Psychology Wiki
Register
Advertisement

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Social psychology: Altruism · Attribution · Attitudes · Conformity · Discrimination · Groups · Interpersonal relations · Obedience · Prejudice · Norms · Perception · Index · Outline


Self & identity
Brain animated color nevit

Articles

Identity
Other articles

A social identity is the portion of an individual's self-concept derived from perceived membership in a relevant social group.[1][2] As originally formulated by Henri Tajfel and John Turner in the 1970s and 80s,[3] social identity theory introduced the concept of a social identity as a way in which to explain intergroup behaviour.[4][5]

Social identity theory is best described as primarily a theory that predicts certain intergroup behaviours on the basis of the perceived status, legitimacy and permeability of the intergroup environment.[4] This contrasts with occasions where the term social identity theory is used to refer to general theorizing about human social selves.[6] Moreover, and although some researchers have treated it as such,[7][8] social identity theory was never intended to be a general theory of social categorization.[3] It was awareness of the limited scope of social identity theory that led John Turner and colleagues to develop a cousin theory in the form of self-categorization theory,[1][9] which built on the insights of social identity theory to produce a more general account of self and group processes.[3] To avoid confusion the term social identity approach, or social identity perspective, is suggested for describing the joint contributions of both social identity theory and self-categorization theory.[9][10]

Aspects of the theory

File:4-14 Marines in Fallujah.jpg

Henri Tajfel suggests that soldiers of opposing armys, fighting outside of view, is an illustrative example of behaviour at the extreme intergroup end of the intergroup-interpersonal continuum.[11]

The interpersonal-intergroup continuum

Social identity theory states that social behaviour will vary along a continuum between interpersonal behavior and intergroup behavior.[4] Completely interpersonal behavior would be behavior determined solely by the individual characteristics and interpersonal relationships that exists between two or more people. Completely intergroup behaviour would be behaviour determined solely by the social category memberships that apply to two or more people. It is toward this latter end of the spectrum where an individual’s social identities are predicated to be highly influential.

The authors of social identity theory state that purely interpersonal or purely intergroup behaviour is unlikely to be found in realistic social situations.[4][11] Rather, behaviour is expected to be driven by a compromise between the two extremes. The cognitive nature of personal vs. social identities, and the relationship between them, is more fully developed in self-categorization theory.[12][13][3][14] Social identity theory instead focuses on the social structural factors that will predict which end of the spectrum will most influence an individual’s behaviour, along with the forms that that behavior may take.

Positive distinctiveness

A key assumption in social identity theory is that individuals are intrinsically motivated to achieve positive distinctiveness. That is, individuals “strive for a positive self-concept”.[4] As individuals to varying degrees may be defined and informed by their respective social identities (as per the interpersonal-intergroup continuum) it is further derived in social identity theory that “individuals strive to achieve or to maintain positive social identity”.[4] It should be noted that the precise nature of this strive for positive self-concept is a matter of debate (see the self esteem hypothesis).

Both the interpersonal-intergroup continuum and the assumption of positive distinctiveness motivation arose as outcomes of the findings of minimal group studies.[3] In particular, it was found that under certain conditions individuals would endorse resource distributions that would maximize the positive distinctiveness of an ingroup in contrast to an outgroup at the expense of personal self-interest.[15]

File:LaurynHill cropped.jpg

The “black is beautiful” movement and the associated African American embrace of African hairdos (e.g. an afro), culture, traditions and music was provided by Tafjel as an example of the cognitive creativity of low status groups in the face of stable intergroup relations.[4][16]

Positive distinctiveness strategies

Building on the above components, social identity theory details a variety of strategies that may be invoked in order to achieve positive distinctiveness.[9] The individual’s choice of behaviour is posited to be dictated largely by the perceived intergroup relationship. In particular the choice of strategy is an outcome of the perceived permeability of group boundaries, as well as the perceived stability and legitimacy of the intergroup status hierarchy. The self enhancing strategies detailed in social identity theory are detailed below.[4] Importantly, although these are viewed from the perspective of a low status group member, comparable behaviours may also be adopted by high status group members.[9]

