Psychology Wiki
(SocPsy)
 
(31 intermediate revisions by 6 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{SocPsy}}
 
   
  +
{{PhilPsy}}
:''See also [[Ethics]].''
 
  +
{{PsyPerspective}}
'''Morality''' deals with that which is regarded as [[Goodness and value theory|right]] or [[evil|wrong]]. The term is used in regard to three contexts: individual [[conscience]]; systems of [[principle]]s and [[judgment]]s — sometimes called [[moral value]]s — shared within a [[cultural]], [[religion|religious]], [[secular]], [[secular humanism|Humanist]], or [[philosophical]] community; and codes of behavior or [[Code of conduct|conduct]] derived from these systems.
 
  +
{{Ethics}}
   
  +
:''See also [[Ethics]] and [[moral psychology]].''
Personal morality defines and distinguishes among right and wrong [[intention]]s, [[Motivation|thoughts]] or [[Action (philosophy)|actions]]. Human [[conscience]] is widely acknowledged to encourage individuals to do right; its origins and role are the subject of much discussion. Belief in an effective system of divine [[Last Judgment|judgment]] often helps with personal motivation, as classically seen in the success of Medieval codes of [[chivalry|knighthood]] and the spread of [[Islamic conquests|Islam]]. The desire to [[conformity|conform]] to the behavior of a group to which an individual belongs or aspires to belong is also a powerful force, though it may generally apply to more general cultural [[norm (sociology)|norm]]s and [[convention (norm)|custom]]s, where the dichotomy is between proper and improper behavior.
 
   
Group morality develops from shared [[concept]]s and [[belief]]s and is often codified to regulate behavior within a [[culture]] or community. Various defined actions come to be called moral or immoral. Individuals who choose moral action are popularly held to possess "moral fibre", whereas those who indulge in immoral behavior may be labelled as socially [[degenerate]]. The continued existence of a group may depend on widespread conformity to codes of morality; an inability to adjust moral codes in response to new challenges is sometimes credited with the demise of a community (a positive example would be the function of [[Cistercian]] reform in reviving monasticism; a negative example would be the role of the [[Empress Dowager Cixi|Dowager Empress]] in the subjugation of China to European interests). Within [[nationalist]] movements, there has been some tendency to feel that a nation will not survive or prosper without acknowledging one, common morality.
 
   
  +
'''Morality''' (from the [[Latin]] ''{{lang|la|moralitas}}'' "manner, character, proper behavior") has three principal meanings.
Codified morality is generally distinguished from [[norm|custom]], another way for a community to define appropriate activity, by the former's derivation from [[Natural rights|natural]] or universal principles. In certain religious communities, the [[Divine]] is said to provide these principles through [[revelation]], sometimes in great detail. Such codes may be called laws, as in the [[Torah|Law of Moses]], or community morality may be defined through commentary on the texts of revelation, as in [[Sharia|Islamic law]]. Such codes are distinguished from legal or judicial [[right]], including [[civil rights]], which are based on the accumulated traditions, decrees and legislation of a political authority, though these latter often invoke the authority of the moral law.
 
   
  +
In its first, descriptive usage, morality means a code of conduct which is held to be authoritative in matters of right and wrong. Morals are created by and define society, philosophy, religion, or individual conscience. An example of the descriptive usage could be "common conceptions of morality have changed significantly over time."
In any society, actual behavior patterns diverge, sometimes seriously, from accepted notions of how one ought to behave. This dissonance is exaggerated for political effects by the [[pundit (politics)|pundit]]s of hypothetical morals, who invite confusion as to the details and applicability of a group's fundamental moral code.
 
   
  +
In its second, normative and universal sense, morality refers to an ideal code of conduct, one which would be espoused in preference to alternatives by all rational people, under specified conditions. In this "prescriptive" sense of morality as opposed to the above described "descriptive" sense, moral value judgments such as "murder is immoral" are made. To deny 'morality' in this sense is a position known as [[moral skepticism]], in which the existence of objective moral "truths" is rejected.<ref>[http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/morality-definition/ The Definition of Morality (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy)<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref>
Morality can also be seen as the collection of beliefs as to what constitutes a good life. Since throughout most of [[human]] [[history]], [[religion]]s have provided both visions and regulations for an [[ideal]] life (through such beliefs characterized by 'the [[god]](s) know what's best for us') morality is often confused with religious [[precept]]s. In secular communities, [[Lifestyle|lifestyle]] choices, which represent an [[individual]]'s conception of the good life, are often discussed in terms of "morality". Individuals sometimes feel that making an appropriate lifestyle choice invokes a true morality, and that accepted codes of conduct within their chosen community are fundamentally moral, even when such codes deviate from more general social principles.
 
   
  +
In its third usage, 'morality' is synonymous with [[ethics]], the systematic philosophical study of the moral domain.<ref>[http://www.philosophyblog.com.au/ethics-vs-morality-the-distinction-between-ethics-and-morals/ Ethics vs morality - the distinction between ethics and morals<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref>
The systematic study of morality is a branch of [[philosophy]] called [[ethics]]. Ethics seeks to address questions such as how one ought to behave in a specific situation ([[applied ethics]]), how one can justify a moral position ([[normative ethics]]), how one should understand the fundamental nature of ethics or morality itself, including whether it has any objective justification ([[meta-ethics]]), and the nature and explanation of moral capacity or the ontogenetic development of moral agency ([[moral psychology]]).
 
