Wikia

Psychology Wiki

Tail flick test

Talk0
34,135pages on
this wiki

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


File:Tail Flick Test Apparatus.jpg

The tail flick test is a test of the pain response in animals, similar to the hot plate test. It is used in basic pain research and to measure the effectiveness of analgesics, by observing the reaction to heat. It was first described by D'Amour and Smith in 1941.[1]

ProcedureEdit

A light beam is focused on the animal's tail and a timer starts. When the animal flicks its tail, the timer stops and the recorded time is a measure of the pain threshold.[2]

InstrumentsEdit

Instruments have been designed for use with this testing method, including the conduction dolorimeter. The conduction dolorimeter has a resistance wire that has a constant heat flow. For the tail flick test, the wire is attached to the tail of the organism, and the wire applies heat to the tail. The researcher then records the time of the tail flick.[3]

ApplicationsEdit

Researchers testing the effectiveness of drugs on the pain threshold often use the tail flick test to measure the extent to which the drug being tested has reduced the amount of pain felt by the model organism.[4]

Rats are a common model organism for these tests. The rats are usually given analgesics which are responsible for weakening a rat's response to pain. Under these weakened responses to pain, with effectiveness peaking about 30 minutes after ingestion, researchers test the effectiveness of the drugs by exposing the rat's tail to constant heat and measuring how long it takes for the rat's tail to flick, signaling its response to the pain.[5][6]

It was found in experimental tests of the tail flick testing method that the temperature of the skin of the tail plays a major role in the critical temperature, the temperature at which the tail flicks in response to pain. Researchers found that if the tail has been exposed to a cooler temperatures before the test, the critical temperature decreases.[7]

By using the tail flick test, researchers have found that genetics plays a role in pain sensation and the effectiveness of analgesics. A mouse of one genetic line may be more or less tolerant of pain than a mouse of another genetic line. Also, a mouse of one genetic line may experience a higher or lower effectiveness of an analgesic than a mouse of another genetic line. Using this test, researchers can also identify the genes that play a role in pain sensation. For example, the Calca gene is primarily responsible for the variability in thermal (heat) nociception.[8] The Sprawling mutation resulted in a moderate sensory neuropathy but the mutation did not affect nociceptive modality or motor function in the mice. The mice with the Sprawling mutation were unable to sense the pain, but their other sensory functions were unaffected.[9]

LimitationsEdit

The tail flick test is one of the many tests used to measure the sensitivity to heat-induced pain in pain research of live species. The reflex response to heat-induced pain is, for the most part, a good indicator of the pain sensitivity of an organism and the reduction of pain sensitivity by analgesics. However, there are a few significant limitations of heat tests such as the tail flick tests. First of all, there is still much more research needed in the field of pain research using murine subjects; therefore, the validity of translating the observed pain responses from these animals to humans cannot be certain.[10] Also, researchers have found that skin temperature can significantly affect the results of the tail flick test and it's important for one to consider this effect when performing the test.[11] Lastly, Dr. Pissiketti et al. found that many thermal tests do not distinguish between opioid agonists and mixed agonist-antagonists and consequently created a tail flick test for mice using cold water in place of heat in order to make that distinction.[12]

ReferencesEdit

  1. (1941). A method for determining loss of pain sensation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 72: 74–78.
  2. Tzschentke, T. M., Christoph, T.; Kogel, B.; Schiene, K.; Hennies, H.-H.; Englberger, W.; Haurand, M.; Jahnel, U.; Cremers, T. I. F. H.; Friderichs, E.; De Vry, J. (23 July 2007). ( )-(1R,2R)-3-(3-Dimethylamino-1-ethyl-2-methyl-propyl)-phenol Hydrochloride (Tapentadol HCl): a Novel  -Opioid Receptor Agonist/Norepinephrine Reuptake Inhibitor with Broad-Spectrum Analgesic Properties. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 323 (1): 265–276.
  3. (1960). Pharmacology of a Series of New 2-Substituted Pyridine Derivatives with Emphasis on their Analgesic and Interneuronal Blocking Properties. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 128.
  4. Doebel K, Gagneux A. "Certain Imidazolone Derivatives and Process for Making Same." US Patent [Internet]. 2012 [2012 September 29]; 1. Available from: http://www.google.com/patents?hl=en&lr=&vid=USPAT3303199&id=tWUeAAAAEBAJ&oi=fnd&dq=tail+flick+test&printsec=abstract#v=onepage&q=tail%20flick%20test&f=false
  5. Irwin S, Houde RW, Bennett DR, Hendershot LC, Seevers MH (February 1951). The effects of morphine methadone and meperidine on some reflex responses of spinal animals to nociceptive stimulation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 101 (2): 132–43.
  6. Fender C, Fujinaga M, Maze M (January 2000). Strain differences in the antinociceptive effect of nitrous oxide on the tail flick test in rats. Anesth. Analg. 90 (1): 195–9.
  7. Rand R, Burton A, Ing T. "The Tail of the Rat, in Temperature Regulation and Acclimatization." NRC Research Test. 2012; 257-267. DOI:10.1139/y65-025
  8. Mogil, Jeffrey S., "The Surprising Complexity of Pain Testing in the Laboratory Mouse." (2007). 11–23.
  9. {{{title}}}.
  10. (2001). Animal Models of Nociception. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 53 (4): 597–652.
  11. Berge OG, Garcia-Cabrera I, Hole K (April 1988). Response latencies in the tail-flick test depend on tail skin temperature. Neurosci. Lett. 86 (3): 284–8.
  12. Pizziketti RJ, Pressman NS, Geller EB, Cowan A, Adler MW (December 1985). Rat cold water tail-flick: a novel analgesic test that distinguishes opioid agonists from mixed agonist-antagonists. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 119 (1-2): 23–9.

External linksEdit


This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).

Around Wikia's network

Random Wiki