Individual mobility

It is predicted that under conditions where the group boundaries are considered permeable (e.g. a group member may pass from a low status group into a high status group) individuals are more likely to engage in individual mobility strategies. That is, individuals “disassociate from the group and pursue individual goals designed to improve their personal lot rather than that of their ingroup”.[9]

Social creativity

Where group boundaries are considered impermeable, and where status relations are considered reasonably stable, individuals are predicted to engage in social creativity behaviours. Here, without changing necessarily the objective resources of in the ingroup or the outgroup, low status ingroup members are still able to increase their positive distinctiveness. This may be achieved by comparing the ingroup to the outgroup on some new dimension, changing the values assigned to the attributes of the group, and choosing an alternative outgroup by which to compare the ingroup.

Social competition

Here an ingroup seeks positive distinctiveness via direct competition with the outgroup in the form of ingroup favoritism. It is considered competitive in that in this case favoritism for the ingroup occurs on a value dimension that is shared by all relevant social groups (in contrast to social creativity scenarios). Social competition is predicted to occur where group boundaries are considered impermeable, and where status relations are considered to be reasonably unstable. Although not privileged in the theory, it is this identity management strategy that has received the greatest amount of attention.[17]

Development

File:Tajfel.jpg

Social psychologist Henri Tajfel

Historical background

The field of social psychology is characterised by a certain degree of conflict between an individualistic approach and an interactionist approach to social cognition and behaviour. By the late 1920s however, the collectivist perspective had all but disappeared from mainstream social psychology.[18] Around the time of the first formal statement of social identity theory, Tajfel wrote this on the state of social psychology:

“Thus, social categorization is still conceived as a haphazardly floating ‘independent variable’ which strikes at random as the spirit moves it. No links are made or attempted, between the conditions determining its presence and mode of operation, and its outcomes in widely diffused commonalities of social behaviour. Why, when and how is social categorisation salient or not salient? What kind of shared constructions of social reality, mediated through social categorization, lead to a social climate in which large masses of people feel they are in long-term conflict with other masses? What, for example, are the psychological transitions from a stable to an unstable social system?”[19] (Original emphasis)

Thus, social identity theory in part reflects a desire to re-establish a more collectivist approach to social psychology of the self and social groups.[18]

Implications

Ingroup favoritism

In-group favoritism (also sometimes known as ingroup bias, despite Turner's objections[13]) is an effect where people give preferential treatment to others when they are perceived to be in the same ingroup. Social identity attributes the cause of ingroup favoritism to a psychological need for positive distinctiveness and describes the situations where ingroup favoritism is likely to occur (as a function of perceived group status, legitimacy, stability, and permiability).[4] It has been shown via the minimal group studies that ingroup favoritism may occur for both arbitrary ingroups (e.g. a coin toss may split participants into a 'heads' group and a 'tails' group) as well as non-arbitrary ingroups (e.g. ingroups based on cultures, genders, and first languages).[20][21]

Importantly, “although vulgarized versions of social identity theory argue that ‘social identification leads automatically to discrimination and bias’, in fact…discrimination and conflict are anticipated only in a limited set of circumstances”.[9] The likening of social identity theory with social competition and ingroup favouritism is partly attributable to the fact that early statements of the theory included empirical examples of ingroup favouritism, while alternative identity management strategies were at that stage theoretical assertions.[6] Regardless, in some circles the prediction of a straight forward identification-bias correlation has earned the pejorative title "SIT-lite".[22]

Continued study into the relationship between social categorization and ingroup favoritism has explored the relative prevalences of the ingroup favoritism vs. outgroup descrimination,[23] as well as the relationship between ingroup favoritism and other psychological constraints (e.g. existential threat).[24]

Applications

Prejudice

Main article: Prejudice

Prejudice is drawing (typically) negative assumptions about someone or something before having enough information to guarantee accuracy of those judgments. In respect to social identity, the integrated threat theory of prejudice states that four types of perceived threats felt from an out-group act as triggers for inter-group prejudice: realistic threats (those to body and possessions, for example), symbolic threats (those to ways of life), inter-group anxiety, and negative stereotypes.[25] In studies of cultural prejudice, not all four types of threats need to be involved for prejudice to be observed.[26] Additional research in cultural prejudice discovered that realistic threats have larger impacts on prejudice displayed by people who highly identify with the in-group, symbolic threats and negative stereotypes have no significant effect differences between high and low identifiers, and inter-group anxiety plays a more significant role for low identifiers.[27]

