Wikia

Psychology Wiki

Changes: Optical illusions

Edit

Back to page

(Well-known illusions)
(Well-known illusions)
Line 38: Line 38:
   
 
==Well-known illusions==
 
==Well-known illusions==
*[[Ehrenstein's square illusion]]
+
*[[Afterimage illusion]]
*[[Ehrenstein's brightness illusion]]
+
*[[Ambiguous image]]
 
*[[Ames room]] illusion
 
*[[Ames room]] illusion
 
*[[Ames Trapezoid Window]] illusion
 
*[[Ames Trapezoid Window]] illusion
Line 51: Line 51:
 
*[[Ebbinghaus illusion]]
 
*[[Ebbinghaus illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein illusion]]
  +
*[[Ehrenstein's brightness illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein's square illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein's square illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein's brightness illusion]]
 
*[[Ehrenstein's brightness illusion]]
Line 73: Line 74:
 
*[[Muller-Lyer distortion illusion]]
 
*[[Muller-Lyer distortion illusion]]
 
*[[Necker cube illusion]]
 
*[[Necker cube illusion]]
  +
*[[Neon color spreading]]
 
*[[Orbison illusion]]
 
*[[Orbison illusion]]
 
*[[Penrose triangle]] aka Impossible triangle illusion
 
*[[Penrose triangle]] aka Impossible triangle illusion
Line 84: Line 86:
 
*[[Sander illusion]]
 
*[[Sander illusion]]
 
*[[Size-weight illusion]]
 
*[[Size-weight illusion]]
  +
*[[Watercolor illusion]]
 
*[[White's illusion]]
 
*[[White's illusion]]
 
*[[Wundt illusion]]
 
*[[Wundt illusion]]

Revision as of 12:16, August 16, 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


This article is in need of attention from a psychologist/academic expert on the subject.
Please help recruit one, or improve this page yourself if you are qualified.
This banner appears on articles that are weak and whose contents should be approached with academic caution
.
Main article: Illusions (perceptual)

An optical illusion is characterized by visually perceived images that, at least in common sense terms, are deceptive or misleading. Therefore, the information gathered by the eye is processed by the brain to give, on the face of it, a percept that does not tally with a physical measurement of the stimulus source. A conventional assumption is that there are physiological illusions that occur naturally and cognitive illusions that can be demonstrated by specific visual tricks that say something more basic about how human perceptual systems work.

Optical.greysquares.arp
An optical illusion. Square A is exactly the same shade of grey as square B. See Same color illusion
LifeartistAdded by Lifeartist
Grid illusion
The Scintillating grid illusion or Hermann grid illusion. Dark spots seem to appear and disappear very quickly at the intersections
LifeartistAdded by Lifeartist
Gradient.illusion.arp
Simultaneous Contrast Illusion. The grey bar is the same shade throughout
LifeartistAdded by Lifeartist
Illusion4
An optical illusion. The two circles seem to move when the viewer's head is moving forwards and backwards while looking at the black dot.
LifeartistAdded by Lifeartist
OpticalIllusionStJohnLateran
Floor tiles at the Basilica of St. John Lateran in Rome. The pattern creates an illusion of three-dimensional boxes.
Dr Joe KiffAdded by Dr Joe Kiff
Grifo mágico
Magic tap, which appears to float in the sky with an endless supply of water. In actuality, there is a pipe hidden in the stream of water.
Dr Joe KiffAdded by Dr Joe Kiff


Physiological illusions

Physiological illusions, such as the afterimages following bright lights or adapting stimuli of excessively longer alternating patterns (contingent perceptual aftereffect), are presumed to be the effects on the eyes or brain of excessive stimulation of a specific type - brightness, tilt, color, movement, and so on. The theory is that stimuli have individual dedicated neural paths in the early stages of visual processing, and that repetitive stimulation of only one or a few channels causes a physiological imbalance that alters perception.

File:Illusion movie.ogg Example movie which produces distortion illusion after you watch it and look away.

Cognitive illusions

Cognitive illusions are assumed to arise by interaction with assumptions about the world, leading to "unconscious inferences", an idea first suggested in the 19th century by Hermann Helmholtz. Cognitive illusions are commonly divided into ambiguous illusions, distorting illusions, paradox illusions, or fiction illusions.

(a). Ambiguous illusions are pictures or objects that elicit a perceptual 'switch' between the alternative interpretations. The Necker cube is a well known example; another instance is the Rubin vase.

(b). Distorting illusions are characterized by distortions of size, length, or curvature. A striking example is the Café wall illusion. Another example is the famous Mueller-Lyer illusion.

(c). Paradox illusions are generated by objects that are paradoxical or impossible, such as the Penrose triangle or impossible staircases seen, for example, in M. C. Escher's Ascending and Descending and Waterfall. The triangle is an illusion dependent on a cognitive misunderstanding that adjacent edges must join.

(d). Fictional illusions are defined as the perception of objects that are genuinely not there to all but a single observer, such as those induced by schizophrenia or a hallucinogenic substance. These are more properly called hallucinations.

Well-known illusions

Many famous artists have worked extensively with optical illusions, including M.C. Escher, Salvador Dalí, Giuseppe Arcimboldo, Marcel Duchamp, Oscar Reutersvärd, and Charles Allan Gilbert.Optical illusion is also used in film by the technique of forced perspective.

Gallery



See also

References & Bibliography

Key texts

Books

  • Purves D, Lotto B (2002) Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

Papers

  • Eagleman, D.M. (2001) Visual Illusions and Neurobiology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2(12): 920-6. (pdf)
  • Gregory Richard (1997) Knowledge in perception and illusion. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 352:1121-1128 (pdf)
  • Purves D, Lotto RB, Nundy S (2002) Why We See What We Do. American Scientist 90 (3): 236-242.
  • Purves D, Williams MS, Nundy S, Lotto RB (2004) Perceiving the intensity of light. Psychological Rev. Vol. 111: 142-158.
  • Yang Z, Purves D (2003) A statistical explanation of visual space.Nature Neurosci 6: 632-640.

Additional material

Books

  • A. Seckel, The Art of Optical Illusions, Carlton Books, 2000

Papers

  • Google Scholar
  • Cooper, A. C. G. & Humphreys, G. W. A New Object-Based Visual Illusion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

External links

Commons-logo
Wikimedia Commons has media related to:


This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).

Around Wikia's network

Random Wiki