accessdate=June 8, 2014}}</ref> In another group of captive gorillas, several individuals were observed throwing sticks and branches into a tree, apparently to knock down leaves and seeds.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Nakamichi, M. |date=July 1999 |title=Spontaneous use of sticks as tools by captive gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) |journal=Primates, 40: 487-498 |url=http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2FBF02557584 |accessdate=June 8, 2014}}</ref> Gorillas at Prague zoo have used tools in several ways, including using wood wool as "slippers" when walking on the snow or to cross a wet section of the floor.<ref name="Vancatova" />
  +
  +
In March 2009, it was reported that "Santino", a male chimpanzee in [[Furuvik]] zoo ([[Sweden]]), had planned hundreds of stone-throwing attacks on visitors to the zoo. He cached hundreds of stones in areas facing the public when he was in a calm state, prior to the zoo opening in the morning. The launching of the stones occurred hours later during dominance displays to zoo visitors, with Santino in an "agitated" state. Further, the chimpanzee learned to recognise how and when parts of his concrete enclosure could be pulled apart to fashion further projectiles.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/science/nature/7928996.stm|title=Zoo chimp 'planned' stone attacks|publisher=BBC|accessdate=August 11, 2013|date=March 9, 2009}}</ref><ref>''Current Biology'', volume 19, issue 5, 10 March 2009, pages 190-191.</ref>
  +
  +
Tool use has been observed in [[capuchin monkey]]s both in captivity and in their natural environments. In a captive environment, capuchins readily insert a stick into a tube containing viscous food that clings to the stick, which they then extract and lick.<ref>Westergaard, G.C., et al. (1998). Why some capuchin monkeys (''Cebus apella'') use probing tools (and others do not). ''Journal of Comparative Psychology'', 112: 207-211</ref> Capuchins also use a stick to push food from the center of a tube retrieving the food when it reaches the far end,<ref>Visalberghi, E, et al., (1995). Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (''Pan troglodytes''), bonobos (''Pan paniscus''), an orangutan (''Pongo pygmaeus''), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). ''Journal of Comparative Psychology'', 109: 52-60</ref> and as a rake to sweep objects or food toward themselves.<ref>Cummins-Sebree, S.E. and Fragaszy, D., (2005). Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (''Cebus apella'') use a different metric than tamarins (''Saguinus oedipus''). ''Journal of Comparative Psychology'', 119: 210-219</ref>
  +
  +
===Elephants===
  +
[[Elephant]]s show an ability to manufacture and use tools with their trunk and feet. Both wild and captive [[Asian elephant]]s (''Elephas maximus'') use branches to swat flies or scratch themselves.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Hart, B. J.; Hart, L. A.; McCory, M.; Sarath, C. R.|year=2001|title=Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: use and modification of branches for fly switching|journal=Animal Behaviour|volume=62|issue=5|pages=839–47|doi=10.1006/anbe.2001.1815}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Holdrege|first=Craig| title = Elephantine intelligence|journal=In Context|issue=5|publisher=[[The Nature Institute]]|date=Spring 2001|url=http://www.natureinstitute.org/pub/ic/ic5/elephant.htm|accessdate=October 30, 2007}}</ref> Eight of 13 captive Asian elephants, maintained under a naturalistic environment, modified branches and switched with the altered branch, indicating this species is capable of the more rare behaviour of tool manufacture. There were different styles of modification of the branches, the most common of which was holding the main stem with the front foot and pulling off a side branch or [[distal]] end with the trunk. Elephants have been observed digging holes to drink water, then ripping bark from a tree, chewing it into the shape of a ball thereby manufacturing a "plug" to fill in the hole, and covering it with sand to avoid [[evaporation]]. They would later go back to the spot to drink.
  +
  +
Asian elephants may use tools in insightful problem solving. A captive male was observed moving a box to a position where it could be stood upon to reach food that had been deliberately hung out of reach.<ref>{{cite journal|authors=Foerder, P., Galloway, M., Barthel, T., Moore, D.E. III, Reiss, D. |date=2011 |title=Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant |journal=PLoS ONE 6(8): e23251. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023251 |url=http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0023251}}</ref><ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.washingtonpost.com/national/health-science/elephants-show-smarts-at-national-zoo/2011/08/18/gIQA3nVZOJ_video.html|title=Elephants show smarts at National Zoo | work=The Washington Post|date=August 19, 2011}}</ref>
  +
  +
Elephants have also been known to drop large rocks onto an [[electric fence]] to either ruin the fence or cut off the electricity.<ref name=Poole>{{cite book|last= Poole|first=Joyce|title=Coming of Age with Elephants|publisher=Trafalgar Square|year=1996|location=[[Chicago, Illinois]]|pages= 131–133, 143–144, 155–157|isbn=0-340-59179-X}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Canines===
  +
In 2011, researchers at the Dingo Discovery and Research Centre in Melbourne, Australia, filmed a [[dingo]] manipulating a table and using this to get food.<ref>[http://www.australiangeographic.com.au/journal/dingoes-show-innovative-signs-of-tool-use.htm Dingoes use tools to solve novel problems]</ref>
  +
  +
===Bears===
  +
[[Moulting]] [[Brown bear]]s in [[Alaska]] have been observed using rocks to [[Exfoliation (cosmetology)|exfoliate]].<ref>[http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn21537-wild-bear-uses-a-stone-to-exfoliate.html Wild bear uses a stone to exfoliate]</ref>
  +
  +
===Cetaceans===
  +
At least some wild [[bottlenose dolphins]] use tools.<ref>{{cite book|last=Stanford|first=Craig B.|title=Significant Others|page=117|publisher=Basic Books|year=2001|isbn=9780786747665}}</ref> In [[Shark Bay]], dolphins place a [[marine sponge]] on their [[rostrum (anatomy)|rostrum]], presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom. This has only been observed in this bay (first in 1997), and predominantly by females.<ref name="Smolker 1997">{{cite journal | author=Smolker, R.A., et al. | title=Sponge Carrying by Dolphins (Delphinidae, ''Tursiops'' sp.): A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use? | journal=Ethology | year=1997 | volume=103 |issue=6| pages=454–465 | doi = 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00160.x }}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/102/25/8939|title=Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins|accessdate=2006-10-24}}</ref> Bottlenose dolphins have also been observed using [[conch shells]] to trap small fish, then lifting the shells out of the water causing the fish to fall into the dolphin's mouth.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://media.murdoch.edu.au/ingenious-fishing-method-dolphins|title=Ingenious fishing method may be spreading through dolphins|publisher=[[Murdoch University]]|date=August 24, 2011}}</ref>
  +
  +
[[Cetacean]]s, such as [[beluga whale]]s and [[dolphin]]s, blow [[bubble ring]]s.<ref>{{cite book|last=Prager|first=Ellen|title=Sex, Drugs, and Sea Slime: The Oceans' Oddest Creatures and Why They Matter|page=136|publisher=University of Chicago Press|year=2011|isbn=9780226678726}}</ref> Dolphins sometimes engage in complex play behaviours, creating bubble rings on purpose, seemingly for amusement.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.deepocean.net/deepocean/index.php?science09.php|title=The physics of bubble rings and other diver's exhausts|accessdate=2006-10-24}}</ref> There are two main methods of bubble ring production: rapid puffing of a burst of air into the water and allowing it to rise to the surface, forming a ring; or swimming repeatedly in a circle and then stopping to inject air into the [[helical]] vortex currents thus formed. The dolphin will often then examine its creation visually and with sonar. They also appear to enjoy biting the vortex-rings they've created, so that they burst into many separate normal bubbles and then rise quickly to the surface.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.bubblerings.com/bubblerings/media.cfm |title=Bubble rings: Videos and Stills|accessdate=2006-10-24}}</ref>
  +
  +
[[Humpback whales]] use a method of hunting know as "bubble net feeding"; a group of whales swims in a shrinking circle blowing bubbles below a school of prey. The shrinking ring of bubbles encircles the school and confines it in an ever-smaller cylinder. This ring can begin at up to {{convert|30|m|ft}} in diameter and involve the cooperation of a dozen animals.<ref>{{cite book|last=Kaldveer|first=Urmas|page=185|title=The Others: The Whale People a Personal Journey of Discovery, Transformation, and Healing|publisher=Balboa Press|year=2012|isbn=9781452558639}}</ref> Some whales blow the bubbles, some dive deeper to drive fish toward the surface, and others herd prey into the net by vocalizing. The whales then suddenly swim upward through the "net", mouths agape, swallowing thousands of fish in one gulp.<ref>{{cite news|first=Deb|last=Acklin|title= Crittercam Reveals Secrets of the Marine World
  +
|url=http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2002/06/0624_020624_TVbubble.html|publisher= National Geographic News
  +
|date=August 5, 2003|accessdate=November 1, 2007}}</ref> Another form of bubble-net feeding is "lobtail feeding". This technique involves the whale slapping the surface of the ocean with its tail between one and four times before creating the bubble net.<ref name=Lobtail>{{cite journal|last=Allen|first=Jenny|author2=Mason Weinrich |author3=Will Hoppitt |author4=Luke Rendell |title=Network-Based Diffusion Analysis Reveals Cultural Transmission of Lobtail Feeding in Humpback Whales|journal=Science|date=26 April 2013|volume=340|issue=6131|pages=485-188|doi=10.1126/science.1231976|accessdate=30 April 2013}}</ref> Dolphins also use bubble net feeding.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.southampton.ac.uk/engineering/news/2012/07/18_dolphins.page|title=Do Dolphins think nonlinearly?|author=Leighton, T.|year=2012|accessdate=July 7, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Sea otters===
  +
[[Image:Sea Otteruses a rock to break a shell open 2.jpg|thumb|right|A sea otter using a rock to break open a shell.]]
  +
Under each foreleg, the [[sea otter]] (''Enhydra lutris'') has a loose pouch of skin that extends across the chest. In this pouch (preferentially the left one), the animal stores collected food to bring to the surface. This pouch also holds a rock, unique to the otter, that is used to break open shellfish and clams.<ref name=haley>{{cite book|title=Marine Mammals of Eastern North Pacific and Arctic Waters|edition=2nd|chapter=Sea Otter|year=1986|location=Seattle, Washington|publisher=Pacific Search Press|isbn=0-931397-14-6|author= Haley, D. (ed.)|oclc=13760343}}</ref> To open hard shells, it may pound its prey with both paws against the rock which it places on its chest. Furthermore, sea otters will use large stones to pry an [[abalone]] off its rock; they will hammer the abalone shell with observed rates of 45 blows in 15 seconds or 180 rpm, and do it in two&ndash;three dives. Releasing an abalone, which can cling to rock with a force equal to 4,000 times its own body weight, requires multiple dives by the otter.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://marinebio.org/species.asp?id=157|title=Sea Otters, ''Enhydra lutris''|publisher=Marinebio|accessdate=July 7, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Mongooses===
  +
Wild [[banded mongoose]]s (''Mungos mungo'') regularly use anvils to open food items with a hard shell such as [[rhinoceros beetle]]s, bird eggs, snail shells or pupating [[dung beetle]]s. They use a range of anvils commonly including rocks and the stems of trees, but will also use the side-walls of gullys and even dried elephant dung. Pups as young as 2 months of age are already showing the behavioural patterns associated with using an anvil, however, successful smashing is usually shown in individuals older than 6 months of age.<ref>Müller, C.A., (2010). Do anvil-using banded mongooses understand means-end relationships? A field experiment. Animal Cognition, 13: 325-330 [http://www.bandedmongoose.org/wp-content/uploads/2012/09/Muller-2010-Anim-Cognition.pdf]</ref>
  +
  +
===American badgers===
  +
[[American badger|North American badgers]] (''Taxidea taxus'') hunt [[Richardson's ground squirrel]]s (''Spermophilus richardsonii''). The most common hunting technique is excavation of burrow systems, but plugging of openings into ground-squirrel tunnels accounts for 5–23% of hunting actions. Badgers usually use soil from around the tunnel opening, or soil dragged 30–270&nbsp;cm from a nearby mound to plug tunnels. The least common (6%), but most novel, form of plugging used by 1 badger involved movement of 37 objects from distances of 20–105&nbsp;cm to plug openings into 23 ground-squirrel tunnels on 14 nights.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Michener, Gail R. |title=Hunting techniques and tool use by North American badgers preying on Richardson's ground squirrels|journal=Journal of Mammalogy|year=2004|volume=85|issue=5|pages=1019–1027|jstor=1383835|doi=10.1644/BNS-102}}</ref>
  +
  +
==In birds==
  +
Tool use is found in at least thirty-three different [[Family (biology)|families]] of birds.<ref name="Map Of Life">{{cite web|url=http://www.mapoflife.org/topics/topic_193_Tool-use-in-birds/|title=Tool use in birds|publisher=Map Of Life|accessdate=July 23, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
According to Jones and Kamil's definition,<ref name="Jones and Kamil"/> an [[Egyptian vulture]] dropping a bone on a rock would not be considered using a tool since the rock cannot be seen as an extension of the body. However, the use of a rock manipulated using the beak to crack an [[ostrich]] egg would qualify the Egyptian vulture as a tool user. [[:Category:Tool-using species|Many other species]], including [[parrot]]s, [[corvid]]s and a range of [[passerine]]s, have been noted as tool users.<ref name=emery>Emery, N.J., (2006). Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 361: 23–43 [http://www.princeton.edu/~asifg/braindiversity/Emery%20-%20Cognitive%20Ornithology.pdf]</ref>