   
For example, in applied ethics, three issues that revolve around interpretations of the moral ban on murder &mdash; [[capital punishment]], [[abortion]] and wars of [[invasion]] &mdash; are under contentious discussion in [[United States]] society and politics. In normative ethics, a common question is how one would justify a lie given for the sake of protecting someone from harm. A common meta-ethical question is of what is meant by the terms right or wrong. [[Moral realism]] would hold that the individual is attempting to elucidate some objective moral fact, whereas the various branches of [[moral non-realism]] would hold that morality is derived from either the norms of the prevalent society ([[cultural relativism]]), the edicts of a God ([[Divine Command Theory]]), is merely an expression of the speakers sentiments ([[emotivism]]), is an implied imperative ([[prescriptivism]]) or is literally nonsense ([[Error Theory]]).
+
Ethics seeks to address questions such as how a moral outcome can be achieved in a specific situation ([[applied ethics]]), how moral values should be determined ([[normative ethics]]), what morals people actually abide by ([[descriptive ethics]]), what the fundamental nature of ethics or morality is, including whether it has any objective justification ([[meta-ethics]]), and how moral capacity or moral agency develops and what its nature is ([[moral psychology]]).<ref>[http://www.iep.utm.edu/e/ethics.htm Ethics [Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy&#93;<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> In applied ethics, for example, the prohibition against taking human life is controversial with respect to [[capital punishment]], [[abortion]] and wars of [[invasion]]. In normative ethics, a typical question might be whether a lie told for the sake of protecting someone from harm is justified. In meta-ethics, a key issue is the meaning of the terms "right" or "wrong". [[Moral realism]] would hold that there are true moral statements which report objective moral facts, whereas moral [[anti-realism]] would hold that morality is derived from any one of the [[norm (sociology)|norms]] prevalent in society ([[cultural relativism]]); the edicts of a god ([[divine command theory]]); is merely an expression of the speakers' sentiments ([[emotivism]]); an implied imperative ([[prescriptive (philosophy)|prescriptive]]); falsely presupposes that there are objective moral facts ([[Moral nihilism|error theory]]). Some thinkers hold that there is no correct definition of right behavior, that morality can only be judged with respect to particular situations, within the standards of particular belief systems and socio-historical contexts. This position, known as [[moral relativism]], often cites empirical evidence from anthropology as evidence to support its claims.<ref>[http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/moral-relativism/ Moral Relativism (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy)<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> The opposite view, that there are universal, eternal moral truths are known as [[moral absolutism]]. Moral absolutists might concede that forces of social [[conformity (psychology)|conformity]] significantly shape moral decisions, but deny that cultural [[norm (sociology)|norm]]s and [[convention (norm)|custom]]s define morally right behavior.
   
  +
==Philosophical Perspectives==
== Evolution of morality ==
 
   
  +
===Clarifying the Usage of the Term “Morality”===
Morality is seen from the evolutionary psychology perspective as a product of evolutionary forces and as evidence for continuity with other group-living organisms.<ref>This differs from common view. Some philosophers and biologists hold that morality is a thin crust hiding [[egoism]], [[amorality]], and anti-social tendencies</ref>
 
Proponents of what could be called "Natural Outgrowth Theory" see no conflict between evolutionary biology and morality since moral codes generally prescribe behavior that enhances individual fitness and group well-being.<ref>For example, the taboo against [[inbreeding]] encourages individuals to avoid producing defective offspring that would depress their reproductive [[fitness (biology)|fitness]].</ref> Compliance with and internalization of social conventions leads to a sense of regularity that makes group living more predictable and hence, less stressful, for its members. [[Reciprocity]] ensures a reliable supply of essential resources, especially for animals living in a habitat where food quantity or quality fluctuates unpredictably. <ref>On any given night for [[vampire bat|vampire bats]], some individuals fail to feed on prey while others consume a surplus of blood. Bats that have successfully fed then regurgitate part of their blood meal to save a conspecific from starvation. Since these animals live in close-knit groups over many years, an individual can count on other group members to return the favor on nights when it goes hungry (Wilkinson, 1984).</ref>
 
   
  +
The fact that there are at least three different usages of the term “morality” (see above) has led to much confusion when that word is used in discussions. Because of that confusion, many thinkers are forced to spend a certain amount of time dealing with that confusion before they even begin to use the term “morality” in their discussions.
There are obviously [[natural selection]] pressures driving the incremental development of moral complexity throughout [[Hominidae|hominid]] evolution. <ref>For example, Christopher Boehm (1982) has advanced a possible mechanism. In primate societies, a fight between high-ranking individuals raises the anxiety level of the entire troupe, so that third parties sometimes intervene to bring the quarreling parties to reconcile. A despotic [[dominance]] style like that observed in many [[macaque]] species also causes more stress for subordinates.</ref> As early hominids moved from arboreal to terrestrial habitats, anxiety-induced dispersal behavior would have exposed individuals to predation, forcing our ancestors to develop more efficient conflict management strategies if they were to enjoy the benefits of group living. The invention of stone tools around 2.5 million years ago made fights potentially more injurious, which further increased selection pressure for conflict interference and group controls on dominance behavior.
 
   
  +
One example of that helpful clarification process is found in [[Walter Terence Stace]]’s book, ''The Concept of Morals'', in which he clarifies his own usage of the terms "[[ethics]]" and "morality," and their differences.<ref>
In summary, living in close quarters on the open savanna with ready access to dangerous weapons compelled early hominids to develop strict codes of acceptable behavior.
 
  +
{{cite book
  +
| last = Stace
  +
| first = Walter T.
  +
| authorlink =
  +
| coauthors =
  +
| title = The Concept of Morals
  +
| publisher = The MacMillan Company
  +
| date = 1937, Reprinted 1975 by permission of MacMillan Publishing Co. Inc.
  +
| location = New York
  +
| pages =
  +
| url =
  +
| doi =
  +
| id =
  +
| isbn = 0-8446-2990-1
  +
}}
  +
</ref>
   
  +
* [[Moral realism]] or [[moral objectivism]] holds that moral codes exist outside of human opinion -- that certain things are right or wrong regardless of human opinion on the topic. Objective morality may be seen as stemming from the inherent nature of humanity, divine command, or both.
Some [[evolutionary psychology|evolutionary psychologists]] have argued that human morality originated from evolutionary processes. An innate tendency to develop a sense of right and wrong helps an individual to survive and reproduce in a species with complex social interactions. Selected behaviors, seen in abstraction as moral codes, are seen to be common to all human cultures, and reflect, in their development, similarities to [[natural selection]] and these aspects of morality can be seen in as the basis of some [[religion|religious]] doctrine. From this, some also argue that there may be a simple [[Darwinian]] explanation for the existence of religion: that, regardless of the truth of religious beliefs, religion tends to encourage behavior beneficial to the species, as a code of morality tends to encourage communality, and communality tends to assist survival.
 
  +
* [[Moral subjectivism]] holds that moral codes depend on human opinion.
  +
* [[Moral relativism]] holds that moral codes are a function of human values and social structures, and hold no meaning outside social convention.
  +
* [[Moral absolutism]] is the view that certain acts are right or wrong regardless of context.
  +
* [[Moral universalism]] compromises between moral relativism and moral absolutism and holds that there is, or should be, a common universal core of morality.
  +
* [[Moral nihilism]] is the view that no morality exists.
  +
* [[Amoralism]] is the view that the concepts of moral right and wrong do not have meaning.
  +
There are many other examples.
   
  +
== Anthropological Perspectives ==
These explanations for the existence of morality do not, however, necessarily assist in deciding what is truly ''right'' for future actions. Should an individual's own morality really be determined by what is best for their genetic offspring ([[Wiktionary:Colloquial|colloquially]], but inaccurately, "the good of the species" (see [[group selection]])? Viewholders counter that evolutionary psychology extends millions of years of [[empirical]] justification for our moral sense, provided that sense is indeed innate &mdash; more than recorded history could demonstrate. They claim sensible people would behave with morality knowing [[subconsciousness|subconsciously]] that it has succeeded in the past. Still, an explanation of why and how humans could have a moral basis does not imply that they ''ought'' to hold these views.
 