Additionally, social identity influences the perception of a person being prejudiced. In-group members tend to give each other the benefit of the doubt in ambiguous situations, attributing events to external rather than internal causes.[28] As such, research shows that people who share in-group status with the potential targets of potentially prejudicial behavior, as well as people who display moral credentials, are less likely to be judged as prejudiced by in-group members than by out-group members.[29]

Controversies

The self-esteem hypothesis

Social identity theory proposes that people are motivated to achieve and maintain positive concepts of themselves. Michael Hogg and Dominic Abrams propose a fairly direct relationship between positive social identity and self-esteem. In what has become known as the 'self-esteem hypothesis', self-esteem is predicted to relate in-group bias in two ways. Firstly, successful intergroup discrimination elevates self-esteem. Secondly, depressed or threatened self-esteem promotes intergroup discrimination.[30][31] Empirical support for these predictions has been mixed.[32]

Some social identity theorists, including John Turner, consider the self-esteem hypothesis as not canonical to social identity theory. In fact, the self-esteem hypothesis is argued to be conflictual with the tenets of the theory.[13] It is argued that the self-esteem hypothesis misunderstands the distinction between a social identity and a personal identity, and that it neglects the alternative strategies to maintaining a positive self-concept that are articulated in social identity theory (I.e. individual mobility and social creativity). Further, Turner and others argue against a picture of positive distinctiveness as a straight forward need for self esteem or “quasi-biological drive toward prejudice”.[33] They instead favour a somewhat more complex conception of positive self concept as a reflection of the ideologies and social values of the perceiver.

Positive-negative asymmetry

In what has been dubbed the Positive-Negative Asymmetry Phenomenon, researchers have shown that punishing the in-group less benefits self-esteem less than does rewarding the in-group more. From this finding it has been extrapolated that social identity theory is therefore unable to deal with bias on negative dimensions. Social identity theorist however point out that for ingroup favouritism to occur a social identity “must be psychologically salient”, and that negative dimensions may be experienced as a “less fitting basis for self -definition”.[13] This important qualification is subtly present in social identity theory, but is further developed in self-categorization theory. Empirical support for this perspective comes from research where it has been shown that when experiment participants can self-select negative self-definitional dimensions no positive-negative asymmetry is found.[34]

Intergroup similarity

It has been posited that social identity theory suggest straight forwardly that similar groups should have an increased motivation to differentiate themselves from each other.[35][31] Subsequently, empirical findings where similar groups are shown to possess increased levels of intergroup attraction and decreased levels of in-group bias have been interpreted as problematic for the theory.[31] Elsewhere it has been suggested that this apparent inconsistency may be resolved by attending to social identity theory’s emphasis on the importance of the perceived stability and legitimacy of the intergroup status hierarchy.[35]

Predictive power

Social identity theory is purportedly criticised for having far greater explanatory power than predictive power.[18] That is, while the relationship between independent variables and the resulting intergroup behaviour may be consistent with the theory in retrospect, that particular outcome is often not that which was predicted at the outset. A rebuttal to this charge is that the theory was never advertised as the definitive answer to understanding intergroup relationships. Instead it is stated that social identity theory must be go hand in hand with sufficient understanding of the specific social context under consideration.[36][13] The latter argument is consistent with the explicit importance that the authors of social identity theory placed on the role of "objective" factors, stating that in any particular situation "the effects of [social identity theory] variables are powerfully determined by the previous social, economic, and political processes".[4]