  +
  +
{{Multiple image
  +
| direction = horizontal
  +
| align = top
  +
| header = Bird nests show a great diversity in complexity
  +
| width1 = 200
  +
| image1 = Nests of weaver birds.jpg
  +
| caption1 = Intricate nests of Weaver Birds<ref>{{cite book|last=Fors|first=Carl|title=Hens: Why Women Are Different|page=89|year=2006|publisher=Infinity Publishing|isbn=9780741429544}}</ref>
  +
| width2 = 245
  +
| image2 = Larus marinus eggs.jpg
  +
| caption2 = Relatively simple nest of a [[Greater Black-backed Gull]]
  +
| width3 = 200
  +
| image3 = EmperorPenguinFeedingChick.jpg
  +
| caption3 = Emperor Penguins do not build a nest<ref>{{cite book|editors=E. A. Schreiber and Joanna Burger|title=Biology of Marine Birds|page=320|publisher=CRC Press|year=2001|isbn=9781420036305}}</ref>
  +
}}
  +
Many birds (and other animals) build nests.<ref>{{cite book|title=Birds of North America: Eastern Region|editor=François Vuilleumier|publisher=Dorling Kindersley|year=2011|page=18}}</ref> It can be argued that this behaviour constitutes tool use according to the definitions given above; the birds "carry objects (twigs, leaves) for future use", the shape of the formed nest prevents the eggs rolling away and thereby "extends the physical influence realized by the animal", and the twigs are bent and twisted to shape the nest, i.e. "modified to fit a purpose". The complexity of bird nests varies markedly, perhaps indicating a range in the sophistication of tool use. For example, compare the highly complex structures of [[Ploceidae|Weaver Birds]] to the simple mats of herbaceous matter with a central cup constructed by [[Gull]]s, and it is noteworthy that some birds do not build nests, e.g. [[Emperor Penguins]]. The classification of nests as tools has been disputed on the basis that the completed nest, or burrow, is not held or manipulated.<ref name="Shumaker" />
  +
  +
===Woodpecker Finches===
  +
Perhaps the best known and most studied example of an avian tool user is the [[Woodpecker Finch]] (''Camarhynchus pallidus'') from the [[Galápagos Islands]]. If the bird uncovers prey in bark which is inaccessible, the bird then flies off to fetch a cactus spine which it may use in one of three different ways: as a goad to drive out an active insect (without necessarily touching it): as a spear with which to impale a slow-moving larva or similar animal; or as an implement with which to push, bring towards, nudge or otherwise manoeuvre an inactive insect from a crevice or hole. Tools that are not exactly fitting the purpose are worked by the bird and adapted for the function thus making the Finch a "tool maker" as well as a "tool user". Some individuals have been observed to use a different type of tool with novel functional features - barbed twigs from blackberry bushes, a plant that is not native to the islands. The twigs were first modified by removing side twigs and leaves and then used such that the barbs helped drag prey out of tree crevices.<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
There is a genetic predisposition for tool use in this species, which is then refined by individual trial-and-error learning during a sensitive phase early in development. This means that, rather than following a stereotypical behavioural pattern, tool use can be modified and adapted by learning.
  +
  +
The importance of tool use by Woodpecker Finches species differs between vegetation zones. In the arid zone, where food is limited and hard to access, tool use is essential, especially during the dry season. Up to half of the finches' prey is acquired with the help of tools, making them even more routine tool users than chimpanzees. The tools allow them to extract large, nutritious insect larvae from tree holes, making tool use more profitable than other foraging techniques. In contrast, in the humid zone, Woodpecker Finches rarely use tools, since food availability is high and prey is more easily obtainable. Here, the time and energy costs of tool use would be too high.<ref name="Map Of Life"/>
  +
  +
There have been reported cases of Woodpecker Finches brandishing a twig as a weapon.<ref name="Boswall" />
  +
  +
===Corvids===
  +
[[File:Cognitive-Processes-Associated-with-Sequential-Tool-Use-in-New-Caledonian-Crows-pone.0006471.s003.ogv|thumb|A New Caledonian Crow showing sequential tool use in the laboratory]]
  +
[[Corvidae|Corvids]] are a [[Family (biology)|family]] of birds characterised by relatively large brains, remarkable behavioural plasticity (especially highly innovative foraging behaviour) and well-developed cognitive abilities.<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
====In the wild====
  +
An [[American Crow]] (''Corvus brachyrhynchos'') has been observed to modify and use a piece of wood as a probe.<ref name=Caffrey2000>{{cite journal|author=Caffrey, Carolee|year=2000|title= Tool Modification and Use by an American Crow|journal=[[Wilson Bulletin|Wilson Bull.]]|volume=112|issue=2|pages= 283–284|doi=10.1676/0043-5643(2000)112[0283:TMAUBA]2.0.CO;2}}</ref> [[Green Jay]]s (''Cyanocorax yncas'') have been observed using sticks as tools to extract [[insect]]s from tree [[bark]],<ref>{{cite journal |url=http://sora.unm.edu/sites/default/files/journals/wilson/v094n04/p0593-p0594.pdf |format=PDF |title=Tool use by Green Jays |journal=Wilson Bulletin |volume=94 |issue=4 |year=1982 |pages=593–594}}</ref> a behavior which has also been observed and filmed in [[New Caledonian Crow]]s (''Corvus moneduloides'')[http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=3RCcVJvZF0I]<ref>{{cite web|title=Tool use in New CaledonianCrows|url=http://users.ox.ac.uk/~kgroup/tools/crow_natur al_history.shtml|publisher=Behavioural Ecology Research Group|accessdate=February 19, 2013}}</ref> The birds poke the insects or larvae until they bite the stick in defence and can be drawn out. This "larva fishing" is very similar to the "termite fishing" practised by chimpanzees. In the wild, stick tools are made from twigs, grass stems or similar plant structures whereas captive individuals have been observed to use a variety of materials, including feathers and garden wire. Stick tools can either be non-hooked - being more or less straight and requiring only little modification - or hooked. Construction of the more complex hooked tools typically involves choosing a forked twig from which parts are removed and the remaining end is sculpted and sharpened. New Caledonian Crows also use pandanus tools, made from barbed leaf edges of screw pines (''[[Pandanus]]'' spp.) by precise ripping and cutting although the function of the pandanus tools is not understood.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hunt|first=Gavin R.|last2=Gray|first2=Russell D.|title=The Crafting of Hook Tools by Wild New Caledonian Crows|journal=Proceedings: Biological Sciences|publisher=The Royal Society|volume=271|issue=Supplement 3|page=S88|date=7 February 2004|url=http://www.jstor.org.libaccess.lib.mcmaster.ca/stable/i388369 {{subscription required}}|doi=10.1098/rsbl.2003.0085}}</ref>
  +
  +
Crows in urban [[Japan]] have been filmed[http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_5_DuZ8WuMM] using an innovative technique to crack hard-shelled [[Nut (fruit)|nut]]s by dropping them onto cross walks (pedestrian crossings) and letting them be run over and cracked by cars. They then retrieve the cracked nuts when the cars are stopped at the red light.<ref>{{cite book|page=478|author=DK Publishing|title=Animal Life|publisher=DK|year=2011|isbn=9780756688868}}</ref> In some towns in America, crows drop walnuts onto busy streets so that the cars will crack the nuts.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://mwsu-bio101.ning.com/profiles/blogs/crows-using-automobiles-as|title=Crows Using Automobiles as Nutcrackers|accessdate=2014-03-29}}</ref>{{better source|date=March 2014}}
  +
  +
[[Hooded Crow]]s (''Corvus cornix'') use bait to catch fish.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.orenhasson.com/EN/bait-fishing.htm|title=Bait-Fishing in Crows|accessdate=2009-02-07}}</ref> Individuals (who may have observed fish being fed bread by humans) will place the bread in the water to attract fish.<ref name="Boswall" />
  +
  +
====In captivity====
  +
While young birds in the wild normally learn to make stick tools from elders, a laboratory New Caledonian Crow named "Betty" was filmed[http://www.youtube.com/watch?feature=player_embedded&v=TtmLVP0HvDg] spontaneously improvising a hooked tool from a wire. It was known that this individual had no prior experience as she had been hand-reared.<ref>[http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2002/08/0808_020808_crow.html Crow making tools]</ref> New Caledonian Crows have been observed to use an easily available small tool to get a less easily available longer tool, and then use this to get an otherwise inaccessible longer tool to get food that was out of reach of the shorter tools. One bird, "Sam", spent 110 seconds inspecting the apparatus before completing each of the steps without any mistakes. This is an example of sequential tool use, which represents a higher cognitive function compared to many other forms of tool use and is the first time this has been observed in non-trained animals. Tool use has been observed in a non-foraging context, providing the first report of multi-context tool use in birds. Captive New Caledonian Crows have used stick tools to make first contact with objects that were novel and hence potentially dangerous, while other individuals have been observed using a tool when food was within reach but placed next to a model snake. It has been claimed "Their [New Caledonian Crow] tool-making skills exceed those of chimpanzees and are more similar to human tool manufacture than those of any other animal."<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
Other species, such as [[Rook (bird)|rooks]] (''Corvus frugilegus''), can also make and use tools in the laboratory, showing a degree of sophistication similar to that of New Caledonian Crows.<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
While not confirmed to have used tools in the wild, captive [[Blue Jay]]s (''Cyanocitta cristata'') have been observed using strips of newspaper as tools to obtain food.<ref name="eggeater" >[http://www.birds.cornell.edu/AllAboutBirds/BirdGuide/Blue_Jay.html Blue Jay]. birds.cornell.edu</ref><ref name=Jones>{{cite journal|author=Jones, Thony B. & Kamil, Alan C. |year=1973|title= Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay|url=http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1066&context=bioscibehavior|journal=[[Science (journal)|Science]]|volume=180|issue=4090|pages= 1076–1078|doi=10.1126/science.180.4090.1076}}</ref>
  +
  +
====Play====
  +
Common [[raven]]s (''Corvus corax'') are one of only a few species who make their own toys. They have been observed breaking off twigs to play with socially.<ref name="Heinrich, B. 1999 pp 282">Heinrich, B., (1999). ''Mind of the Raven: Investigations and Adventures with Wolf-Birds'' pp 282. New York: Cliff Street Books. ISBN 978-0-06-093063-9</ref> At least one corvid has been filmed repeatedly sliding down a snowy roof while balancing on a coffee lid<ref>{{youtube|id=6uXiAe7Oc-I|title=Crowboarding}}</ref> and another playing with a [[table tennis]] ball in partnership with a dog.<ref>http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QqLU-o7N7Kw Dog and corvid playing with a ball</ref>- potentially both rare examples of tool use for the purposes of play. Blue Jays, like other corvids, are highly curious and are considered [[Bird intelligence|intelligent birds]]. Young Blue Jays playfully snatch brightly coloured or reflective objects, such as bottle caps or pieces of aluminium foil, and carry them around until they lose interest.
  +
  +
===Warblers===
  +
[[File:Common Tailorbird (Orthotomus sutorius) Nest in Hyderabad, AP W IMG 7248.jpg|right|thumb|150px|A pouch constructed
  +
by a Tailorbird which contains the nest]]
  +
The [[Tailorbird]] (genus ''Orthotomus'') takes a large growing leaf (or two or more small ones) and with its sharp bill pierces holes into opposite edges. It then grasps spider silk, silk from cocoons, or plant fibres with its bill, pulls this "thread" through the two holes, and knots it to prevent it from pulling through (although the use of knots is disputed<ref name="Answers.com" />). This process is repeated several times until the leaf or leaves forms a pouch or cup in which the bird then builds its nest.<ref name="Boswall">Boswall, J. (1977). Tool-using by birds and related behaviour. Avicultural Magazine, 83: 88-97</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://animal.discovery.com/birds/tailorbird-info.htm|title=Tailorbird|publisher=Animal Planet|accessdate=July 22, 2013}}</ref> The leaves are sewn together in such a way that the upper surfaces are outwards making the structure difficult to see. The punctures made on the edge of the leaves are minute and do not cause browning of the leaves, further aiding camouflage. The processes used by the Tailorbird have been classified as sewing, rivetting, lacing and matting. Once the stitch is made, the fibres fluff out on the outside and in effect they are more like rivets. Sometimes the fibres from one rivet are extended into an adjoining puncture and appear more like sewing. There are many variations in the nest and some may altogether lack the cradle of leaves.It is believed that only the female performs this sewing behaviour.<ref name="Answers.com">{{cite web|url=http://www.answers.com/topic/common-tailorbird|title=Common Tailorbird|publisher=Answers.com.|accessdate=July 22, 2013}}</ref> The Latin binomial name of the Common Tailorbird, ''Orthotomus sutorius'', means "straight-edged" "cobbler" rather than tailor.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://10000birds.com/common-tailorbird.htm|title=Common Tailorbird|publisher=10,000 Birds|accessdate=July 22, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