  +
{{Expand-section|date=June 2008}}
   
  +
=== Tribal and territorial moralities ===
Some observers hold that individuals have distinct sets of moral rules that they apply to different groups of people. There is the "ingroup," which includes the individual and those they believe to be of the same culture or race, and there is the "outgroup," whose members are not entitled to be treated according to the same rules. Some biologists, anthropologists and [[evolutionary psychology|evolutionary psychologists]] believe this ingroup/outgroup difference is an evolutionary mechanism, one which evolved due to its enhanced survival aspects. [[Gary R. Johnson]] and [[V.S. Falger]] have argued that [[nationalism]] and [[patriotism]] are forms of this ingroup/outgroup boundary.
 
   
  +
[[Celia Green]] has made a distinction between tribal and territorial morality.<ref name="Green">Green, Celia (2004). ''Letters from Exile: Observations on a Culture in Decline''. Oxford: Oxford Forum. Chapters I-XX. </ref> She characterizes the latter as predominantly negative and proscriptive: it defines a person’s territory, including his or her property and dependents, which is not to be damaged or interfered with. Apart from these proscriptions, territorial morality is permissive, allowing the individual whatever behaviour does not interfere with the territory of another. By contrast, tribal morality is prescriptive, imposing the norms of the collective on the individual. These norms will be arbitrary, culturally dependent and ‘flexible’, whereas territorial morality aims at rules which are universal and absolute, such as [[Immanuel Kant|Kant]]’s ‘[[categorical imperative]]’. Green relates the development of territorial morality to the rise of the concept of private property, and the ascendancy of contract over status.
The evolutionary critique points to the radical ways which morality differs across times and cultures among human beings. Very few activities are always morally wrong across all human societies. For example, some groups still practice forms of infanticide or incest, activities that would be condemned harshly in most Western societies. It has been argued that morality is simply whatever norms are present within a given society at a given time, while the other argument lies in the existence of morality.
 
   
  +
=== In-group and out-group ===
* Rajiv K. Saxena: [http://www.geeta-kavita.com/article.asp?article=biology_morals_dharma Biology, Morals and Dharma] -- A working paper that focuses on the biological basis of morality
 
  +
{{Expand-section|date=June 2008}}
   
  +
Some observers hold that individuals have distinct sets of moral rules that they apply to different groups of people. There is the "ingroup," which includes the individual and those they believe to be of the same culture or race, and there is the "outgroup," whose members are not entitled to be treated according to the same rules. Some biologists, anthropologists and [[evolutionary psychology|evolutionary psychologists]] believe this ingroup/outgroup difference is an evolutionary mechanism, one which evolved due to its enhanced survival aspects. Gary R. Johnson and V.S. Falger have argued that [[nationalism]] and [[patriotism]] are forms of this ingroup/outgroup boundary.
==Morality in judicial systems==
 
The [[law]] considers itself independent of morality, even if the law happens to reflect or intends to reflect morality. (Of course, it is not difficult toems, the word '''morality''' concretely means a requirement for the access to certain charges or careers, or for the obtaining of certain licenses or concessions, and generally consists of the absence of previous records on (e.g.) crimes, [[bankruptcy]], political or commercial irregularities.
 
   
  +
=== Comparing cultures ===
In some systems, the lack of morality of the individual can also be a sufficient cause for punishment, or can be an element for the grading of the punishment.
 
   
  +
Peterson and Seligman <ref>Peterson, Christopher, and Martin E. P. Seligman. ''Character Strengths and Virtues''. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2004.</ref> approach the anthropological view looking across cultures and across millennia. The conclude that certain virtues have prevailed in all cultures they examined. The major virtues they identified include ''wisdom / knowledge; courage; humanity; justice; temperance; and transcendence''. Each of these includes several divisions. For instance ''humanity'' includes ''love'', ''kindness'', and ''social intelligence''.
Especially in the systems where modesty (i.e., with reference to sexual crimes) is legally protected or otherwise regulated, the definition of morality as a legal element and in order to determine the cases of infringement, is usually left to the vision and appreciation of the single judge and hardly ever precisely specified. In such cases, it is common to verify an application of the prevalent common morality of the interested community, that consequently becomes enforced by the law for further reference.
 
   
  +
[[Fons Trompenaars]], author of [[Did the Pedestrian Die?]], tested members of different cultures with various [[moral dilemma]]s. One of these was whether the driver of a car would have his friend, a passenger riding in the car, lie in order to protect the driver from the consequences of driving too fast and hitting a pedestrian. Trompenaars found that different cultures had quite different expectations (from none to almost certain)<!-- , and in some cultures it mattered whether the pedestrian had died to how much assistance would be expected -->.
The government of [[South Africa]] is attempting to create a Moral Regeneration movement. Part of this is a proposed [[Bill of Morals]], which will bring a biblical-based "moral code" into the realm of law. This move by a nominally secular democracy has attracted relatively little criticism.
 
   
  +
== Evolutionary perspectives ==
==Comparative morality among cultures==
 
  +
{{Expand-section|date=June 2008}}
  +
{{see also|Evolution of morality}}
  +
:''Further:[[Altruism#Altruism in ethology and evolutionary biology|Altruism]]
   
  +
Some evolutionary biologists, particularly sociobiologists, believe that morality is a product of evolutionary forces acting at an individual level and also at the group level through [[group selection]] (though whether "group selection" actually occurs is a controversial topic in evolutionary theory). Some sociobiologists contend that the set of behaviors that constitute morality evolved largely because they provided possible survival and/or reproductive benefits (i.e. increased evolutionary success). Humans consequently evolved "pro-social" emotions, such as feelings of empathy or guilt, in response to these moral behaviors.
There has been considerable work done in studying comparative morality among cultures. To such researchers, morality is not seen as a constant essential "truth" but as a series of values that is influenced by (and influences) the cultural context. This is often called [[moral relativism]].
 
