Psychology Wiki
(Created page with '{{Biopsy}} also called ''orienting reflex'', is the innate reflex that causes an organism to respond immediately to a change in its environment. The phenomenon was first …')
 
m (Reverted edits by 67.167.233.176 (talk | block) to last version by Dr Joe Kiff)
 
(6 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{Biopsy}}
 
{{Biopsy}}
also called ''orienting reflex'', is the innate [[reflex]] that causes an [[organism]] to respond immediately to a change in its environment. The phenomenon was first described by Russian physiologist [[Ivan Sechenov]] in his 1863 book ''Reflexes of the Brain'', and the term was coined by [[Ivan Pavlov]], who also referred to it as the "What is it?" reflex. The orienting response is a reaction to novelty. Such reflexes include:
+
The ''orienting reflex'' (also called the '''orienting response''', is the innate [[reflex]] that causes an [[organism]] to respond immediately to a change in its environment, when that change is not sudden enough to elicit the [[startle response |startle reflex]]. It is characterised by patterns of behavioral and physiological responses to [[novelty|novel]] or potential [[threatening]] stimuli such as : a focusing of [[attention]], a turning of the head and body towards the stimuli and the arousal of the [[reticular activating system]] and the [[sympathetic nervous system]]<ref>Coleman,A F (2006). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology, 2nd ed. Oxford:OUP. </ref>.
  +
  +
Associated reflexes include:
 
*[[Pupil dilation]]
 
*[[Pupil dilation]]
*[[Galvanic skin response]]
+
*[[Galvanic skin response]] changes
 
*[[EEG]] response to [[novel stimuli]]
 
*[[EEG]] response to [[novel stimuli]]
   
==Description==
+
==History==
  +
The phenomenon was first described by Russian physiologist [[Ivan Sechenov]] in his 1863 book ''Reflexes of the Brain'', and the term was coined by [[Ivan Pavlov]], who also referred to it as the ''Shto eto takoi?'' (Что это такое? or ''What is it?'') reflex. The orienting response is a reaction to novel or significant stimuli. In the 1950s the orienting response was studied systematically by the Russian scientist [[Evgeny Sokolov]], who documented the phenomenon called "[[habituation]]", referring to a gradual "familiarity effect" and reduction of the orienting response with repeated stimulus presentations.<ref name="Sokolov, E.N 1960, pp. 187">Sokolov, E.N, Neuronal models and the orienting reflex, in ''The Central Nervous System and Behavior'', Mary A.B. Brazier, ed. NY: JosiahMacy, Jr. Foundation, 1960, pp. 187–276</ref>
When people see a bright flash or light or hear a sudden loud noise, they pay attention to it even before they identify it. This orienting reflex seems to be present from birth. It is useful in helping people react quickly to events that call for immediate action.
 
   
  +
==Physiology==
This reflex can be controlled by the [[cerebral cortex]], but more typically it is controlled by subcortical brain regions.
 
  +
Researchers have found a number of physiological mechanisms associated with OR, including changes in phasic and tonic [[skin conductance response]] (SCR), [[electroencephalogram]] (EEG), and heart rate following a novel or significant stimulus. These observations all occur within seconds of stimulus introduction.<ref name="ReferenceA">{{cite journal | doi = 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x | last1 = Bradley | first1 = M.M. | year = 2008 | title = Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion | url = | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 46 | issue = 1| pages = 1–11 | pmid = 18778317 }}</ref> In particular, EEG studies of OR have corresponded particularly with the [[P300]] wave and [[P3a]] component of the OR-related [[event related potential]] (ERP).<ref name="fMRI">Polich, J. (2003). Overview of P3a and P3b. In J. Polich (Ed.), Detection of Change:Event-Related Potential and fMRI Findings (pp. 83-98). Kluwer Academic Press: Boston.</ref>
  +
  +
===Neural Correlates===
  +
Current understanding of the localization of OR in the brain is still unclear. In one study using [[fMRI]] and SCR, researchers found novel visual stimuli associated with SCR responses typical of an OR also corresponded to activation in the [[hippocampus]], [[anterior cingulate cortex|anterior cingulate gyrus]], and [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]]. These regions are also believed to be largely responsible for emotion, decision making, and memory. Increases in cerebellar and extrastriate cortex were also recorded, which are significantly implicated in visual perception and processing.<ref>{{cite journal | doi = | last1 = Williams | first1 = L | last2 = Brammer | first2 = M | last3 = Skerrett | first3 = D | last4 = Lagapolous | first4 = J | last5 = Rennie | first5 = C | last6 = Kozek | first6 = K | last7 = Oliveri | first7 = G | last8 = Peduto | first8 = T | last9 = Gordon | first9 = E | year = 2000 | title = The neural correlates of orienting: An integration of fMRI and skin conductance orienting | url = http://tamu.edu/faculty/bortfeld/689/689_Spring'07/readings/Williams_etal_NR2000.pdf| journal = Brain Imaging | volume = 11 | issue = 13| pages = 3011–3015 | pmid = }}</ref>
  +
  +
==Function==
 