Some birds of the genus ''Prinia'' also practice this sewing and stitching behaviour.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/476987/prinia|title=Prinia|publisher=Encyclopaedia Britanica|accessdate=July 22, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Parrots===
  +
Many owners of household parrots have observed their pets using various tools to scratch various parts of their bodies. These tools include discarded feathers, bottle caps, popsicle sticks, matchsticks, cigarette packets and nuts in their shells.<ref name="Boswall" />
  +
  +
A captive [[Tanimbar Corella]] (''Cacatua goffiniana'') was observed spontaneously breaking off and "shaping" splinters of wood and small sticks to create [[Rake (tool)|rakes]] that were then used to retrieve otherwise unavailable food items on the other side of the aviary mesh - a rare example of tool-making.<ref name=Auersperg>{{cite journal|last=Auersperg|first=Alice M.I.|coauthors=Birgit Szabo, Auguste M.P. von Bayern, and Alex Kacelnik|title=Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo|journal=Current Biology|date=6 November 2012|volume=22|issue=21|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.002}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.bbc.co.uk/nature/20170195|title=Cockatoo shows tool-making skills|author=Warwicker, M.|publisher=BBC Nature|year=2012|accessdate=July 21, 2013}}</ref> This behaviour has been filmed.[http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=0HO8mueWLoo]
  +
  +
[[Hyacinth Macaw]]s (''Anodorhynchus hyacinthinus'') have been repeatedly observed to use tools when breaking open nuts, for example, pieces of wood being used as a wedge. Several birds have wrapped a piece of leaf around a nut to hold it in place. This behaviour is also shown by [[Palm Cockatoo]]s (''Probosciger aterrimus''). It seems that the Hyacinth Macaw has an innate tendency to use tools during manipulation of nuts, as naïve juveniles tried out a variety of objects in combination with nuts.<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
===Egyptian Vultures===
  +
When an Egyptian Vulture (''Neophron percnopterus'') encounters a large egg, it takes a stone into its beak and forcefully throws it at the egg until the shell is broken, usually taking a few minutes. This behaviour, first reported in 1966,<ref>van Lawick-Goodall, J. and van Lawick, H., (1966). Use of tools by the Egyptian Vulture, Neophron percnopterus. ''Nature'', 212(5069): 1468–1469. {{DOI|10.1038/2121468a0}}</ref> seems to be largely innate and is displayed by naïve individuals. Its origin could be related to the throwing of eggs and, interestingly, rounded (egg-like) stones are preferred to jagged ones.<ref>Thouless, C.R., Fanshawe, J.H. and Bertram, B.C.R., (1989). Egyptian Vultures Neophron percnopterus and Ostrich Struthio camelus eggs: The origins of stone-throwing behaviour. Ibis, 131: 9–15. {{DOI|10.1111/j.1474-919X.1989.tb02737.x}}</ref>
  +
  +
In a small population in Bulgaria, Egyptian Vultures use twigs to collect sheep wool for padding their nests. Although both twigs and wool can serve as nesting material, this appears to be deliberate tool use. The birds approached bits of discarded wool with a twig in their beak, which was then either used as a rake, to gather the wool into heaps, or to roll up the wool. Interestingly, wool was collected only after shearing or simulated shearing of sheep had taken place, but not after wool had simply been deposited in sheep enclosures.<ref>Stoyanova, Y., Stefanov, N. and Schmutz, J.K., (2010). Twig used as a tool by the Egyptian Vulture (Neophron percnopterus). Journal of Raptor Research, 44(2): 154–156. {{DOI|10.3356/JRR-09-20.1}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Brown-headed Nuthatches===
  +
[[Brown-headed Nuthatch]]es (''Sitta pusilla'') have been observed to methodically use [[bark]] pieces to remove other flakes of bark from a tree. The birds insert the bark piece underneath an attached bark scale, using it like a wedge and lever, to expose hiding insects. Occasionally, they reuse the same piece of bark several times and sometimes even fly short distances carrying the bark flake in their beak. The evolutionary origin of this tool use might be related to these birds frequently wedging seeds into cracks in the bark to hammer them open with their beak, which can lead to bark coming off.
  +
  +
Brown-headed Nuthatches have also used a bark flake for concealing a [[Hoarding (animal behavior)|seed cache]].<ref name="Map Of Life" />
  +
  +
===Gulls===
  +
[[Seagull]]s have been known to drop live oyster shells on paved and hard surfaces so that cars can drive over them and break the shell. So many get dropped that it is difficult to drive safely near some waterways. Certain species (e.g. the [[Herring Gull]]) have exhibited tool use behavior, using pieces of bread as [[Bait (luring substance)|bait]] to catch [[goldfish]], for example.<ref name="henry">{{cite journal|last=Henry|first=Pierre-Yves|author2=Jean-Christophe Aznar |date=June 2006|title=Tool-use in Charadrii: Active Bait-Fishing by a Herring Gull|journal=Waterbirds|publisher=The Waterbird Society|year=2006|volume=29|issue=2|pages=233–234|doi=10.1675/1524-4695(2006)29[233:TICABB]2.0.CO;2|issn=1524-4695}}</ref>
  +
  +
===Owls===
  +
[[Burrowing Owl]]s (''Athene cunicularia'') frequently collect mammalian dung, which they use as a bait to attract dung beetles, a major item of prey.<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v431/n7004/abs/431039a.html |title=Animal behaviour: Use of dung as a tool by burrowing owls |journal=Nature;431(7004):39.|date=September 2, 2004 |date=June 8, 2014}}</ref>
  +
  +
==In reptiles==
  +
Tool use by [[American alligator]]s and [[Mugger crocodile]]s has been documented. During the breeding season, birds such as herons and egrets look for sticks to build their nests. Alligators and crocodiles collect sticks to use as bait to catch birds. The crocodilian positions itself near a rookery, partially submerges with the sticks balanced on its head, and when a bird approaches to take the stick, it springs its trap. This stick displaying strategy is the first known case of a predator not only using an object as a lure, but also taking into account the seasonal behavior of its prey.<ref>{{Cite journal|url=http://dinets.info/DinetsBrueggen&Brueggen.pdf|title=Crocodilians use tools for hunting|author=Dinets,V., J.C. Brueggen and J.D. Brueggen|year=2013 |journal= Ethology, Ecology & Evolution |volume=DOI:10.1080/03949370.2013.858276}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/12/131204182433.htm|title=Crocodiles are cleverer than previously thought: Some crocodiles use lures to hunt their prey|publisher=ScienceDaily|date=December 4, 2013|accessdate=December 8, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
==In cephalopods==
  +
[[File:Octopus shell.jpg|thumb|right|A small octopus (4-5 cm diameter) individual using a nut shell and clam shell as shelter]]
  +
At least four [[Veined Octopus]] (''Amphioctopus marginatus'') individuals were witnessed retrieving coconut shells, manipulating them, stacking them, transporting them some distance (up to 20 metres), and then reassembling them to use as a shelter.<ref>Finn, J.K., Tregenza, T. and M.D., (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology, {{DOI|10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.052}}</ref> The octopuses use coconut shells discarded by humans which have eventually settled in the ocean. They probe their arms down to loosen the mud, then rotate the shells out. After turning the shells so the open side faces upwards, the octopuses blow jets of mud out of the bowl before extending their arms around the shell - or if they have two halves, stacking them first, one inside the other. They then stiffen their legs and move away in a manner which has been called "stilt-walking". The octopuses eventually use the shells as a protective shelter in areas where little other shelter exists. If they just have one half, they simply turn it over and hide underneath. But if they are lucky enough to have retrieved two halves, they assemble them back into the original closed coconut form and sneak inside. This behaviour has been filmed.[http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/8408233.stm] The authors of the research article claimed this behaviour falls under the definition of tool use because the shells are carried for later use. However, this argument remains contested by a number of other biologists who state that the shells actually provide continuous protection from abundant bottom-dwelling predators in their home range.
  +
  +
Octopuses deliberately place stones, shells and even bits of broken bottle to form a wall that constricts the aperture to the den - a type of tool use.<ref name="Map Of Life2">{{cite web|url=http://www.mapoflife.org/topics/topic_59_Simple-tool-use-in-owls-and-cephalopods/|title=Simple tool use in owls and cephalopods|publisher=Map Of Life|year=2010|accessdate=July 23, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
In laboratory studies, ''Octopus mercatoris'', a small pygmy species of octopus, has been observed to block its lair using a plastic [[Lego]] block.<ref name="Oinuma" />
  +
  +
==In fish==
  +
Several species of [[wrasses]] have been observed using rocks as anvils to crack [[bivalve]] (scallops, urchins and clams) shells. It was first filmed[http://scienceblog.com/48078/video-show-tool-use-by-a-fish/] in an orange-dotted tuskfish (''Choerodon anchorago'') in 2009 by Giacomo Bernardi. The fish fans sand to unearth the bivalve, takes it into its mouth, swims several metres to a rock which it uses as an anvil by smashing the mollusc apart with sideward thrashes of the head. This behaviour has been recorded in a [[blackspot tuskfish]] (''Choerodon schoenleinii'') on Australia's Great Barrier Reef, yellowhead wrasse (''[[Halichoeres garnoti]]'') in Florida and a six-bar wrasse (''[[Thalassoma hardwicke]]'') in an aquarium setting. These species are at opposite ends of the phylogenetic tree in this [[Family (biology)|family]], so this behaviour may be a deep-seated trait in all wrasses.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://bio.research.ucsc.edu/people/bernardi/Bernardi/Publications/2011Tools.pdf|title=The use of tools by wrasses (Labridae). DOI=10.1007/s00338-011-0823-6|author=Bernardi, G.|year=2011|accessdate=July 7, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
It has been reported that freshwater [[stingray]]s use water as a tool by manipulating their bodies to direct a flow of water and extract food trapped amongst plants.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/earth/hi/earth_news/newsid_8452000/8452008.stm|title=Clever stingray fish use tools to solve problems|author=Bourton, J.|accessdate=July 8, 2013|work=BBC News|date=January 13, 2010}}</ref>
  +
  +
Prior to laying their eggs on a vertical rock face, male and female whitetail major [[damselfish]] clean the site by sand-blasting it. The fish pick up sand in their mouths and spit it against the rock face. Then they fan the area with their fins. Finally they remove the sand grains that remain stuck to the rock face by picking them off with their mouths.<ref>Keenleyside, M.H.A., (1979). ''Diversity and Adaptation in Fish Behaviour'', Springer-Verlag, Berlin.</ref>
  +
  +
Banded acara, (''[[Bujurquina]] vittata''), South American [[cichlid]]s, lays their eggs on a loose leaf. The male and female of a mating pair often “test” leaves before spawning: they pull and lift and turn candidate leaves, possibly trying to select leaves that are easy to move. After spawning, both parents guard the eggs. When disturbed, the parent acara often seize one end of the egg-carrying leaf in their mouth and drag it to deeper and safer locations.<ref>Keenleyside, M.H.A. and Prince, C., (1976). Spawning-site selection in relation to parental care of
  +
eggs in Aequidens paraguayensis (Pisces: Cichlidae). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 54: 2135-2139</ref>
  +
  +
[[Archerfish]] are found in the tropical mangrove swamps of India and Australasia. They approach the surface, take aim at insects that sit on plants above the surface, squirt a jet of water at them, and grab them after the insects have been knocked off into the water. The jet of water is formed by the action of the tongue, which presses against a groove in the roof of the mouth. Some archerfish can hit insects up to 1.5 m above the water surface. They use
  +
more water, which gives more force to the impact, when aiming at larger prey.
  +
  +
[[Triggerfish]] (''Pseudobalistes fuscus'') blow water to turn sea urchins over and expose their more vulnerable ventral side.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.howfishbehave.ca/pdf/Tool%20use.pdf|title=Tool use in fishes|author=Reebs, S.G.|year=2011|accessdate=July 7, 2013}}</ref>
  +
  +
==In insects==
  +
Ants of the species ''[[Conomyrma bicolor]]'' pick up stones and other small objects with their mandibles and drop them down the vertical entrances of rival colonies, allowing workers to forage for food without competition.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Michael H.J. Möglich & Gary D. Alpert |year=1979 |title=Stone dropping by Conomyrma bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): A new technique of interference competition |journal=[[Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology]] |volume=2 |issue=6 |pages=105–113 |jstor=4599265 |doi=10.1007/bf00292556}}</ref>
  +
  +
Hunting wasps of the genus ''[[Prionyx]]'' use weights (such as compacted sediment or a small pebble) to settle sand surrounding a recently provisioned burrow containing eggs and live prey in order to camouflage and seal the entrance. The wasp vibrates its wing muscles with an audible buzz while holding the weight in its mandibles, and applies the weight to the sand surrounding its burrow, causing the sand to vibrate and settle. Another hunting wasp, ''[[Ammophila (Sphecidae)|Ammophila]]'', uses pebbles to close burrow entrances.<ref>[http://books.google.ca/books?id=v7lV9tz8fXAC&printsec=frontcover&source=gbs_ge_summary_r&cad=0#v=onepage&q=wasps%20pebble&f=false ''Sociobiology: the New Synthesis''] by [[Edward O. Wilson]], p. 172; published 2000, by [[Harvard University Press]] (via [[Google Books]])</ref>
   