  +
  +
In this respect, morality is not absolute, but relative and constitutes any set of behaviors that encourage human cooperation based on their ideology. Biologists contend that all social animals, from ants to elephants, have modified their behaviors, by restraining selfishness in order to make group living worthwhile. Human morality, though sophisticated and complex relative to other animals, is essentially a natural phenomenon that evolved to restrict excessive individualism and foster human cooperation.
  +
<ref name="shermer">{{cite book
  +
|title=[[The Science of Good and Evil]]
  +
|isbn=0805075208
  +
|last=Shermer
  +
|first=Michael
  +
|authorlink=Michael Shermer
  +
|chapter=Transcendent Morality
  +
|chapterurl=http://books.google.com/books?id=eevvWAcMBaAC&pg=PA19&dq=shermer+exegesis&ei=EIC1SNOiE4uWyATTmaj2Bg&sig=ACfU3U3KFh8kP8Ns8-YgpqBuI03N1JrpEg
  +
}}</ref>
   
  +
On this view, moral codes are ultimately founded on emotional instincts and intuitions that were selected for in the past because they aided survival and reproduction ([[inclusive fitness]]). The strength of the [[maternal bond]] is one example. Another is the [[Imprinting (psychology)#Westermarck effect|Westermarck effect]], seen as underpinning [[Incest taboo|taboos against incest]], which decreases the likelihood of [[inbreeding depression]].
One well known commentator is [[Fons Trompenaars]], author of [[Did the Pedestrian Die?]], which tested various moral propositions. One of these was whether the driver of a car would have his friend, a passenger riding in the car, lie in order to protect the driver from the consequences of driving too fast and hitting a pedestrian. Trompenaars found that different cultures had quite different expectations (from none to almost certain), and in some cultures it mattered whether the pedestrian had died to how much assistance would be expected.
 
   
  +
<!-- kinship altruism theory -->
== Moral Codes ==
 
  +
<!-- handicap altruism theory -->
  +
<!-- reputation theory -->
  +
<!-- reciprocity theory -->
  +
The phenomenon of '[[reciprocity]]' in nature is seen by evolutionary biologists as one way to begin to understand human morality. Its function is typically to ensure a reliable supply of essential resources, especially for animals living in a habitat where food quantity or quality fluctuates unpredictably. For example, on any given night for [[vampire bat]]s, some individuals fail to feed on prey while others consume a surplus of blood. Bats that have successfully fed then regurgitate part of their blood meal to save a conspecific from starvation. Since these animals live in close-knit groups over many years, an individual can count on other group members to return the favor on nights when it goes hungry (Wilkinson, 1984)
   
  +
It has been convincingly demonstrated that chimpanzees show empathy for each other in a wide variety of contexts.<ref>{{cite journal |last=O’Connell |first= Sanjida |authorlink= |coauthors= |year= 1995|month= July|title= Empathy in chimpanzees: Evidence for theory of mind? |journal= primates|volume= 36|issue= 3|pages= 397–410|id= 0032-8332 |url= |accessdate= 2007-11-08 |quote=|doi= 10.1007/BF02382862 }}</ref> They also possess the ability to engage in deception, and a level of social 'politics'<ref>[http://www.amazon.com/dp/0674356616 Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals].</ref> prototypical of our own tendencies for [[gossip]] and [[reputation]] management.
{{cleanup-date|January 2006}}
 
  +
<!-- [[hypocrisy]] Johnathan Haidt -->
   
  +
Christopher Boehm (1982) has hypothesized that the incremental development of moral complexity throughout [[Great ape|hominid]] evolution was due to the increasing need to avoid disputes and injuries in moving to open savanna and developing stone weapons. Other theories are that increasing complexity was simply a correlate of increasing group size and brain size, and in particular the development of [[theory of mind]] abilities. [[Richard Dawkins]] in ''[[The God Delusion]]'' suggested that our morality is a result of our biological evolutionary history and that the [[Moral Zeitgeist]] helps describe how morality evolves from biological and cultural origins and evolves with time within a culture.
'''Moral codes''' are often complex statements of right and wrong. Although some people might think that the moral code is simple, rarely there is anything simple about one's morals or [[ethics]] or for that matter judgment of others' morals. The difficulty lies in the fact that morals are often part of a [[religion]] and more often than not about [[culture]] codes.
 
   
  +
== Neuroscientific and psychiatric perspectives ==
A common version of a moral code is a [[legal code]] which states the penalties or corrective actions associated with any particular act (note that many of the legal codes are built on a foundaton of religion).
 
  +
=== Mirror-neurons ===
   
  +
Research on ''[[mirror neuron]]s'', since their discovery in 1996<ref>Giacomo Rizzolatti et al. (1996). ''Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions'', Cognitive Brain Research 3 131-141</ref>, suggests that they may have a strong role to play in [[empathy]]. Social neuro-scientist [[Jean Decety]] thinks that the ability to recognize and vicariously experience what another creature is undergoing was a key step forward in the evolution of social behavior, and ultimately, morality.<ref>[http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/05/27/AR2007052701056_pf.html If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural - washingtonpost.com<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> The inability to feel empathy is one of the defining characteristic of [[psychopath]]y, and this would appear to lend support to Decety's view.<ref>{{cite journal |author=de Wied M, Goudena PP, Matthys W |title=Empathy in boys with disruptive behavior disorders |journal=Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines |volume=46 |issue=8 |pages=867–80 |year=2005 |pmid=16033635 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00389.x}}</ref> <ref>{{cite journal |author=Fernandez YM, Marshall WL |title=Victim empathy, social self-esteem, and psychopathy in rapists |journal=Sexual abuse : a journal of research and treatment |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=11–26 |year=2003 |pmid=12616926 |doi=10.1023/A:1020611606754}}</ref>
A [[value system]], which is the prioritization of the [[value#Personal_and_cultural_values|values]] held by an individual or group, can also be used to form the basis of a moral code.
 
   
  +
==Psychological perspectives==
In some cultures, the relationship between moral and legal codes are often absolute - they're one and the same. Moral codes help drive [[personal conduct]].
 
  +
{{see|Jean_Piaget#Education and development of morality}}
  +
{{see|Kohlberg's stages of moral development}}
  +
{{see|Ethics of care}}
   
  +
In modern psychology, morality is considered to change through personal development. A number of psychologists have produced theories on the development of morals, usually going through stages of different morals. [[Lawrence Kohlberg]], [[Jean Piaget]], and [[Elliot Turiel]] have cognitive-developmental approaches to moral development; to these theorists morality forms in a series of constructive stages or domains. [[Social psychology|Social psychologists]] such as [[Martin Hoffman]] and [[Jonathan Haidt]] emphasize social and emotional development based on biology, such as [[empathy]]. Moral identity theorists, such as [[William Damon]] and [[Mordechai Nisan]], see moral commitment as arising from the development of a self-identity that is defined by moral purposes: this moral self-identity leads to a sense of responsibility to pursue such purposes. Of historical interest in psychology are the theories of [[psychoanalyst]]s such as [[Sigmund Freud]], who believe that moral development is the product of aspects of the [[super-ego]] as guilt-shame avoidance.
Examples of moral codes include the [[Golden Rule|golden rule]]; [[Wiccan Rede]], the noble [[Eightfold Path|eightfold path]] of [[Buddhism]]; the [[Ten Commandments|ten commandments]] of [[Judaism]], [[Christianity]], and [[Islam]]; and the [[Ten Indian commandments|ten Indian commandments]].
 