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 Turner, John (1986). The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence. British Journal of Social Psychology 25 (3): 237–252.
  2. Hogg, Michael (2002). Social Psychology, Upper Saddle River , NJ: Prentice Hall.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 Turner, J. C. & Reynolds, K. J. (2010). The story of social identity. In T. Postmes & N. Branscombe (Eds). Rediscovering Social Identity: Core Sources. Psychology Press.
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole
  5. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.
  6. 6.0 6.1 (2010). The social identity perspective today: An overview of its defining ideas. Rediscovering social identity: 341–356.
  7. Brown, R. J., & Zagefka, H. (2006). Choice of comparisons in intergroup settings: the role of temporal information and comparison motives. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(5), 649–671.
  8. Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 80–114.
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 9.4 9.5 Haslam, A. S. (2001). Psychology in Organizations. London, SAGE Publications.
  10. Postmes, T. & Branscombe, N. (2010). Sources of social identity. In T. Postmes & N. Branscombe (Eds). Rediscovering Social Identity: Core Sources. Psychology Press.
  11. 11.0 11.1 (1978). Interindividual and intergroup behaviour.. Differentiation between groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations: 27–60.
  12. (1994) Stereotyping and social reality, Blackwell: Oxford.
  13. 13.0 13.1 13.2 13.3 13.4 (2001). The Social Identity Perspective in Intergroup Relations: Theories, Themes, and Controversies. Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology 3 (1).
  14. Haslam, S. Alexander (2011). The new psychology of leadership: Identity, influence and power, New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  15. (1978). Social categorization and social discrimination in the minimal group paradigm. Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations: 235–250.
  16. (1978). The achievement of group differentiation. Differentiation between groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations: 77–100.
  17. (2010). The social identity perspective tomorrow: Opportunities and avenues for advance. Rediscovering social identity: 357–379.
  18. 18.0 18.1 18.2 Hogg, Michael A., Williams, Kipling D. (1 January 2000). From I to we: Social identity and the collective self.. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 4 (1): 81–97.
  19. (1979). Individuals and groups in social psychology. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 18: 183–190.
  20. Brewer, Marilynn B. (1 January 1979). Ingroup bias in the minimal intergroup situations: A cognitive motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin 86 (2): 307–324.
  21. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self-stereotyping and the salience of social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology 26: 325–340.
  22. McGarty, C. (2001). Social Identity Theory does not maintain that identification produces bias, and Self-categorization Theory does not maintain that salience is identification: Two comments on Mummendey, Klink and Brown. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 173-176.
  23. Ahmed, Ali M. (1 June 2007). Group identity, social distance and intergroup bias. Journal of Economic Psychology 28 (3): 324–337.
  24. Giannakakis, Andrew Erik, Fritsche, Immo (1 January 2011). Social Identities, Group Norms, and Threat: On the Malleability of Ingroup Bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37 (1): 82–93.
  25. Stephan, Walter G., Stephan, Cookie W. (1 September 1996). Predicting prejudice. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 20 (3–4): 409–426.
  26. Stephan, Walter G. (1 July 1998). Prejudice toward immigrants to Spain and Israel: An integrated threat theory analysis. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (4): 559–576.
  27. Bizman, Aharon, Yinon, Yoel (1 September 2001). Intergroup and Interpersonal Threats as Determinants of Prejudice: The Moderating Role of In-Group Identification. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 23 (3): 191–196.
  28. Tajfel, Henri (1979). The social psychology of intergroup relations, 33–48, Monteray, CA: Brooks/Cole.
  29. Krumm, Angela J., Corning, Alexandra F. (1 December 2008). Who Believes Us When We Try to Conceal Our Prejudices? The Effectiveness of Moral Credentials With In-Groups Versus Out-Groups.. The Journal of Social Psychology 148 (6): 689–709.
  30. (1990). Social motivation, self-esteem, and social identity. Social identity theory. Constructive and critical advances: 44–70.
  31. 31.0 31.1 31.2 Brown, Rupert (1 November 2000). Social Identity Theory: past achievements, current problems and future challenges. European Journal of Social Psychology 30 (6): 745–778.
  32. Rubin, M., & Hewstone, M. (1998). Social identity theory’s self-esteem hypothesis: A review and some suggestions for clarification. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 40-62.[View]
  33. (1997). The socially structured mind. The message of social psychology: 355–373.
  34. (2000). When are we better than them and they worse than us? A closer look at social discrimination in positive and negative domains. Journal of personality and social psychology 72: 353–372.
  35. 35.0 35.1 (1984). The role of similarity in intergroup relations. The social dimension 2: 603-623.
  36. (1984). Intergroup relations, social myths and social justice in social psychology. The social dimension 2: 695-715.

External links

Template:Social networking

This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).
Advertisement