 
== See also ==
 
== See also ==
* [[Animal cognition#Tool and weapon use]]
+
* [[Animal cognition]]
* [[Bird intelligence#Tool use]]
+
* [[Cephalopod intelligence]]
* [[Cetacean intelligence#Use of tools]]
+
* [[Bird intelligence]]
* [[Elephant intelligence#Tool use]]
+
* [[Cetacean intelligence]]
  +
* [[Elephant intelligence]]
  +
* [[Structures built by animals]]
  +
  +
== References ==<!-- AnimalBehaviour72:1329. AnimalCognition5:71. CurrentBiology16:R244,17:412,17:1504. -->
  +
{{Reflist|2}}
  +
  +
==Further reading==
  +
* {{cite book|title=Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals|author1=Robert W. Shumaker|author2=Kristina R. Walkup|author3=Benjamin B. Beck|year=2011|publisher=The Johns Hopkins University Press}}
  +
  +
==External links==
  +
* [http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/7922109.stm Chimpanzee making and using a termite "fishing rod"]
  +
* [http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/sci/tech/7949335.stm Chimpanzee using tool to break into beehive to get honey]
  +
* [http://pages.cthome.net/rwinkler/crowintel.htm Crow making a tool by bending wire to snag food]
  +
* [http://users.ox.ac.uk/~kgroup/tools/toolpublications.shtml Various tool use by birds]
  +
* [http://www.howfishbehave.ca/pdf/Tool%20use.pdf Fish using an anvil to break open prey]
  +
* [http://www.livescience.com/9761-10-animals-tools.html Dolphin using a marine sponge to protect its rostrum]
  +
* [http://www.bbc.co.uk/nature/14227783 Mandrill using a tool to clean under its nails]
   
  +
{{Animal cognition}}
== References ==<!-- CurrBiol16:R244,17:412,17:1504. -->
 
{{reflist|2}}
 
   
[[Category:Tool-using species|*]]
 
   
<!--
 
[[de:Werkzeuggebrauch bei Tieren]]
 
[[pt:Uso de ferramentas por animais]]
 
-->
 
 
{{enWP|Tool use by animals}}
 
{{enWP|Tool use by animals}}
  +
[[Category:Animal intelligence]]
  +
[[Category:Ethology]]
  +
[[Category:Tools]]
  +
[[Category:Tool-using species]]

Revision as of 19:34, 19 August 2014

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Animals · Animal ethology · Comparative psychology · Animal models · Outline · Index



Tools are used by some animals to perform behaviours including the acquisition of food and water, grooming, defence, recreation or construction.[1] Originally thought to be a skill only possessed by humans, some tool use requires a sophisticated level of cognition. There is considerable discussion about the definition of what constitutes a tool and therefore which behaviours can be considered as true examples of tool use. A wide range of animals is considered to use tools including mammals, birds, fish, cephalopods and insects.

Rarely, animals have been observed making their own tools, e.g. primates sharpening a stick to use as a weapon,[2] or removing leaves and twigs from a branch and fishing for termites with a stem frayed by chewing.[3]

Definitions and terminology

The key to identifying tool use is defining what constitutes a tool. Researchers of animal behavior have arrived at different formulations.

In 1980, Beck published a widely used definition of tool use.[4] More recently, this has been modified to -

"The external employment of an unattached or manipulable attached environmental object to alter more efficiently the form, position, or condition of another object, another organism, or the user itself, when the user holds and directly manipulates the tool during or prior to use and is responsible for the proper and effective orientation of the tool.[5]

Other, briefer definitions have been proposed -

"An object carried or maintained for future use"

Finn, Tregenza, and Norman, 2009.[6]

"The use of physical objects other than the animal's own body or appendages as a means to extend the physical influence realized by the animal"

Jones and Kamil, 1973[7]

"An object that has been modified to fit a purpose" or "An inanimate object that one uses or modifies in some way to cause a change in the environment, thereby facilitating one's achievement of a target goal".

Hauser, 2000[8]

Others, for example Lawick-Goodall,[9] distinguish between "tool use" and "object use".

Different terms have been given to the tool according to whether the tool is altered by the animal. If the "tool" is not held or manipulated by the animal in any way, such as an immobile anvil, objects in a Bowerbird's bower, or a heron using discarded crackers as bait to catch fish, it is sometimes referred to as a "proto-tool". Several studies in primates and birds have found that tool use is correlated with an enlargement of the brain as a whole or of particular regions. For example, true tool-using birds have relatively larger brains than proto-tool users.[10]

When an animal uses a tool that acts on another tool, this has been termed use of a "meta-tool". For example, New Caledonian Crows will spontaneously use a short tool to obtain an otherwise inaccessible longer tool that then allows them to extract food from a hole.[10] Similarly, bearded capuchin monkeys will use smaller stones to loosen bigger quartz pebbles embedded in conglomerate rock, which they subsequently use as tools.[11]

Rarely, animals may use one tool followed by another, for example, bearded capuchins use stones and sticks, or two stones.[11] This is called "associative", "secondary" or "sequential" tool use.[12]

Some animals use other individuals in a way which could be interpreted as tool use, for example, ants crossing water over a bridge of other ants, or weaver ants using conspecifics to glue leaves together. These have been termed "social tools".[13]

Borderline examples

Play: Play has been defined as “activity having no immediate benefits and structurally including repetitive or exaggerated actions that may be out of sequence or disordered”.[14] When play is discussed in relation to manipulating objects, it is often used in association with the word "tool".[15] Some birds, notably crows, parrots and birds of prey "play" with objects, many of them playing in flight with such items as stones, sticks, leaves, by letting them go and catching them again before they reach the ground. A few species repeatedly drop stones, apparently for the enjoyment of the sound effects.[16] Many other species of animals, both avian and non-avian, play with objects in a similar manner and this appears to be consistent with the definition of tool use by Shumaker et al. given above.[5] However, if this is the case, there appears no logical reason to exclude play behaviour and object manipulation by domestic pets such as cats, dogs and horses, making them potentially, tool-users.[original research?]


Fixed "devices:" The impaling of prey on thorns by many of the Shrikes (Laniidae) is well known.[17] Several other birds may use spines or forked sticks to anchor a carcass while they flay it with the bill. It has been concluded that "This is an example of a fixed device which serves as an extension of the body, in this case, talons" and is thus a true form of tool use. On the other hand, the use of fixed skewers may not be true tool-use because the thorn (or whatever) is not manipulated by the bird.[16] Leopards perform a similar behaviour by dragging carcasses up trees and caching them in the forks of branches.

Use of baits: Several species of herons such as the Striated Heron (Butorides striatus) will place bread in the water to attract fish.[16][18] Whether this is tool use is disputed because the bread is not manipulated or held by the bird.

Learning and cognition

Tool use by animals may indicate different levels of learning and cognition.

For some animals, tool use is largely instinctive and inflexible. For example, the Woodpecker Finch of the Galápagos Islands use twigs or spines as an essential and regular part of its foraging behaviour, however, these behaviours are often quite inflexible and are not applied effectively in different situations. Other tool use, e.g. chimpanzees using twigs to "fish" for termites, may be developed by watching others use tools and may even be a true example of animal teaching. Tools may even be used in solving puzzles in which the animal appears to experience a "Eureka moment".

In mammals

Primates

File:Gorilla tool use.png

A gorilla using a stick possibly to gauge the depth of water

Tool use has been reported many times in both wild and captive primates, particularly the great apes. The use of tools by primates is varied and includes hunting (mammals, invertebrates, fish), collecting honey, processing food (nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds), collecting water, weapons and shelter.

In 1960, Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to “fish” or "dip" for termites.[19]

Tool manufacture is much rarer than simple tool use and probably represents higher cognitive functioning. Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.[19] In 1990, it was claimed the only primate to manufacture tools in the wild was the chimpanzee.[20] However, since then, several primates have been reported as tool makers in the wild.[21]

Both bonobos and chimpanzees have been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water and using these for grooming. Sumatran orangutans will take a live branch, remove twigs and leaves and sometimes the bark, before fraying or flattening the tip for use on ant or bees.[22] In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.[23] Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.[24]

Chimpanzees

Common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are sophisticated tool users with behaviors including cracking nuts with stone tools and fishing for ants or termites with sticks. There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo (Pan paniscus) using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity,[25] It has been reported that both female chimpanzees and bonobos use tools more avidly than males.[26] Wild chimpanzees predominantly use tools in the context of food acquisition while wild bonobos appear to use tools mainly for personal care (cleaning, protection from rain) and social purposes.