   
  +
== Morality in judicial systems ==
A related and more advanced (some say more corrupt) concept is an [[ethical code]], which establishes tradeoffs and rationale for making decisions for the greater good. Some of these resemble a moral code, most are less strict and make no special claim to actually distinguish 'right' from 'wrong' in any absolute sense. The ethical code is concerned with weighing all the negative and positive results of an action, and making a decision based upon the greater good for a greater number. That [[adultery]] is wrong is debated.
 
   
  +
In most systems, the lack of morality of the individual can also be a sufficient cause for punishment{{Fact|date=July 2007}}, or can be an element for the grading of the punishment.
Another related concept is the [[moral core]] which is assumed to be innate in each individual, to those who accept that differences between individuals are more important than Creators or their rules. This, in some religious systems (e.g. [[Taoism]] and [[Gnosticism]]), is assumed to be the basis of all [[aesthetics]] and thus moral choice. Moral codes as such are therefore seen as coercive — part of human [[politics]].
 
   
  +
Especially in the systems where [[modesty]] (i.e., with reference to sexual crimes) is legally protected or otherwise regulated, the definition of morality as a legal element and in order to determine the cases of infringement, is usually left to the vision and appreciation of the single judge and hardly ever precisely specified. In such cases, it is common to verify an application of the prevalent common morality of the interested community, that consequently becomes enforced by the law for further reference.
== Moral Core ==
 
   
  +
The government of [[South Africa]] is attempting to create a Moral Regeneration movement. Part of this is a proposed [[Bill of Morals]], which will bring a [[Bible|biblical]]-based "moral code" into the realm of law. This move by a nominally secular democracy has attracted relatively little criticism.
{{cleanup-date|January 2006}}
 
   
  +
== Morality and politics ==
The '''moral core''' of an individual is the extent to which that person will apply his or her notions of morality. It is centered on the individual and can be extended to include other people or groups. The individual sees these others within the moral core as deserving to be treated in the same way the individual personally wants to be treated.
 
   
  +
If morality is the answer to the question 'how ought we to live' at the individual level, politics can be seen as addressing the same question at the social level. It is therefore unsurprising that evidence has been found of a relationship between attitudes in morality and politics. [[Jonathan Haidt]] and [[Jesse Graham]] have studied the differences between [[Liberalism|liberals]] and [[conservatives]], in this regard.<ref name="Haidt">[[Jonathan Haidt|Haidt, Jonathan]] and Graham, Jesse (2006). [http://faculty.virginia.edu/haidtlab/articles/haidt.graham.when-morality-opposes-justice.doc ''When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize''] (DOC) [[Social Justice Research]].</ref><ref>[http://www.newyorker.com/online/video/conference/2007/haidt Morality: 2012: Online Only Video: The New Yorker<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref><ref>[http://dangerousintersection.org/?p=1445 Why conservatives and liberals talk past each other on moral issues. | Dangerous Intersection<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> According to their model, political conservatives make their moral choices using five moral variables (harm/care, fairness/reciprocity, ingroup loyalty, authority/respect, purity/sanctity), whereas liberals use only two (harm/care and fairness/reciprocity). Haidt also hypothesizes that the origin of this division in the United States can be traced to geohistorical factors, with conservatism strongest in closely knit, ethnically homogenous communities, in contrast to [[port]]-cities, where the cultural mix is greater, thus requiring more liberalism.
The moral core is a principle that can determine how an individual applies particular moral values and beliefs. It is described in some theories of [[ethics]] as the limits to the [[rationality]] of ethics itself. From this perspective, morals are considered primarily [[aesthetic]] notions and not seen as directly sharable.
 
   
  +
Group morality develops from shared [[concept]]s and [[belief]]s and is often codified to regulate behavior within a [[culture]] or community. Various defined actions come to be called moral or immoral. Individuals who choose moral action are popularly held to possess "moral fiber", whereas those who indulge in immoral behavior may be labeled as socially [[degenerate]]. The continued existence of a group may depend on widespread conformity to codes of morality; an inability to adjust moral codes in response to new challenges is sometimes credited with the demise of a community (a positive example would be the function of [[Cistercian]] reform in reviving monasticism; a negative example would be the role of the [[Empress Dowager Cixi|Dowager Empress]] in the subjugation of China to European interests). Within [[Nationalism|nationalist]] movements, there has been some tendency to feel that a nation will not survive or prosper without acknowledging one common morality, regardless of in what it consists.
Persons who fall outside of an individual's moral core are not covered by that individual's notions of morality and do not enjoy its protections. Thus, the concept of a moral core can serve to explain apparent [[hypocrisy]] in people who claim to have particular ethical principles. For example, it might be used to explain why someone whose [[religion]] forbids [[murder]] can nevertheless support involvement in [[war]] or imposition of the [[death penalty]] for certain [[crime]]s. According to this theory, the people whose killing can be justified somehow fall outside the individual's moral core.
 
  +
Political Morality is also relevant to the behaviour internationally of national governments, and to the support they receive from their host population. Noam Chomsky states that <ref>[http://www.zmag.org/content/showarticle.cfm?ItemID=2064] Terror and Just Response, ZNet, 02 July 2002, Noam Chomsky</ref> <ref>[http://www.artsandopinion.com/2007_v6_n6/chomsky-4.htm] Arts and Opinion Vol. 6, No. 6, 2007 Gabriel Matthew Schivone interviews Noam Chomsky</font></ref>
   
  +
{{cquote|... if we adopt the principle of universality : if an action is right (or wrong) for others, it is right (or wrong) for us. Those who do not rise to the minimal moral level of applying to themselves the standards they apply to others -- more stringent ones, in fact -- plainly cannot be taken seriously when they speak of appropriateness of response; or of right and wrong, good and evil.}}
A moral core is presumed to be formed by experience, including especially parental [[moral example]]s, and the slow growth via cognition of a set of [[conditioning]]s, [[inhibition]]s, and concepts of [[beauty]] through his or her entire lifetime. Although it may be demonstrated to train or inspire others, it cannot be shared in any way, and is constantly changing.
 
   
  +
{{cquote|In fact, one of the, maybe the most, elementary of moral principles is that of universality, that is, If something's right for me, it's right for you; if it's wrong for you, it's wrong for me. Any moral code that is even worth looking at has that at its core somehow. But that principle is overwhelmingly disregarded all the time. If you want to run through examples we can easily do it. Take, say, George W. Bush, since he happens to be president. If you apply the standards that we applied to Nazi war criminals at Nuremberg, he'd be hanged. Is it an even conceivable possibility? It's not even discussable. Because we don't apply to ourselves the principles we apply to others. There's a lot of talk about 'terror' and how awful it is. Whose terror? Our terror against them? I mean, is that considered reprehensible? No, it's considered highly moral; it's considered self-defense. Now, their terror against us, that's awful, and terrible. But, to try to rise to the level of becoming a minimal moral agent, and just entering into the domain of moral discourse is very difficult. Because that means accepting the principle of universality. And you can experiment for yourself and see how often that's accepted, either in personal or political life. Very rarely.}}
Some theories of [[morality]], notably [[moral relativism]], but also branches of [[theology]], hold that there is little value in attempting to share moral cores or even to align moral choices except to the bare minimum needed to prevent conflict.
 