When an individual encounters a novel environmental stimulus, such as a bright flash of light or a sudden loud noise, he or she will pay attention to it even before identifying it. This orienting reflex seems to be present early in development, as babies will turn their head toward an environmental change{Cowan, 1995}. From an evolutionary perspective, this mechanism is useful in reacting quickly to events that call for immediate action.
  +
  +
===Habituation===
  +
Sokolov's investigation of OR was primarily motivated in understanding habituation. Provided the first introduction of a novel stimulus, defined in Sokolovian terms as any change from the "currently active neuronal model" (what the individual is currently focused on), results in OR. However, with repeated introduction of the same stimulus, the orienting response will decrease in intensity and eventually cease.<ref name="Sokolov, E.N 1960, pp. 187"/> When novel stimuli have an associated contextual significance, repeated stimulus will still result in a sequentially decreasing OR, though at a modified rate of decay.<ref name="ReferenceA"/>
  +
  +
===Orienting in Decision-Making===
  +
The orienting response is believed to play an integral role in preference formation. When faced with deciding between two options, subjects in studies by Simion & Shimojo were shown to choose the items they preferentially orient their gaze toward. This gaze can occur while the stimulus is present or after it has been removed, the latter causing gaze to be fixated at the point in which the stimulus had been present. Interestingly, gaze bias ceases following a decision, suggesting that gaze bias is the cause of preference and not its effect. Noting this postulated causal link with the irrelevance of a stimulus presence, it is argued that gaze orientation supports decision-making mechanisms in inducing a preferential bias.<ref>{{cite journal | doi = | last1 = Simion | first1 = C | last2 = Shimojo | first1 = S | year = 2007 | title = Interrupting the cascade: Orienting contributes to decision making even in the absence of visual stimulation | url = | journal = Perception & Psychophysics | volume = 69 | issue = 4| pages = 591–595 | pmid = 17727112 }}</ref>
  +
  +
===Role between Emotion and Attention===
  +
Both novelty and significance of a stimulation are implicated in the generation of an orienting response. Specifically, the emotional significance of a stimulus, defined by its level of pleasantness, can affect the intensity of the orienting response toward focusing attention on a subject. Studies showed that during exposure to neutral and emotionally significant novel images, both pleasant and unpleasant images produced higher skin conductance readings than neutral images. With repeated stimulation, all skin conductance readings diminished relative to novel introduction, though with emotionally significant content diminishing more slowly. Conversely, studies observing cardiac deceleration during novel stimuli introduction showed significantly more deceleration for unpleasant stimuli compared to pleasant and neutral stimuli. These findings suggest that OR represents a combination of responses that act in tandem to a common stimulus. More importantly, the differences between emotionally charged and neutral stimuli demonstrates the influence of emotion in orienting attention, despite novelty.<ref name="ReferenceA"/>
   
 
==See also==
 
==See also==
  +
*[[Attention]]
 
*[[Orienting responses]]
 
*[[Orienting responses]]
 
*[[Sensory adaptation]]
 
*[[Sensory adaptation]]
   
  +
==References==
  +
<references/>
  +
  +
[[Category:Attention]]
  +
[[Category:Novelty]]
 
[[category:Reflexes]]
 
[[category:Reflexes]]
 
[[category:Sensory adaptation]]
 
[[category:Sensory adaptation]]

Latest revision as of 16:48, 11 November 2016

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Biological: Behavioural genetics · Evolutionary psychology · Neuroanatomy · Neurochemistry · Neuroendocrinology · Neuroscience · Psychoneuroimmunology · Physiological Psychology · Psychopharmacology (Index, Outline)


The orienting reflex (also called the orienting response, is the innate reflex that causes an organism to respond immediately to a change in its environment, when that change is not sudden enough to elicit the startle reflex. It is characterised by patterns of behavioral and physiological responses to novel or potential threatening stimuli such as : a focusing of attention, a turning of the head and body towards the stimuli and the arousal of the reticular activating system and the sympathetic nervous system[1].