Orangutans

Orangutans (genus Pongo) were first observed using tools in the wild in 1994 in the northwest corner of Sumatra.[27] As with the chimpanzees, orangutans use tools made from branches and leaves to scratch, scrape, wipe, sponge, swat, fan, hook, probe, scoop, pry, chisel, hammer, cover, cushion and amplify. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites.[22][28] Sumatran orangutans use a variety of tools - up to 54 types for extracting insects or honey, and as many as 20 types for opening or preparing fruits such as the hard to access Neesia Malayana.[29] They also use an 'autoerotic tool' - a stick which they use to stimulate the genitals and masturbate (both male and female).[30][31] In parts of Borneo, orangutans use handfuls of leaves as napkins to wipe their chins while orangutans in parts of Sumatra use leaves as gloves, helping them handle spiny fruits and branches, or as seat cushions in spiny trees.[32] There have been reports that individuals in both captivity and in the wild use tools held between the lips or teeth, rather than in the hands.[33] In captivity, orangutans have been taught to chip stone handaxes.[34]

Gorillas

There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild.[35] An adult female Western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge. One possible explanation for the absence of observed tool use in wild gorillas is that they are less dependent on foraging techniques that require the use of tools, since they exploit food resources differently from chimpanzees. Whereas chimpanzees and orangutans feeding involves tools such as hammers to crack open nuts and sticks to fish for termites, gorillas access these foods by breaking nuts with their teeth and smashing termite mounds with their hands.[36]

Among the remaining primates, capuchin monkeys, baboons, mandrills and crab-eating macaques are known to use tools, either in captivity or in the wild.

Capuchin monkeys

The black-striped capuchin (Sapajus libidinosus) was the first non-ape primate for which tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone.[37] Robust capuchin monkeys (genus Sapajus) use stone tools in the wild.[37][38][39][40][41] It may take a capuchin up to 8 years to master this skill.[42] These are used to pound open hard shelled fruits, seeds and even oysters. They have also been known to use tools for digging and probing in the dirt. Robust capuchins are also known at times to rub defensive secretions from arthropods over their bodies before eating them;[38] such secretions are believed to act as natural insecticides.

Baboons

Tool use by baboons in the wild was mentioned by Charles Darwin in his book The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex.[43]

Brehm states, on the authority of the well-known traveller Schimper, that in Abyssinia when the baboons belonging to one species (C. gelada) descend in troops from the mountains to plunder the fields, they sometimes encounter troops of another species (C. hamadryas), and then a fight ensues. The Geladas roll down great stones, which the Hamadryas try to avoid...

Darwin continued:

Brehm, when accompanying the Duke of Coburg-Gotha, aided in an attack with fire-arms on a troop of baboons in the pass of Mensa in Abyssinia. The baboons in return rolled so many stones down the mountain, some as large as a man's head, that the attackers had to beat a hasty retreat; and the pass was actually for a time closed against the caravan.

These rather anecdotal reports of stone throwing by baboons have been corroborated by more recent research on chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) troops living on the desert floor of the Kuiseb Canyon in South West Africa. Stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side. The stones tumble down the side of the cliff or falls directly to the canyon floor. The researchers recorded 23 such incidents involving the voluntary release of 124 stones.[44]

A subadult male from a captive group of Guinea baboons (Papio papio) learned, by trial-and-error, to use a tool to rake in food. He then used the tool 104 times over 26 days, thereby providing the group with most of its food.[45]

Mandrills

In the wild mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.[23]

Crab-eating macaques

In Thailand and Myanmar, crab-eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails (nerites, muricids, trochids, etc.) along the Andaman sea coast and offshore islands.[46]

Hunting mammals

Research in 2007 showed that common chimpanzees sharpen sticks to use as weapons when hunting mammals. This is considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans. Researchers documented 22 occasions where wild chimpanzees on a savanna in Senegal fashioned sticks into "spears" to hunt lesser bush babies (Galago senegalensis).[47] In each case, a chimpanzee modified a branch by breaking off one or two ends and, frequently using its teeth, sharpened the stick. The tools, on average, were about 60 cm (24 in) long and 1.1 cm (0.4 in) in circumference. The chimpanzee then jabbed the spear into hollows in tree trunks where bush babies sleep.[48] There was a single case in which a chimpanzee successfully extracted a bush baby with the tool. The tools are referred to as "spears", however, they differ from throwing spears in the sense that they are jabbed into tree trunks and branches, not thrown. It has been suggested the word "spear" is an overstatement that makes the chimpanzees seem too much like early humans and the term "bludgeon" is more accurate.[2] Females, particularly adolescent females, and young chimps in general were seen exhibiting this behaviour more frequently than adult males.[49]

Chimpanzees regularly eat the marrow of long bones of colobus monkeys with the help of small sticks, after opening the ends of the bones with their teeth.[50] A juvenile female was observed to eat small parts of the brain of an intact skull that she could not break open by inserting a small stick through the foramen magnum. On another occasion, an adult female used three sticks to clean the orbits of a colobus monkey skull after she had just eaten the eyes.[20]

Hunting invertebrates

In the Gombe National Park in 1960, Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee, David Greybeard, poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what David was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. David had been using the grass as a tool to “fish” or "dip" for termites.[19] Soon after this initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed David and other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves, and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans made and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.[19]

More recent studies of the Gombe chimps show that young females and males learn to fish for termites differently. Female chimps learn to fish for termites earlier and better than the young males.[51] Females also spend more time fishing while at the mounds with their mothers - males spend more time playing. When they are adults, females need more termite protein because with young to care for, they can't hunt the way males can.[52]

Populations differ in the prevalence of tool use for fishing for invertebrates. Chimpanzees in the Tai National Park only sometimes use tools, whereas Gombe chimpanzees rely almost exclusively on tools for their intake of driver ants. This may be due to difference in the rewards gained by tool use; Gombe chimpanzees collect 760 ants/min compared to 180 ants/min for the Tai chimpanzees.[20]

Some chimpanzees use tools to hunt large bees (Xylocopa sp.) which make nests in dead branches on the ground or in trees. To get to the grubs and the honey, the chimpanzee first tests for the presence of adults by probing the nest entrance with a stick. If present, adult bees block the entrance with their abdomens, ready to sting. The chimpanzee then disables them with the stick to make them fall out and eats them rapidly. Afterwards, the chimpanzee opens the branch with its teeth to obtain the grubs and the honey.[20]

Chimpanzees have even been observed using two tools, a stick to dig into an ant nest and a "brush" made from grass stems with their teeth to collect the ants.[20]

Wild bearded capuchins use sticks to flush prey from inside rock crevices.[11]

Hunting fish

Orangutans living in Borneo scavenge fish that wash up along the shore and scoop catfish out of small ponds for fresh meals. Over two years, anthropologist Anne Russon saw several animals on these forested islands learn on their own to jab at catfish with sticks, so that the panicked prey would flop out of ponds and into the orangutan's waiting hands.[53] Although orangutans usually fished alone, Russon observed pairs of apes catching catfish on a few occasions.[54] On the island of Kaja in Borneo, a male orangutan was observed using a pole apparently trying to spear or bludgeon fish. This individual had seen humans fishing with spears. Although not successful, he was later able to improvise by using the pole to catch fish already trapped in the locals' fishing lines.[55]

Collecting honey

Honey of four bee species is eaten by chimpanzees. Groups of chimpanzees fish with sticks for the honey after having tried to remove what they can with their hands. They usually extract with their hands honeycombs from undisturbed hives of honey bees and run away from the bees to quietly eat their catch. By contrast, hives that have already been disturbed either through the falling of the tree or because of the intervention of other predators, are cleaned of the remaining honey with fishing tools.[20]

Sumatran orangutans will use a stick to poke a bees' nest wall, move it around and catch the honey.[28]

Processing nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds

File:Chimpanzee and stick.jpg

A chimpanzee gathering food with a stick

Tai chimpanzees crack open nuts with rocks while there is no record of Gombe chimpanzees using rocks in this manner.[19] After opening nuts by pounding with a hammer, parts of the kernels may be too difficult to be reached with the teeth or fingernails and some individuals use sticks to remove these remains, instead of pounding the nut further with the hammer as other individuals do;[20] a relatively rare combination of using two different tools. Hammers for opening nuts may be either wood or stone.

Chimpanzees in the Nimba Mountains of Guinea, Africa, use both stone and wooden cleavers, as well as stone anvils, to chop up and reduce Treculia fruits into smaller bite-sized portions. These fruits, which can be the size of a volleyball and weigh up to 8.5 kg, are hard and fibrous. But, despite lacking a hard outer shell, they are too large for a chimpanzee to get its jaws around and bite into. Instead, the chimpanzees use a range of tools to chop them into smaller pieces. This is the first account of chimpanzees using a pounding tool technology to break down large food items into bite-sized chunks rather than just extract it from other unobtainable sources such as baobab nuts. It is also the first time wild chimpanzees have been found to use two distinct types of percussive technology, i.e. movable cleavers against a non-movable anvil, to achieve the same goal. Neighbouring chimpanzees in the nearby region of Seringbara do not process their food in this way, indicating how tool use among apes is culturally learnt.[56]

Sumatran orangutans use sticks to acquire seeds from a particular fruit.[57] When the fruit of the Neesia tree ripens, its hard, ridged husk softens until it falls open. Inside are seeds that are highly desirable to the orangutans, but they are surrounded by fibreglass-like hairs that are painful if eaten. A Neesia-eating orangutan will select a 12 cm stick, strip off the bark, and then carefully collect the hairs with it. Once the fruit is safe, the ape will eat the seeds using the stick or its fingers.[28]

Wild capuchin monkeys in many areas use stone hammers and anvils to crack nuts and encased seeds.[58][59] They transport stones and nuts to an anvil for this purpose.[60] Capuchins also use stones to excavate tubers.[11]

Collecting water

When chimpanzees can't reach water that has formed in hollows high up inside trees, they have been observed taking a handful of leaves, chewing them, and dipping this “sponge” into the pool to suck out the water.[52]

Both bonobos and chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water and are used as grooming tools.[61]

Other uses

Gorrila tool use-Efi

A gorilla pushing a stick into the ground and using it as a stabilizing stick while dredging aquatic herbs

Western lowland gorillas have been observed using sticks to apparently measure the depth of water and as "walking sticks" to support their posture when crossing deeper water.[62] Orangutans have also been observed using sticks to apparently measure the depth of water.

It has been reported that orangutans use tools for a wide range of purposes including using leaves as protective gloves or napkins, using leafy branches to swat insects or gather water, and building sun or rain covers above the nests used for resting.[63] It has been reported that a Sumatran orangutan used a large leaf as an umbrella in a tropical rainstorm.[28]

Orangutans produce an alarm call known as a “kiss squeak” when they encounter a predator like a snake or a human. Sometimes, orangutans will strip leaves from a branch and hold them in front of their mouth when making the sound. It has been found this lowers the maximum frequency of the sound i.e. makes it deeper, and in addition, smaller orangutans are more likely to use the leaves. It has been suggested they use the leaves to make themselves sound bigger than they really are, the first case of an animal using a tool to manipulate sound.[64]

Some examples of tool use in wild bonobos include using leaves as cover for rain, or the use of branches in social displays.[25]

A troop of wild macaques which regularly interact with humans have learnt to remove hairs from the human's heads, and use the hair to floss their teeth.[65]

Captive western lowland gorillas have been observed to threaten each other with sticks and larger pieces of wood, while others use sticks for hygienic purposes. Some females have attempted to use logs as ladders.[66] In another group of captive gorillas, several individuals were observed throwing sticks and branches into a tree, apparently to knock down leaves and seeds.[67] Gorillas at Prague zoo have used tools in several ways, including using wood wool as "slippers" when walking on the snow or to cross a wet section of the floor.[24]

In March 2009, it was reported that "Santino", a male chimpanzee in Furuvik zoo (Sweden), had planned hundreds of stone-throwing attacks on visitors to the zoo. He cached hundreds of stones in areas facing the public when he was in a calm state, prior to the zoo opening in the morning. The launching of the stones occurred hours later during dominance displays to zoo visitors, with Santino in an "agitated" state. Further, the chimpanzee learned to recognise how and when parts of his concrete enclosure could be pulled apart to fashion further projectiles.[68][69]

Tool use has been observed in capuchin monkeys both in captivity and in their natural environments. In a captive environment, capuchins readily insert a stick into a tube containing viscous food that clings to the stick, which they then extract and lick.[70] Capuchins also use a stick to push food from the center of a tube retrieving the food when it reaches the far end,[71] and as a rake to sweep objects or food toward themselves.[72]

Elephants

Elephants show an ability to manufacture and use tools with their trunk and feet. Both wild and captive Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) use branches to swat flies or scratch themselves.[73][74] Eight of 13 captive Asian elephants, maintained under a naturalistic environment, modified branches and switched with the altered branch, indicating this species is capable of the more rare behaviour of tool manufacture. There were different styles of modification of the branches, the most common of which was holding the main stem with the front foot and pulling off a side branch or distal end with the trunk. Elephants have been observed digging holes to drink water, then ripping bark from a tree, chewing it into the shape of a ball thereby manufacturing a "plug" to fill in the hole, and covering it with sand to avoid evaporation. They would later go back to the spot to drink.