   
  +
== Moral codes ==
The opposite belief, imposing various degrees of standardization via a [[moral code]] and its enforcement, usually in a [[legal system]], is that such cores either can be shared or are irrelevant to the process of social control and learning proper conduct.
 
   
  +
Codified morality is generally distinguished from [[Norm (sociology)|custom]], another way for a community to define appropriate activity, by the former's derivation from [[Natural rights|natural]] or universal principles. In certain religious communities, the [[divinity|Divine]] is said to provide these principles through [[revelation]], sometimes in great detail. Such codes may be called laws, as in the [[Torah|Law of Moses]], or community morality may be defined through commentary on the texts of revelation, as in [[Sharia|Islamic law]]. Such codes are distinguished from legal or judicial [[right]], including [[civil rights]], which are based on the accumulated traditions, decrees and legislation of a political authority, though these latter often invoke the authority of the moral law.
==Footnotes==
 
   
  +
Morality can also be seen as the collection of beliefs as to what constitutes a good life. Since throughout most of [[human]] [[history]], [[religion]]s have provided both visions and regulations for an [[Ideal (ethics)|ideal]] life, morality is often confused with religious [[precept]]s. In secular communities, [[lifestyle]] choices, which represent an [[individual]]'s conception of the good life, are often discussed in terms of "morality." Individuals sometimes feel that making an appropriate lifestyle choice invokes a true morality, and that accepted codes of conduct within their chosen community are fundamentally moral, even when such codes deviate from more general social principles.
<references/>
 
   
  +
Moral codes are often complex definitions of right and wrong that are based upon well-defined [[value systems]]. Although some people might think that a moral code is simple, rarely is there anything simple about one's [[Value (personal and cultural)|values]], [[ethics]], etc. or, for that matter, the judgment of those of others. The difficulty lies in the fact that morals are often part of a [[religion]] and more often than not about [[culture]] codes. Sometimes, moral codes give way to [[legal code]]s, which couple penalties or corrective actions with particular practices. Note that while many legal codes are merely built on a foundation of religious and/or cultural moral codes, ofttimes they are one and the same.
== See also ==
 
   
  +
Examples of moral codes include the [[Ethic of reciprocity|Golden Rule]]; the [[Noble Eightfold Path]] of [[Buddhism]]; the ancient Egyptian code of [[Ma'at]] ;the [[Ten Commandments|ten commandments]] of [[Judaism]], [[Christianity]], and [[Islam]]; the [[yamas]] and [[niyama]] of the Hindu scriptures; the [[ten Indian commandments]]; and the principle of the [[Dessek]].
* [[Ethics]]
 
  +
  +
Another related concept is the [[Morality|moral core]] which is assumed to be innate in each individual, to those who accept that differences between individuals are more important than posited Creators or their rules. This, in some religious systems and beliefs (e.g. [[Taoism]], [[Moralism]] and [[Gnosticism]]), is assumed to be the basis of all [[aesthetics]] and thus moral choice. Moral codes as such are therefore seen as coercive — part of human [[politics]].
  +
  +
== Moral psychology ==
  +
=== Religiosity and morality ===
  +
{{see also|Morality without religion}}
  +
In the scientific literature, the degree of religiosity is generally found to be associated with higher ethical attitudes.<ref>As is expressed in the review of literature on this topic by: {{cite journal |author=Conroy, S.J. and Emerson, T.L.N. |title=[http://www.springerlink.com/content/r30712pn2q513456 Business Ethics and Religion: Religiosity as a Predictor of Ethical Awareness Among Students] | journal=Journal of Business Ethics |year=2004 | volume=50 | number=4 |pages=383--396 | doi = 10.1023/B:BUSI.0000025040.41263.09 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}} DOI:10.1023/B:BUSI.0000025040.41263.09</ref> Although a recent study by [[Gregory S. Paul]] published in the ''Journal of Religion and Society'' argues for a positive [[correlation]] between the degree of ''public religiosity'' in a society and certain measures of dysfunction.<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/2005/2005-11.html| journal=Journal of Religion and Society|title=Cross-National Correlations of Quantifiable Societal Health with Popular Religiosity and Secularism in the Prosperous Democracies: A First Look|first=Gregory S.|last=Paul |location=Baltimore, Maryland|year=2005| volume=7}}</ref> An analysis published later in the same journal contends that a number of methodological problems undermine any findings or conclusions to be taken from the research.<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/2006/2006-1.html| journal=Journal of Religion and Society|title=Religiosity, Secularism, and Social Health|author=Gerson Moreno-Riaño| coauthors=Mark Caleb Smith, Thomas Mach|location=Cedarville University|year=2006| volume=8}}</ref> In another response, Gary Jensen builds on and refines Paul's study.<ref name="Jensen">Gary F. Jensen (2006) Department of Sociology, Vanderbilt University ''Religious Cosmologies and Homicide Rates among Nations: A Closer Look'' http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/2006/2006-7.html http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/pdf/2006-7.pdf Journal of Religion and Society, Volume 8, ISSN 1522-5658 http://purl.org/JRS </ref> His conclusion, after carrying out elaborate multivariate statistical studies, is that a complex relationship exists between religiosity and homicide with some dimensions of religiosity encouraging homicide and other dimensions discouraging it." Meanwhile, other studies seem to show positive links in the relationship between [[morality and religion|religiosity and moral behavior]]<ref>KERLEY, KENT R., MATTHEWS, TODD L. & BLANCHARD, TROY C. (2005) Religiosity, Religious Participation, and Negative Prison Behaviors. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion '''44''' (4), 443-457. {{doi|10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00296.x}}</ref> <ref>SAROGLOU, VASSILIS, PICHON, ISABELLE, TROMPETTE, LAURENCE, VERSCHUEREN, MARIJKE & DERNELLE, REBECCA (2005) Prosocial Behavior and Religion: New Evidence Based on Projective Measures and Peer Ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion '''44''' (3), 323-348. {{doi|10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00289.x}}</ref> <ref>Regnerus, Mark D. & Burdette, Amy (2006)
  +
RELIGIOUS CHANGE AND ADOLESCENT FAMILY DYNAMICS. The Sociological Quarterly '''47''' (1), 175-194. {{doi|10.1111/j.1533-8525.2006.00042.x}}</ref> — for example, surveys suggesting a positive connection between faith and altruism.<ref>eg [http://www.ropercenter.uconn.edu/data_access/data/datasets/social_capital_community_survey.html a survey] by [[Robert D. Putnam|Robert Putnam]] showing that membership of religious groups was positively correlated with membership of voluntary organisations</ref> Modern research in [[Relationship between criminology and sociology of education|criminology]] also acknowledges an ''inverse'' relationship between religion and crime,<ref>As is stated in: Doris C. Chu (2007). Religiosity and Desistance From Drug Use. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 2007; 34; 661 originally published online Mar 7, 2007; DOI: 10.1177/0093854806293485</ref> with many studies establishing this beneficial connection (though some claim it is a modest one).<ref>
  +
For example:
  +
  +
* Albrecht, S. I., Chadwick, B. A., & Alcorn, D. S. (1977). Religiosity and deviance:Application of an attitude-behavior contingent consistency model. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 16, 263-274.
  +
* Burkett, S.,& White, M. (1974). Hellfire and delinquency:Another look. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,13,455-462.
  +
* Chard-Wierschem, D. (1998). In pursuit of the “true” relationship: A longitudinal study of the effects of religiosity on delinquency and substance abuse. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Dissertation.
  +
* Cochran, J. K.,& Akers, R. L. (1989). Beyond hellfire:An explanation of the variable effects of religiosity on adolescent marijuana and alcohol use. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 26, 198-225.
  +
* Evans, T. D.,Cullen, F. T.,Burton, V. S.,Jr.,Dunaway, R. G.,Payne, G. L.,& Kethineni, S. R. (1996). Religion, social bonds, and delinquency. Deviant Behavior, 17, 43-70.
  +
* Grasmick, H. G., Bursik, R. J., & Cochran, J. K. (1991). “Render unto Caesar what is Caesar’s”: Religiosity and taxpayer’s inclinations to cheat. The Sociological Quarterly, 32, 251-266.
  +
* Higgins, P. C., & Albrecht, G. L. (1977). Hellfire and delinquency revisited. Social Forces, 55, 952-958.
  +
* Johnson, B. R.,Larson, D. B.,DeLi,S.,& Jang, S. J. (2000). Escaping from the crime of inner cities:Church attendance and religious salience among disadvantaged youth. Justice Quarterly, 17, 377-391.
  +
* Johnson, R. E., Marcos, A. C., & Bahr, S. J. (1987). The role of peers in the complex etiology of adolescent drug use. Criminology, 25, 323-340.
  +
* Powell, K. (1997). Correlates of violent and nonviolent behavior among vulnerable inner-city youths. Family and Community Health, 20, 38-47.
  +
  +
</ref> Indeed, a meta-analysis of 60 studies on religion and crime concluded, “religious behaviors and beliefs exert a moderate deterrent effect on individuals’ criminal behavior”.<ref>Baier, C. J.,& Wright, B. R. (2001). “If you love me, keep my commandments”:A meta-analysis of the effect of religion on crime. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency,38,3-21.</ref>
  +
  +
== See also ==
  +
* [[Amorality]]
 
