Psychology Wiki
No edit summary
 
Line 2: Line 2:
 
{{Response}}
 
{{Response}}
   
A '''response cost''' is the negative consequences of a response. By administering a response cost you are attempting to reduce a targeted behavior through withdrawal of specific amounts of previously established [[reinforcers]] contingent upon that behavior's occurrence.
+
A '''response cost''' is the negative consequence of a response. By administering a response cost you are attempting to reduce a targeted behavior through withdrawal of specific amounts of previously established [[reinforcers]] contingent upon that behavior's occurrence.
   
 
==Example==
 
==Example==
Line 78: Line 78:
 
*Turner, B. L. B. (1996). Effects of response cost, bibliotherapy and academic tutoring on attentional behavior, achievement, work productivity, accuracy, and self-esteem in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Turner, Becky Louise Beno: Auburn U, US.
 
*Turner, B. L. B. (1996). Effects of response cost, bibliotherapy and academic tutoring on attentional behavior, achievement, work productivity, accuracy, and self-esteem in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Turner, Becky Louise Beno: Auburn U, US.
 
*Viola, V. J. (1996). The effects of token reinforcement and response cost on Category Test performance of medicated boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Viola, Vincent James: East Texas State U, US.
 
*Viola, V. J. (1996). The effects of token reinforcement and response cost on Category Test performance of medicated boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Viola, Vincent James: East Texas State U, US.
 
 
 
 
[[Category:Behaviour modification]]
 
[[Category:Behaviour modification]]
 
[[Category:Behavior therapy]]
 
[[Category:Behavior therapy]]

Latest revision as of 14:51, 18 April 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


A response cost is the negative consequence of a response. By administering a response cost you are attempting to reduce a targeted behavior through withdrawal of specific amounts of previously established reinforcers contingent upon that behavior's occurrence.

Example

As an example: in a behaviour modification program aimed at reducing a childs aggressive verbal behavior we might deduct a point or token, or a promise of future reward (the response cost), if they swear (the target behavior) in an attempt to reduce the frequency of that behavior.

The concept has been used in token economy programs.

Principals of administration

Advantages

Ventajas

Disadvantages

Animal studies

Token economy studies

See also

References

Books

  • Mitchell, S. H. (2003). Discounting the Value of Commodities According to Different Types of Cost. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Pergamon/Elsevier Science Inc.