Asian elephants may use tools in insightful problem solving. A captive male was observed moving a box to a position where it could be stood upon to reach food that had been deliberately hung out of reach.[75][76]

Elephants have also been known to drop large rocks onto an electric fence to either ruin the fence or cut off the electricity.[77]

Canines

In 2011, researchers at the Dingo Discovery and Research Centre in Melbourne, Australia, filmed a dingo manipulating a table and using this to get food.[78]

Bears

Moulting Brown bears in Alaska have been observed using rocks to exfoliate.[79]

Cetaceans

At least some wild bottlenose dolphins use tools.[80] In Shark Bay, dolphins place a marine sponge on their rostrum, presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom. This has only been observed in this bay (first in 1997), and predominantly by females.[81][82] Bottlenose dolphins have also been observed using conch shells to trap small fish, then lifting the shells out of the water causing the fish to fall into the dolphin's mouth.[83]

Cetaceans, such as beluga whales and dolphins, blow bubble rings.[84] Dolphins sometimes engage in complex play behaviours, creating bubble rings on purpose, seemingly for amusement.[85] There are two main methods of bubble ring production: rapid puffing of a burst of air into the water and allowing it to rise to the surface, forming a ring; or swimming repeatedly in a circle and then stopping to inject air into the helical vortex currents thus formed. The dolphin will often then examine its creation visually and with sonar. They also appear to enjoy biting the vortex-rings they've created, so that they burst into many separate normal bubbles and then rise quickly to the surface.[86]

Humpback whales use a method of hunting know as "bubble net feeding"; a group of whales swims in a shrinking circle blowing bubbles below a school of prey. The shrinking ring of bubbles encircles the school and confines it in an ever-smaller cylinder. This ring can begin at up to 30 metres (Template:Convert/ft)Template:Convert/test/A in diameter and involve the cooperation of a dozen animals.[87] Some whales blow the bubbles, some dive deeper to drive fish toward the surface, and others herd prey into the net by vocalizing. The whales then suddenly swim upward through the "net", mouths agape, swallowing thousands of fish in one gulp.[88] Another form of bubble-net feeding is "lobtail feeding". This technique involves the whale slapping the surface of the ocean with its tail between one and four times before creating the bubble net.[89] Dolphins also use bubble net feeding.[90]

Sea otters

File:Sea Otteruses a rock to break a shell open 2.jpg

A sea otter using a rock to break open a shell.

Under each foreleg, the sea otter (Enhydra lutris) has a loose pouch of skin that extends across the chest. In this pouch (preferentially the left one), the animal stores collected food to bring to the surface. This pouch also holds a rock, unique to the otter, that is used to break open shellfish and clams.[91] To open hard shells, it may pound its prey with both paws against the rock which it places on its chest. Furthermore, sea otters will use large stones to pry an abalone off its rock; they will hammer the abalone shell with observed rates of 45 blows in 15 seconds or 180 rpm, and do it in two–three dives. Releasing an abalone, which can cling to rock with a force equal to 4,000 times its own body weight, requires multiple dives by the otter.[92]

Mongooses

Wild banded mongooses (Mungos mungo) regularly use anvils to open food items with a hard shell such as rhinoceros beetles, bird eggs, snail shells or pupating dung beetles. They use a range of anvils commonly including rocks and the stems of trees, but will also use the side-walls of gullys and even dried elephant dung. Pups as young as 2 months of age are already showing the behavioural patterns associated with using an anvil, however, successful smashing is usually shown in individuals older than 6 months of age.[93]

American badgers

North American badgers (Taxidea taxus) hunt Richardson's ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii). The most common hunting technique is excavation of burrow systems, but plugging of openings into ground-squirrel tunnels accounts for 5–23% of hunting actions. Badgers usually use soil from around the tunnel opening, or soil dragged 30–270 cm from a nearby mound to plug tunnels. The least common (6%), but most novel, form of plugging used by 1 badger involved movement of 37 objects from distances of 20–105 cm to plug openings into 23 ground-squirrel tunnels on 14 nights.[94]

In birds

Tool use is found in at least thirty-three different families of birds.[10]

According to Jones and Kamil's definition,[7] an Egyptian vulture dropping a bone on a rock would not be considered using a tool since the rock cannot be seen as an extension of the body. However, the use of a rock manipulated using the beak to crack an ostrich egg would qualify the Egyptian vulture as a tool user. Many other species, including parrots, corvids and a range of passerines, have been noted as tool users.[95]

<div class="thumb tright" style="width: Expression error: Unrecognized punctuation character "[".px; ">

Bird nests show a great diversity in complexity

Intricate nests of Weaver Birds[96]
Relatively simple nest of a Greater Black-backed Gull
Emperor Penguins do not build a nest[97]

Many birds (and other animals) build nests.[98] It can be argued that this behaviour constitutes tool use according to the definitions given above; the birds "carry objects (twigs, leaves) for future use", the shape of the formed nest prevents the eggs rolling away and thereby "extends the physical influence realized by the animal", and the twigs are bent and twisted to shape the nest, i.e. "modified to fit a purpose". The complexity of bird nests varies markedly, perhaps indicating a range in the sophistication of tool use. For example, compare the highly complex structures of Weaver Birds to the simple mats of herbaceous matter with a central cup constructed by Gulls, and it is noteworthy that some birds do not build nests, e.g. Emperor Penguins. The classification of nests as tools has been disputed on the basis that the completed nest, or burrow, is not held or manipulated.[5]

Woodpecker Finches

Perhaps the best known and most studied example of an avian tool user is the Woodpecker Finch (Camarhynchus pallidus) from the Galápagos Islands. If the bird uncovers prey in bark which is inaccessible, the bird then flies off to fetch a cactus spine which it may use in one of three different ways: as a goad to drive out an active insect (without necessarily touching it): as a spear with which to impale a slow-moving larva or similar animal; or as an implement with which to push, bring towards, nudge or otherwise manoeuvre an inactive insect from a crevice or hole. Tools that are not exactly fitting the purpose are worked by the bird and adapted for the function thus making the Finch a "tool maker" as well as a "tool user". Some individuals have been observed to use a different type of tool with novel functional features - barbed twigs from blackberry bushes, a plant that is not native to the islands. The twigs were first modified by removing side twigs and leaves and then used such that the barbs helped drag prey out of tree crevices.[10]

There is a genetic predisposition for tool use in this species, which is then refined by individual trial-and-error learning during a sensitive phase early in development. This means that, rather than following a stereotypical behavioural pattern, tool use can be modified and adapted by learning.

The importance of tool use by Woodpecker Finches species differs between vegetation zones. In the arid zone, where food is limited and hard to access, tool use is essential, especially during the dry season. Up to half of the finches' prey is acquired with the help of tools, making them even more routine tool users than chimpanzees. The tools allow them to extract large, nutritious insect larvae from tree holes, making tool use more profitable than other foraging techniques. In contrast, in the humid zone, Woodpecker Finches rarely use tools, since food availability is high and prey is more easily obtainable. Here, the time and energy costs of tool use would be too high.[10]

There have been reported cases of Woodpecker Finches brandishing a twig as a weapon.[16]

Corvids

File:Cognitive-Processes-Associated-with-Sequential-Tool-Use-in-New-Caledonian-Crows-pone.0006471.s003.ogv

A New Caledonian Crow showing sequential tool use in the laboratory

Corvids are a family of birds characterised by relatively large brains, remarkable behavioural plasticity (especially highly innovative foraging behaviour) and well-developed cognitive abilities.[10]

In the wild

An American Crow (Corvus brachyrhynchos) has been observed to modify and use a piece of wood as a probe.[99] Green Jays (Cyanocorax yncas) have been observed using sticks as tools to extract insects from tree bark,[100] a behavior which has also been observed and filmed in New Caledonian Crows (Corvus moneduloides)[7][101] The birds poke the insects or larvae until they bite the stick in defence and can be drawn out. This "larva fishing" is very similar to the "termite fishing" practised by chimpanzees. In the wild, stick tools are made from twigs, grass stems or similar plant structures whereas captive individuals have been observed to use a variety of materials, including feathers and garden wire. Stick tools can either be non-hooked - being more or less straight and requiring only little modification - or hooked. Construction of the more complex hooked tools typically involves choosing a forked twig from which parts are removed and the remaining end is sculpted and sharpened. New Caledonian Crows also use pandanus tools, made from barbed leaf edges of screw pines (Pandanus spp.) by precise ripping and cutting although the function of the pandanus tools is not understood.[102]

Crows in urban Japan have been filmed[8] using an innovative technique to crack hard-shelled nuts by dropping them onto cross walks (pedestrian crossings) and letting them be run over and cracked by cars. They then retrieve the cracked nuts when the cars are stopped at the red light.[103] In some towns in America, crows drop walnuts onto busy streets so that the cars will crack the nuts.[104]Template:Better source

Hooded Crows (Corvus cornix) use bait to catch fish.[105] Individuals (who may have observed fish being fed bread by humans) will place the bread in the water to attract fish.[16]

In captivity

While young birds in the wild normally learn to make stick tools from elders, a laboratory New Caledonian Crow named "Betty" was filmed[9] spontaneously improvising a hooked tool from a wire. It was known that this individual had no prior experience as she had been hand-reared.[106] New Caledonian Crows have been observed to use an easily available small tool to get a less easily available longer tool, and then use this to get an otherwise inaccessible longer tool to get food that was out of reach of the shorter tools. One bird, "Sam", spent 110 seconds inspecting the apparatus before completing each of the steps without any mistakes. This is an example of sequential tool use, which represents a higher cognitive function compared to many other forms of tool use and is the first time this has been observed in non-trained animals. Tool use has been observed in a non-foraging context, providing the first report of multi-context tool use in birds. Captive New Caledonian Crows have used stick tools to make first contact with objects that were novel and hence potentially dangerous, while other individuals have been observed using a tool when food was within reach but placed next to a model snake. It has been claimed "Their [New Caledonian Crow] tool-making skills exceed those of chimpanzees and are more similar to human tool manufacture than those of any other animal."[10]

Other species, such as rooks (Corvus frugilegus), can also make and use tools in the laboratory, showing a degree of sophistication similar to that of New Caledonian Crows.[10]

While not confirmed to have used tools in the wild, captive Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) have been observed using strips of newspaper as tools to obtain food.[107][108]

Play

Common ravens (Corvus corax) are one of only a few species who make their own toys. They have been observed breaking off twigs to play with socially.[109] At least one corvid has been filmed repeatedly sliding down a snowy roof while balancing on a coffee lid[110] and another playing with a table tennis ball in partnership with a dog.[111]- potentially both rare examples of tool use for the purposes of play. Blue Jays, like other corvids, are highly curious and are considered intelligent birds. Young Blue Jays playfully snatch brightly coloured or reflective objects, such as bottle caps or pieces of aluminium foil, and carry them around until they lose interest.