* [[Applied ethics]]
 
* [[Applied ethics]]
* [[Guilt]]
 
 
* [[Conscience]]
 
* [[Conscience]]
  +
* [[Consequentialism]]
  +
* [[Deontological ethics]]
  +
* [[Ethics]]
  +
* [[Ethos]]
  +
* [[Evolution of morality]]
  +
* [[Guilt]]
  +
* [[Integrity]]
  +
* [[Justice]]
 
* [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development]]
 
* [[Kohlberg's stages of moral development]]
  +
* [[Moral absolutism]]
  +
* [[Moral agency]]
  +
* [[Moral character]]
  +
* [[Moral development]]
  +
* [[Moral philosophy]]
  +
* [[Moral psychology]]
  +
* [[Moral reasoning]]
  +
* [[Moral realism]]
  +
* [[Moral relativism]]
  +
* [[Moral therapy]]
  +
* [[Moral treatment]]
  +
* [[Moral universalism]]
  +
* [[Mores]]
  +
* [[Neuroethics]]
  +
* [[Norm (philosophy)|Norms]]
  +
* [[Normlessness]]
  +
* [[Personal values]]
 
* [[Public morality]]
 
* [[Public morality]]
  +
* [[Religious beliefs]]
* [[The ends justify the means]]
 
* [[Religion]]
+
* [[Reputation]]
  +
* [[Science of morality]]
  +
* [[Secular ethics]]
  +
* [[Shame]]
  +
* [[Sin]]
  +
* [[Situational ethics]]
  +
* [[Social cognitive theory of morality]]
  +
* [[Social influences]]
  +
* [[Social intuitionism]]
  +
* [[Social values]]
  +
* [[Value systems]]
  +
* [[Virtue ethics]]
   
==Further reading==
+
== Footnotes ==
   
  +
<div class="references-small"><!-- See http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wikipedia:Footnotes for an explanation of how to generate footnotes using the <ref(erences/)> tags -->
*Christopher Boehm (1982)
 
   
  +
{{reflist}}
* Duntley, J.D., & Buss, D.M. (2004). The evolution of evil. In A. Miller (Ed.), ''The social psychology of good and evil''. New York: Guilford. 102-123. [http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/Group/BussLAB/pdffiles/The%20evolution%20of%20evil.pdf Full text]
 
   
  +
== Bibliography ==
* Krebs, D. L. (2005). The evolution of morality. In D. Buss (Ed.) ''The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology'', (pp. 747-771). John Wiley & Sons. [http://www.sfu.ca/psyc/faculty/krebs/publications/The%20Evolution%20of%20Morality.pdf Full text]
 
   
  +
* Walker, Martin G. ''LIFE! Why We Exist...And What We Must Do to Survive'' ([http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/LIFE_Why_We_Exist...] Wiki Book Page) ([http://www.meaninginmylife.com] Web Site), Dog Ear Publishing, 2006, ISBN 1-59858-243-7
* Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O’Brien, E. (in press). Audience effects on moralistic punishment. ''[[Evolution and Human Behavior]]''. [http://www.psych.upenn.edu/PLEEP/pdfs/in%20press%20Kurzban%20DeScioli%20Obrien.pdf Full text]
 
  +
* Trompenaars, Fons. ''Did the Pedestrian Die?'' ISBN 1-84112-436-2
   
  +
== External links ==
* Richerson, P.J. & Boyd, R. (2004). Darwinian Evolutionary Ethics: Between Patriotism and Sympathy. In P. Clayton & J. Schloss (Eds.), ''Evolution and Ethics: Human Morality in Biological and Religious Perspective''. Wm. B. Eerdmans Publishing, (pp 50-77) [http://www.des.ucdavis.edu/faculty/richerson/Dethics.pdf Full text]
 
   
  +
* [http://www.in-mind.org/artikelen/fairness-judgments-genuine-morality-or-disguised-egocentrism.html Fairness Judgments: Genuine Morality or Disguised Egoism?] Psychological Article on Fairness (registration required)
* Trompenaars, Fons. ''Did the Pedestrian Die?'' ISBN 1841124362
 
  +
* [http://selfhelpinspiration.com/article/morals_in_society.html Morals and Conscience in Society]
 
* Wilkinson, 1984
 
 
==External links==
 
* [http://www.frostcloud.com/forum/forumdisplay.php?f=11 FrostCloud.com: Ethics and Morality] Discuss issues on ethics, morality, and justice.
 