Associated reflexes include:

History

The phenomenon was first described by Russian physiologist Ivan Sechenov in his 1863 book Reflexes of the Brain, and the term was coined by Ivan Pavlov, who also referred to it as the Shto eto takoi? (Что это такое? or What is it?) reflex. The orienting response is a reaction to novel or significant stimuli. In the 1950s the orienting response was studied systematically by the Russian scientist Evgeny Sokolov, who documented the phenomenon called "habituation", referring to a gradual "familiarity effect" and reduction of the orienting response with repeated stimulus presentations.[2]

Physiology

Researchers have found a number of physiological mechanisms associated with OR, including changes in phasic and tonic skin conductance response (SCR), electroencephalogram (EEG), and heart rate following a novel or significant stimulus. These observations all occur within seconds of stimulus introduction.[3] In particular, EEG studies of OR have corresponded particularly with the P300 wave and P3a component of the OR-related event related potential (ERP).[4]

Neural Correlates

Current understanding of the localization of OR in the brain is still unclear. In one study using fMRI and SCR, researchers found novel visual stimuli associated with SCR responses typical of an OR also corresponded to activation in the hippocampus, anterior cingulate gyrus, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. These regions are also believed to be largely responsible for emotion, decision making, and memory. Increases in cerebellar and extrastriate cortex were also recorded, which are significantly implicated in visual perception and processing.[5]

Function

When an individual encounters a novel environmental stimulus, such as a bright flash of light or a sudden loud noise, he or she will pay attention to it even before identifying it. This orienting reflex seems to be present early in development, as babies will turn their head toward an environmental change{Cowan, 1995}. From an evolutionary perspective, this mechanism is useful in reacting quickly to events that call for immediate action.

Habituation

Sokolov's investigation of OR was primarily motivated in understanding habituation. Provided the first introduction of a novel stimulus, defined in Sokolovian terms as any change from the "currently active neuronal model" (what the individual is currently focused on), results in OR. However, with repeated introduction of the same stimulus, the orienting response will decrease in intensity and eventually cease.[2] When novel stimuli have an associated contextual significance, repeated stimulus will still result in a sequentially decreasing OR, though at a modified rate of decay.[3]

Orienting in Decision-Making

The orienting response is believed to play an integral role in preference formation. When faced with deciding between two options, subjects in studies by Simion & Shimojo were shown to choose the items they preferentially orient their gaze toward. This gaze can occur while the stimulus is present or after it has been removed, the latter causing gaze to be fixated at the point in which the stimulus had been present. Interestingly, gaze bias ceases following a decision, suggesting that gaze bias is the cause of preference and not its effect. Noting this postulated causal link with the irrelevance of a stimulus presence, it is argued that gaze orientation supports decision-making mechanisms in inducing a preferential bias.[6]

Role between Emotion and Attention

Both novelty and significance of a stimulation are implicated in the generation of an orienting response. Specifically, the emotional significance of a stimulus, defined by its level of pleasantness, can affect the intensity of the orienting response toward focusing attention on a subject. Studies showed that during exposure to neutral and emotionally significant novel images, both pleasant and unpleasant images produced higher skin conductance readings than neutral images. With repeated stimulation, all skin conductance readings diminished relative to novel introduction, though with emotionally significant content diminishing more slowly. Conversely, studies observing cardiac deceleration during novel stimuli introduction showed significantly more deceleration for unpleasant stimuli compared to pleasant and neutral stimuli. These findings suggest that OR represents a combination of responses that act in tandem to a common stimulus. More importantly, the differences between emotionally charged and neutral stimuli demonstrates the influence of emotion in orienting attention, despite novelty.[3]

See also

References

  1. Coleman,A F (2006). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology, 2nd ed. Oxford:OUP.
  2. 2.0 2.1 Sokolov, E.N, Neuronal models and the orienting reflex, in The Central Nervous System and Behavior, Mary A.B. Brazier, ed. NY: JosiahMacy, Jr. Foundation, 1960, pp. 187–276
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 (2008). Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology 46 (1): 1–11.
  4. Polich, J. (2003). Overview of P3a and P3b. In J. Polich (Ed.), Detection of Change:Event-Related Potential and fMRI Findings (pp. 83-98). Kluwer Academic Press: Boston.
  5. (2000). The neural correlates of orienting: An integration of fMRI and skin conductance orienting. Brain Imaging 11 (13): 3011–3015.
  6. (2007). Interrupting the cascade: Orienting contributes to decision making even in the absence of visual stimulation. Perception & Psychophysics 69 (4): 591–595.