Warblers

File:Common Tailorbird (Orthotomus sutorius) Nest in Hyderabad, AP W IMG 7248.jpg

A pouch constructed by a Tailorbird which contains the nest

The Tailorbird (genus Orthotomus) takes a large growing leaf (or two or more small ones) and with its sharp bill pierces holes into opposite edges. It then grasps spider silk, silk from cocoons, or plant fibres with its bill, pulls this "thread" through the two holes, and knots it to prevent it from pulling through (although the use of knots is disputed[112]). This process is repeated several times until the leaf or leaves forms a pouch or cup in which the bird then builds its nest.[16][113] The leaves are sewn together in such a way that the upper surfaces are outwards making the structure difficult to see. The punctures made on the edge of the leaves are minute and do not cause browning of the leaves, further aiding camouflage. The processes used by the Tailorbird have been classified as sewing, rivetting, lacing and matting. Once the stitch is made, the fibres fluff out on the outside and in effect they are more like rivets. Sometimes the fibres from one rivet are extended into an adjoining puncture and appear more like sewing. There are many variations in the nest and some may altogether lack the cradle of leaves.It is believed that only the female performs this sewing behaviour.[112] The Latin binomial name of the Common Tailorbird, Orthotomus sutorius, means "straight-edged" "cobbler" rather than tailor.[114]

Some birds of the genus Prinia also practice this sewing and stitching behaviour.[115]

Parrots

Many owners of household parrots have observed their pets using various tools to scratch various parts of their bodies. These tools include discarded feathers, bottle caps, popsicle sticks, matchsticks, cigarette packets and nuts in their shells.[16]

A captive Tanimbar Corella (Cacatua goffiniana) was observed spontaneously breaking off and "shaping" splinters of wood and small sticks to create rakes that were then used to retrieve otherwise unavailable food items on the other side of the aviary mesh - a rare example of tool-making.[116][117] This behaviour has been filmed.[10]

Hyacinth Macaws (Anodorhynchus hyacinthinus) have been repeatedly observed to use tools when breaking open nuts, for example, pieces of wood being used as a wedge. Several birds have wrapped a piece of leaf around a nut to hold it in place. This behaviour is also shown by Palm Cockatoos (Probosciger aterrimus). It seems that the Hyacinth Macaw has an innate tendency to use tools during manipulation of nuts, as naïve juveniles tried out a variety of objects in combination with nuts.[10]

Egyptian Vultures

When an Egyptian Vulture (Neophron percnopterus) encounters a large egg, it takes a stone into its beak and forcefully throws it at the egg until the shell is broken, usually taking a few minutes. This behaviour, first reported in 1966,[118] seems to be largely innate and is displayed by naïve individuals. Its origin could be related to the throwing of eggs and, interestingly, rounded (egg-like) stones are preferred to jagged ones.[119]

In a small population in Bulgaria, Egyptian Vultures use twigs to collect sheep wool for padding their nests. Although both twigs and wool can serve as nesting material, this appears to be deliberate tool use. The birds approached bits of discarded wool with a twig in their beak, which was then either used as a rake, to gather the wool into heaps, or to roll up the wool. Interestingly, wool was collected only after shearing or simulated shearing of sheep had taken place, but not after wool had simply been deposited in sheep enclosures.[120]

Brown-headed Nuthatches

Brown-headed Nuthatches (Sitta pusilla) have been observed to methodically use bark pieces to remove other flakes of bark from a tree. The birds insert the bark piece underneath an attached bark scale, using it like a wedge and lever, to expose hiding insects. Occasionally, they reuse the same piece of bark several times and sometimes even fly short distances carrying the bark flake in their beak. The evolutionary origin of this tool use might be related to these birds frequently wedging seeds into cracks in the bark to hammer them open with their beak, which can lead to bark coming off.

Brown-headed Nuthatches have also used a bark flake for concealing a seed cache.[10]

Gulls

Seagulls have been known to drop live oyster shells on paved and hard surfaces so that cars can drive over them and break the shell. So many get dropped that it is difficult to drive safely near some waterways. Certain species (e.g. the Herring Gull) have exhibited tool use behavior, using pieces of bread as bait to catch goldfish, for example.[121]

Owls

Burrowing Owls (Athene cunicularia) frequently collect mammalian dung, which they use as a bait to attract dung beetles, a major item of prey.[122]

In reptiles

Tool use by American alligators and Mugger crocodiles has been documented. During the breeding season, birds such as herons and egrets look for sticks to build their nests. Alligators and crocodiles collect sticks to use as bait to catch birds. The crocodilian positions itself near a rookery, partially submerges with the sticks balanced on its head, and when a bird approaches to take the stick, it springs its trap. This stick displaying strategy is the first known case of a predator not only using an object as a lure, but also taking into account the seasonal behavior of its prey.[123][124]

In cephalopods

File:Octopus shell.jpg

A small octopus (4-5 cm diameter) individual using a nut shell and clam shell as shelter

At least four Veined Octopus (Amphioctopus marginatus) individuals were witnessed retrieving coconut shells, manipulating them, stacking them, transporting them some distance (up to 20 metres), and then reassembling them to use as a shelter.[125] The octopuses use coconut shells discarded by humans which have eventually settled in the ocean. They probe their arms down to loosen the mud, then rotate the shells out. After turning the shells so the open side faces upwards, the octopuses blow jets of mud out of the bowl before extending their arms around the shell - or if they have two halves, stacking them first, one inside the other. They then stiffen their legs and move away in a manner which has been called "stilt-walking". The octopuses eventually use the shells as a protective shelter in areas where little other shelter exists. If they just have one half, they simply turn it over and hide underneath. But if they are lucky enough to have retrieved two halves, they assemble them back into the original closed coconut form and sneak inside. This behaviour has been filmed.[11] The authors of the research article claimed this behaviour falls under the definition of tool use because the shells are carried for later use. However, this argument remains contested by a number of other biologists who state that the shells actually provide continuous protection from abundant bottom-dwelling predators in their home range.

Octopuses deliberately place stones, shells and even bits of broken bottle to form a wall that constricts the aperture to the den - a type of tool use.[126]

In laboratory studies, Octopus mercatoris, a small pygmy species of octopus, has been observed to block its lair using a plastic Lego block.[14]

In fish

Several species of wrasses have been observed using rocks as anvils to crack bivalve (scallops, urchins and clams) shells. It was first filmed[12] in an orange-dotted tuskfish (Choerodon anchorago) in 2009 by Giacomo Bernardi. The fish fans sand to unearth the bivalve, takes it into its mouth, swims several metres to a rock which it uses as an anvil by smashing the mollusc apart with sideward thrashes of the head. This behaviour has been recorded in a blackspot tuskfish (Choerodon schoenleinii) on Australia's Great Barrier Reef, yellowhead wrasse (Halichoeres garnoti) in Florida and a six-bar wrasse (Thalassoma hardwicke) in an aquarium setting. These species are at opposite ends of the phylogenetic tree in this family, so this behaviour may be a deep-seated trait in all wrasses.[127]

It has been reported that freshwater stingrays use water as a tool by manipulating their bodies to direct a flow of water and extract food trapped amongst plants.[128]

Prior to laying their eggs on a vertical rock face, male and female whitetail major damselfish clean the site by sand-blasting it. The fish pick up sand in their mouths and spit it against the rock face. Then they fan the area with their fins. Finally they remove the sand grains that remain stuck to the rock face by picking them off with their mouths.[129]

Banded acara, (Bujurquina vittata), South American cichlids, lays their eggs on a loose leaf. The male and female of a mating pair often “test” leaves before spawning: they pull and lift and turn candidate leaves, possibly trying to select leaves that are easy to move. After spawning, both parents guard the eggs. When disturbed, the parent acara often seize one end of the egg-carrying leaf in their mouth and drag it to deeper and safer locations.[130]

Archerfish are found in the tropical mangrove swamps of India and Australasia. They approach the surface, take aim at insects that sit on plants above the surface, squirt a jet of water at them, and grab them after the insects have been knocked off into the water. The jet of water is formed by the action of the tongue, which presses against a groove in the roof of the mouth. Some archerfish can hit insects up to 1.5 m above the water surface. They use more water, which gives more force to the impact, when aiming at larger prey.

Triggerfish (Pseudobalistes fuscus) blow water to turn sea urchins over and expose their more vulnerable ventral side.[131]

In insects

Ants of the species Conomyrma bicolor pick up stones and other small objects with their mandibles and drop them down the vertical entrances of rival colonies, allowing workers to forage for food without competition.[132]

Hunting wasps of the genus Prionyx use weights (such as compacted sediment or a small pebble) to settle sand surrounding a recently provisioned burrow containing eggs and live prey in order to camouflage and seal the entrance. The wasp vibrates its wing muscles with an audible buzz while holding the weight in its mandibles, and applies the weight to the sand surrounding its burrow, causing the sand to vibrate and settle. Another hunting wasp, Ammophila, uses pebbles to close burrow entrances.[133]