Papers

  • Armstrong, K. J., & Drabman, R. S. (1998). Treatment of a nine year old girl's public masturbatory behavior. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 20(1), pp.
  • Bebko, J. M., Demark, J. L., Osborn, P. A., Majumder, S., Ricciuti, C. J., & Rhee, T. (2003). Acquisition and automatization of a complex task: An examination of three-ball cascade juggling. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), pp.
  • Bebko, J. M., Demark, J. L., Osborne, P. A., Majumder, S., Ricciuti, C. J., & Rhee, T. (2003). "Acquisition and automatization of a complex task: An examination of three-ball cascade juggling": Erratum. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(3), pp.
  • Buckley, S. D., & Newchok, D. K. (2005). An evaluation of simultaneous presentation and differential reinforcement with response cost to reduce packing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(3), pp.
  • Buckley, S. D., Strunck, P. G., & Newchok, D. K. (2005). A Comparison Of Two Multicomponent Procedures To Increase Food Consumption. Behavioral Interventions, 20(2), pp.
  • Carlson, C. L., Mann, M., & Alexander, D. K. (2000). Effects of reward and response cost on the performance and motivation of children with ADHD. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(1), pp.
  • Conyers, C., Miltenberger, R. G., Maki, A., Barenz, R., Jurgens, M., Sailer, A., et al. (2004). A comparison of response cost and differential reinforcement of other behavior to reduce disruptive behavior in a preschool classroom. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(3), pp.
  • Crosbie, J., Williams, A. M., Lattal, K. A., Anderson, M. M., & Brown, S. M. (1997). Schedule interactions involving punishment with pigeons and humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68(2), pp.
  • Erika, G. M., & Gustavo, B. M. (2007). Behavioral sequence formation through response cost. Revista Mexicana de Psicologia, 24(2), pp.
  • Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Hovanetz, A. N., Kettering, T. L., & Keeney, K. M. (2004). An evaluation of response cost in the treatment of inappropriate vocalizations maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(1), pp.
  • Foltin, R. W. (1997). Food and amphetamine self-administration by baboons: Effects of alternatives. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68(1), pp.
  • Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., Klein, W., & Kohlisch, O. (2004). Total sleep deprivation increases the costs of shifting between simple cognitive tasks. Acta Psychologica, 117(1), pp.
  • Homburg, A., & Wagner, U. (2007). Factors of individual threat, efficacy and cost appraisal in the context of environmental stressors. Zeitschrift fur Sozialpsychologie, 38(1), pp.
  • Jurbergs, N., Palcic, J., & Kelley, M. L. (2007). School-home notes with and without response cost: Increasing attention and academic performance in low-income children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(3), pp.
  • Keeney, K. M., Fisher, W. W., Adelinis, J. D., & Wilder, D. A. (2000). The effects of response cost in the treatment of aberrant behavior maintained by negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(2), pp.
  • Larrea, C., & Santos, J. (2006). Cost allocation schemes: An asymptotic approach. Games and Economic Behavior, 57(1), pp.
  • Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Corrigendum to "The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: A review and theoretical appraisal". Clinical Psychology Review, 25(4), pp.
  • Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: A review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(2), pp.
  • McGoey, K. E., & DuPaul, G. J. (2000). Token reinforcement and response cost procedures: Reducing the disruptive behavior of preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 15(3), pp.
  • Milella, M. S., Amato, D., Badiani, A., & Nencini, P. (2008). The influence of cost manipulation on water contrafreeloading induced by repeated exposure to quinpirole in the rat. [[Psychopharmacology (journal), 197(3), pp.
  • Neill, D. B., Fenton, H., & Justice, J. B., Jr. (2002). Increase in accumbal dopaminergic transmission correlates with response cost not reward of hypothalamic stimulation. Behavioural Brain Research, 137(1-2), pp.
  • Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Effects of reward and response cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anxious, and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 26(3), pp.
  • Pietras, C. J., & Hackenberg, T. D. (2005). Response-cost punishment via token loss with pigeons. Behavioural Processes, 69(3), pp.
  • Reimeres, T. M. (1996). A biobehavioral approach toward managing encopresis. Behavior Modification, 20(4), pp.
  • Reynolds, L. K., & Kelley, M. L. (1997). The efficacy of a response cost-based treatment package for managing aggressive behavior in preschoolers. Behavior Modification, 21(2), pp.
  • Rogers, P., & Darnley, S. (1997). Self-monitoring, competing response and response cost in the treatment of trichotillomania: A case report. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25(3), pp.
  • Roper, K. L., & Zentall, T. R. (1999). Observing behavior in pigeons: The effect of reinforcement probability and response cost using a symmetrical choice procedure. Learning and Motivation, 30(3), pp.
  • Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The Costs of Changing the Representation of Action: Response Repetition and Response-Response Compatibility in Dual Tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(3), pp.
  • Shibasaki, M., & Kawai, N. (2008). The effects of response cost and time on choosing a stimulus. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 79(3), pp.
  • Sturges, J. W., Sims, J., Omar, K., Balian, R., Angell, C., Davenport, J., et al. (2009). It doesn't feel good: The biggest obstacle to condom use among college students. the Behavior Therapist, 32(2), pp.
  • Thorpe, C. M., & Wilkie, D. M. (2007). Rats acquire a low-response-cost daily time-place task with differential amounts of food. Learning & Behavior, 35(1), pp.
  • Truchlicka, M., McLaughlin, T., & Swain, J. C. (1998). Effects of token reinforcement and response cost on the accuracy of spelling performance with middle-school special education students with behavior disorders. Behavioral Interventions, 13(1), pp.
  • Walton, M. E., Rudebeck, P. H., Bannerman, D. M., & Rushworth, M. F. (2007). Calculating the cost of acting in frontal cortex. In Reward and decision making in corticobasal ganglia networks (pp. 340-356). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Widman, D. R., Gordon, D., & Timberlake, W. (2000). Response cost and time-place discrimination by rats in maze tasks. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28(3), pp.
  • Widman, D. R., Sermania, C. M., & Genismore, K. E. (2004). Evidence for time-place learning in the Morris water maze without food restriction but with increased response cost. Behavioural Processes, 67(2), pp.

Dissertations

  • Allin, J. L. (2006). The effects of vicarious rewards and vicarious response-cost on incidental learning. Allin, Jessica Leigh: U Tennessee, US.
  • Christensen, M. (1997). An interdisciplinary theoretical framework for the mailed questionnaire process and the development of a theory on immediacy and salience as significant variables of response rates. Christensen, Maribeth: Utah State U, US.
  • Greenlaw, C. P. (2007). A comparison of web-based and paper-based survey methods: Testing assumptions of survey mode and response cost. Greenlaw, Corey P : California State U, Fresno And University Of California, Davis, US.
  • McGoey, K. E. (1998). Positive reinforcement and response cost procedures: Reducing the disruptive behavior of preschool children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mcgoey, Kara Elizabeth: Lehigh U, US.
  • Peck, R. J. (2005). Contingent magnitude of reward: An alternative to response cost for behavior suppression. Peck, Rebecca Jean: Argosy U/Seattle, US
  • Turner, B. L. B. (1996). Effects of response cost, bibliotherapy and academic tutoring on attentional behavior, achievement, work productivity, accuracy, and self-esteem in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Turner, Becky Louise Beno: Auburn U, US.
  • Viola, V. J. (1996). The effects of token reinforcement and response cost on Category Test performance of medicated boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Viola, Vincent James: East Texas State U, US.