See also

References

  1. Michael Henry Hansell (2005). Animal architecture, Oxford University Press. URL accessed 29 June 2011.
  2. 2.0 2.1 Roach, J. (2007). Chimps use "spears" to hunt mammals, study says. National Geographic News. URL accessed on August 1, 2013.
  3. Tool use in chimpanzees. OneKind. URL accessed on February 1, 2014.
  4. Beck, B., (1980). Animal Tool Behaviour: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals Garland STPM Pub.
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 Shumaker, R.W., Walkup, K.R. and Beck, B.B., (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore
  6. (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Curr. Biol. 19 (23): R1069–R1070.
  7. 7.0 7.1 Jones, T. B. & Kamil, A. C. 1973 Tool-making and tool-using in the northern blue jay. Science 180, 1076–1078.
  8. (2011) The Oxford Handbook of Animal Ethics, Oxford University Press.
  9. Lawick-Goodall, J.V., (1970). Tool using in primates and other vertebrates in Lehrman, D.S, Hinde, R.A. and Shaw, E. (Eds) Advances in the Study of Behaviour, Vol. 3. Academic Press.
  10. 10.0 10.1 10.2 10.3 10.4 10.5 10.6 10.7 10.8 10.9 Tool use in birds. Map Of Life. URL accessed on July 23, 2013.
  11. 11.0 11.1 11.2 11.3 Mannu, M. and Ottoni, E.B., (2009). The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. American Journal of Primatology, 3: 242-251
  12. Mulcahy, N.J., Call, J. and Dunbar, R.I.B., (2005). Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119: 23–32
  13. Pierce, J.D., (1986). A review of tool use in insects. The Florida Entomologist, 69: 95-104
  14. 14.0 14.1 Oinuma, C., (2008). Octopus mercatoris response behavior to novel objects in a laboratory setting: Evidence of play and tool use behavior? In, Octopus Tool Use and Play Behavior.[1]
  15. (2011) "Object Play and Tool Use: Developmental and Evolutionary Perspectives" Anthony D. Pellegrini The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play, Oxford University Press.
  16. 16.0 16.1 16.2 16.3 16.4 16.5 16.6 Boswall, J. (1977). Tool-using by birds and related behaviour. Avicultural Magazine, 83: 88-97
  17. Weathers, Wesley (1983). Birds of Southern California's Deep Canyon, University of California Press.
  18. Birds of Stanford: Tool Using. Stanford University. URL accessed on 2013-205-23.
  19. 19.0 19.1 19.2 19.3 19.4 Tool use, hunting & other discoveries. The Jane Goodall Institute. URL accessed on August 1, 2013.
  20. 20.0 20.1 20.2 20.3 20.4 20.5 20.6 Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees.. Folia Primatol., 54: 86-99.
  21. (2000) The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution, Oxford University Press.
  22. 22.0 22.1 van Schaik, C., Fox, E. and Sitompul, A., (1996). Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften, 83: 186–188
  23. 23.0 23.1 Gill, Victoria Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool. BBC. URL accessed on August 11, 2013.
  24. 24.0 24.1 Vancatova, M. (2008). Gorillas and Tools - Part I. URL accessed on August 4, 2013.
  25. 25.0 25.1 (2010). Bonobos. ApeTag. URL accessed on August 3, 2013.
  26. (2010)A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour, 80: 1023-1033.
  27. Just Hangin' on: Orangutan tools. Nature. URL accessed on August 5, 2013.
  28. 28.0 28.1 28.2 28.3 Sumatran orangutans. OrangutanIslands.com. URL accessed on August 2, 2013.
  29. (2005) World Atlas of Great Apes and Their Conservation, University of California Press.
  30. http://www.aim.uzh.ch/Research/orangutannetwork/CultureList.html
  31. Anne E. Russon, Carel P. van Schaik, Purwo Kuncoro, Agnes Ferisa, Dwi P. Handayani and Maria A. van Noordwijk Innovation and intelligence in orangutans, Chapter 20 in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation, ed. Wich, Serge A., Oxford University Press, 2009 ISBN 9780199213276, p.293
  32. Orangutan behavior. Orangutan International Foundation. URL accessed on August 5, 2013.
  33. O’Malley R.C. and McGrew W.C., (2000). Oral tool use by captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Folia Primatol., 71: 334–341.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  34. Orangutan behavior. Orangutan Foundation International. URL accessed on March 30, 2014.
  35. (2006) Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees, 2006.
  36. Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M. and Fishlock, V., (2005). First observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLoS Biol. Nov;3(11):e380 [2]
  37. 37.0 37.1 Fragaszy, D., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E.B., de Oliveira, M.G., (2004). Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology, 64: 359–366.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  38. 38.0 38.1 Lynch Alfaro, J.W., Silva, J.S. and Rylands, A.B., (2012). How different are robust and gracile capuchin monkeys? an argument for the use of Sapajus and Cebus. American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 273-286. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.222007 [3]
  39. Ottoni, E.B., Izar, P., (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17: 171–178.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  40. Ottoni, E.B. and Mannu, M., (2001). Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. International Journal of Primatology, 22: 347–358. doi=10.1023/A:1010747426841 [4]
  41. Boinski, S., Quatrone, R. P. & Swartz, H. (2008). Substrate and tool use by brown capuchins in Suriname: Ecological contexts and cognitive bases. American Anthropologist 102 (4): 741–761.
  42. Life series. 2009. Episode 1. BBC.
  43. Darwin, C., (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. John Murray, London, pp. 51-52
  44. Hamilton, W.J., Buskirk, R.E. and Buskirk, W.H., (1975). Defensive stoning by baboons. Nature, 256: 488-489.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  45. Beck, B., (1973). Observation learning of tool use by captive Guinea baboons (Papio papio). American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 38: 579–582
  46. Gumert, MD (2009). The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand. American Journal of Primatology 71: 594–608.
  47. (2009) Cognitive Archaeology and Human Evolution, Cambridge University Press.
  48. Raffaele, Paul (2011). Among the Great Apes: Adventures on the Trail of Our Closest Relatives, Harper.
  49. includeonly>"Chimpanzees 'hunt using spears'", BBC, February 22, 2007. Retrieved on August 11, 2013.
  50. Shipman, Pat (2011). The Animal Connection: A New Perspective on What Makes Us Human, W. W. Norton & Company.
  51. Raffaele, Paul (2011). Among the Great Apes: Adventures on the Trail of Our Closest Relatives, Harper.
  52. 52.0 52.1 Study corner - tool use. The Jane Goodall Institute. URL accessed on August 1, 2013.
  53. Bower, Bruce (May 7, 2011). Template:Subscription required Borneo Orangs Fish for Their Dinner: Behavior Suggests Early Human Ancestors Were Piscivores. Science News 179 (10).
  54. includeonly>Bower, B.. "Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish", Wired, April 18, 2011. Retrieved on August 5, 2013.
  55. includeonly>"Orangutan attempts to hunt fish with spear", MailOnline, April 26, 2008. Retrieved on August 1, 2013.
  56. includeonly>Walker, M.. "Chimps use cleavers and anvils as tools to chop food", BBC, December 24, 2009. Retrieved on August 11, 2013.
  57. (2012) The Evolution of Primate Societies, University of Chicago Press.
  58. Fragaszy, D. et al., (2004). Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology, 64: 359-366
  59. Ottoni, E.B. and Izar, P., (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17: 171-187
  60. Visalberghi, E. et al., (2009). Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded capuchin monkeys. Current Biology, 3: 213-217
  61. Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology, Harvard University. Dept. of Anthropology (1995). Symbols, Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology.
  62. Gross, L. (2005). Wild gorillas handy with a stick. URL accessed on August 2, 2013.
  63. Evidence for orangutan culture. DailyNews. URL accessed on August 2, 2013.
  64. Zielinski, S. (2009). Orangutans use leaves to sound bigger. URL accessed on August 5, 2013.
  65. The One Show. Television programme broadcast by the BBC on March 26, 2014
  66. Fontaine, B., Moisson, P.Y. and Wickings, E.J. (1995). Observations of spontaneous tool making and tool use in a captive group of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla).
  67. Nakamichi, M. (July 1999). Spontaneous use of sticks as tools by captive gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates, 40: 487-498.
  68. includeonly>"Zoo chimp 'planned' stone attacks", BBC, March 9, 2009. Retrieved on August 11, 2013.
  69. Current Biology, volume 19, issue 5, 10 March 2009, pages 190-191.
  70. Westergaard, G.C., et al. (1998). Why some capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use probing tools (and others do not). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112: 207-211
  71. Visalberghi, E, et al., (1995). Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109: 52-60
  72. Cummins-Sebree, S.E. and Fragaszy, D., (2005). Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119: 210-219
  73. Hart, B. J.; Hart, L. A.; McCory, M.; Sarath, C. R. (2001). Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: use and modification of branches for fly switching. Animal Behaviour 62 (5): 839–47.
  74. Holdrege, Craig (Spring 2001). Elephantine intelligence. In Context (5).
  75. (2011)Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant. PLoS ONE 6(8): e23251. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023251.
  76. includeonly>"Elephants show smarts at National Zoo", The Washington Post, August 19, 2011.
  77. Poole, Joyce (1996). Coming of Age with Elephants, 131–133, 143–144, 155–157, Chicago, Illinois: Trafalgar Square.
  78. Dingoes use tools to solve novel problems
  79. Wild bear uses a stone to exfoliate
  80. Stanford, Craig B. (2001). Significant Others, Basic Books.
  81. Smolker, R.A., et al. (1997). Sponge Carrying by Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use?. Ethology 103 (6): 454–465.
  82. Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. URL accessed on 2006-10-24.
  83. Ingenious fishing method may be spreading through dolphins. Murdoch University.
  84. Prager, Ellen (2011). Sex, Drugs, and Sea Slime: The Oceans' Oddest Creatures and Why They Matter, University of Chicago Press.
  85. The physics of bubble rings and other diver's exhausts. URL accessed on 2006-10-24.
  86. Bubble rings: Videos and Stills. URL accessed on 2006-10-24.
  87. Kaldveer, Urmas (2012). The Others: The Whale People a Personal Journey of Discovery, Transformation, and Healing, Balboa Press.
  88. includeonly>Acklin, Deb. "Crittercam Reveals Secrets of the Marine World", National Geographic News, August 5, 2003. Retrieved on November 1, 2007.
  89. Allen, Jenny (26 April 2013). Network-Based Diffusion Analysis Reveals Cultural Transmission of Lobtail Feeding in Humpback Whales. Science 340 (6131): 485-188.
  90. Leighton, T. (2012). Do Dolphins think nonlinearly?. URL accessed on July 7, 2013.
  91. Haley, D. (ed.) (1986). "Sea Otter" Marine Mammals of Eastern North Pacific and Arctic Waters, 2nd, Seattle, Washington: Pacific Search Press.
  92. Sea Otters, Enhydra lutris. Marinebio. URL accessed on July 7, 2013.
  93. Müller, C.A., (2010). Do anvil-using banded mongooses understand means-end relationships? A field experiment. Animal Cognition, 13: 325-330 [5]
  94. Michener, Gail R. (2004). Hunting techniques and tool use by North American badgers preying on Richardson's ground squirrels. Journal of Mammalogy 85 (5): 1019–1027.
  95. Emery, N.J., (2006). Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 361: 23–43 [6]
  96. Fors, Carl (2006). Hens: Why Women Are Different, Infinity Publishing.
  97. (2001) Biology of Marine Birds, CRC Press.
  98. (2011) François Vuilleumier Birds of North America: Eastern Region, Dorling Kindersley.
  99. Caffrey, Carolee (2000). Tool Modification and Use by an American Crow. Wilson Bull. 112 (2): 283–284.
  100. (1982). Tool use by Green Jays. Wilson Bulletin 94 (4): 593–594.
  101. al_history.shtml Tool use in New CaledonianCrows. Behavioural Ecology Research Group. URL accessed on February 19, 2013.
  102. , Gavin R. (7 February 2004)Template:Subscription required The Crafting of Hook Tools by Wild New Caledonian Crows. Proceedings: Biological Sciences 271 (Supplement 3).
  103. DK Publishing (2011). Animal Life, DK.
  104. Crows Using Automobiles as Nutcrackers. URL accessed on 2014-03-29.
  105. Bait-Fishing in Crows. URL accessed on 2009-02-07.
  106. Crow making tools
  107. Blue Jay. birds.cornell.edu
  108. Jones, Thony B. & Kamil, Alan C. (1973). Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay. Science 180 (4090): 1076–1078.
  109. Heinrich, B., (1999). Mind of the Raven: Investigations and Adventures with Wolf-Birds pp 282. New York: Cliff Street Books. ISBN 978-0-06-093063-9
  110. Template:Youtube
  111. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QqLU-o7N7Kw Dog and corvid playing with a ball
  112. 112.0 112.1 Common Tailorbird. Answers.com.. URL accessed on July 22, 2013.
  113. Tailorbird. Animal Planet. URL accessed on July 22, 2013.
  114. Common Tailorbird. 10,000 Birds. URL accessed on July 22, 2013.
  115. Prinia. Encyclopaedia Britanica. URL accessed on July 22, 2013.
  116. Auersperg, Alice M.I., Birgit Szabo, Auguste M.P. von Bayern, and Alex Kacelnik (6 November 2012). Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo. Current Biology 22 (21).
  117. Warwicker, M. (2012). Cockatoo shows tool-making skills. BBC Nature. URL accessed on July 21, 2013.
  118. van Lawick-Goodall, J. and van Lawick, H., (1966). Use of tools by the Egyptian Vulture, Neophron percnopterus. Nature, 212(5069): 1468–1469.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  119. Thouless, C.R., Fanshawe, J.H. and Bertram, B.C.R., (1989). Egyptian Vultures Neophron percnopterus and Ostrich Struthio camelus eggs: The origins of stone-throwing behaviour. Ibis, 131: 9–15.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  120. Stoyanova, Y., Stefanov, N. and Schmutz, J.K., (2010). Twig used as a tool by the Egyptian Vulture (Neophron percnopterus). Journal of Raptor Research, 44(2): 154–156.
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  121. Henry, Pierre-Yves (June 2006). Tool-use in Charadrii: Active Bait-Fishing by a Herring Gull. Waterbirds 29 (2): 233–234.
  122. (June 8, 2014)Animal behaviour: Use of dung as a tool by burrowing owls. Nature;431(7004):39..
  123. Dinets,V., J.C. Brueggen and J.D. Brueggen (2013). Crocodilians use tools for hunting. Ethology, Ecology & Evolution DOI:10.1080/03949370.2013.858276.
  124. Crocodiles are cleverer than previously thought: Some crocodiles use lures to hunt their prey. ScienceDaily. URL accessed on December 8, 2013.
  125. Finn, J.K., Tregenza, T. and M.D., (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology,
    1. REDIRECT Template:Doi
  126. (2010). Simple tool use in owls and cephalopods. Map Of Life. URL accessed on July 23, 2013.
  127. Bernardi, G. (2011). The use of tools by wrasses (Labridae). DOI=10.1007/s00338-011-0823-6. URL accessed on July 7, 2013.
  128. includeonly>Bourton, J.. "Clever stingray fish use tools to solve problems", BBC News, January 13, 2010. Retrieved on July 8, 2013.
  129. Keenleyside, M.H.A., (1979). Diversity and Adaptation in Fish Behaviour, Springer-Verlag, Berlin.
  130. Keenleyside, M.H.A. and Prince, C., (1976). Spawning-site selection in relation to parental care of eggs in Aequidens paraguayensis (Pisces: Cichlidae). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 54: 2135-2139
  131. Reebs, S.G. (2011). Tool use in fishes. URL accessed on July 7, 2013.
  132. Michael H.J. Möglich & Gary D. Alpert (1979). Stone dropping by Conomyrma bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): A new technique of interference competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2 (6): 105–113.
  133. Sociobiology: the New Synthesis by Edward O. Wilson, p. 172; published 2000, by Harvard University Press (via Google Books)

Further reading

  • (2011) Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals, The Johns Hopkins University Press.

External links



This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).