 
* [http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/morality-definition/ The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy on the Definition of Morality]
 
* [http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/morality-definition/ The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy on the Definition of Morality]
  +
* [http://www.percepp.com/morality.htm Objective Morality An evolutionary approach]
 
* [http://www.examinethetruth.com/page_morale_01.htm Christian and Muslim debates on Morality]
 
* [http://www.examinethetruth.com/page_morale_01.htm Christian and Muslim debates on Morality]
 
* [http://www.chabad.org/search/keyword.asp?kid=1222 Morality and Judaism] chabad.org
 
* [http://www.chabad.org/search/keyword.asp?kid=1222 Morality and Judaism] chabad.org
* [http://www.sfu.ca/psyc/faculty/krebs/publications/publications.htm List of publications on evolution and morality by Dennis Krebs]
 
 
* [http://www.geeta-kavita.com/article.asp?article=biology_morals_dharma An article that focuses on the Biological basis of morality is available at Geeta-kavita.com]
 
* [http://www.geeta-kavita.com/article.asp?article=biology_morals_dharma An article that focuses on the Biological basis of morality is available at Geeta-kavita.com]
 
* [http://www.worldmoralmovement.org Wiki site for discussing and taking action on shared morals (WorldMoralMovement.org)]
 
* [http://www.worldmoralmovement.org Wiki site for discussing and taking action on shared morals (WorldMoralMovement.org)]
  +
* [http://moralsandethics.wordpress.com/ Morals and Ethics in Islam]
  +
* [http://www.examinethetruth.com/moralitydebate.htm Understanding the Islam, Christianity Debate]
   
 
{{Philosophy (navigation)}}
 
{{Philosophy (navigation)}}
  +
   
 
[[Category:Ethics]]
 
[[Category:Ethics]]
[[Category:Evolution]]
+
[[Category:Morality|Morality]]
[[Category:Evolution and human behavior]]
+
[[Category:Social influences]]
[[Category:Human evolution]]
+
[[Category:Virtues]]
  +
[[Category:Values]]
   
  +
<!--
  +
[[cs:Morálka]]
  +
[[da:Moral]]
  +
[[de:Moral]]
  +
[[et:Moraal]]
  +
[[es:Moral]]
  +
[[eo:Moralo]]
  +
[[fr:Morale]]
  +
[[id:Moral]]
  +
[[ia:Moral]]
  +
[[is:Siðfræði]]
  +
[[it:Morale]]
  +
[[he:מוסר]]
  +
[[lv:Morāle]]
  +
[[lt:Moralės norma]]
  +
[[mk:морал]]
  +
[[nl:Moraal]]
  +
[[ja:道徳]]
  +
[[no:Moral]]
  +
[[pl:Moralność]]
  +
[[pt:Moral]]
  +
[[ru:Мораль]]
  +
[[sq:Morali]]
  +
[[sr:Моралност]]
  +
[[fi:Moraali]]
  +
[[sv:Moral]]
  +
[[th:คุณธรรม]]
  +
[[yi:מוסר]]
  +
[[zh:道德]]
  +
-->
 
{{enWP|Morality}}
 
{{enWP|Morality}}

Latest revision as of 08:09, 20 July 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Philosophy Index: Aesthetics · Epistemology · Ethics · Logic · Metaphysics · Consciousness · Philosophy of Language · Philosophy of Mind · Philosophy of Science · Social and Political philosophy · Philosophies · Philosophers · List of lists


This article needs rewriting to enhance its relevance to psychologists..
Please help to improve this page yourself if you can..


See also Ethics and moral psychology.


Morality (from the Latin moralitas "manner, character, proper behavior") has three principal meanings.

In its first, descriptive usage, morality means a code of conduct which is held to be authoritative in matters of right and wrong. Morals are created by and define society, philosophy, religion, or individual conscience. An example of the descriptive usage could be "common conceptions of morality have changed significantly over time."

In its second, normative and universal sense, morality refers to an ideal code of conduct, one which would be espoused in preference to alternatives by all rational people, under specified conditions. In this "prescriptive" sense of morality as opposed to the above described "descriptive" sense, moral value judgments such as "murder is immoral" are made. To deny 'morality' in this sense is a position known as moral skepticism, in which the existence of objective moral "truths" is rejected.[1]

In its third usage, 'morality' is synonymous with ethics, the systematic philosophical study of the moral domain.[2]

Ethics seeks to address questions such as how a moral outcome can be achieved in a specific situation (applied ethics), how moral values should be determined (normative ethics), what morals people actually abide by (descriptive ethics), what the fundamental nature of ethics or morality is, including whether it has any objective justification (meta-ethics), and how moral capacity or moral agency develops and what its nature is (moral psychology).[3] In applied ethics, for example, the prohibition against taking human life is controversial with respect to capital punishment, abortion and wars of invasion. In normative ethics, a typical question might be whether a lie told for the sake of protecting someone from harm is justified. In meta-ethics, a key issue is the meaning of the terms "right" or "wrong". Moral realism would hold that there are true moral statements which report objective moral facts, whereas moral anti-realism would hold that morality is derived from any one of the norms prevalent in society (cultural relativism); the edicts of a god (divine command theory); is merely an expression of the speakers' sentiments (emotivism); an implied imperative (prescriptive); falsely presupposes that there are objective moral facts (error theory). Some thinkers hold that there is no correct definition of right behavior, that morality can only be judged with respect to particular situations, within the standards of particular belief systems and socio-historical contexts. This position, known as moral relativism, often cites empirical evidence from anthropology as evidence to support its claims.[4] The opposite view, that there are universal, eternal moral truths are known as moral absolutism. Moral absolutists might concede that forces of social conformity significantly shape moral decisions, but deny that cultural norms and customs define morally right behavior.

Philosophical Perspectives

Clarifying the Usage of the Term “Morality”

The fact that there are at least three different usages of the term “morality” (see above) has led to much confusion when that word is used in discussions. Because of that confusion, many thinkers are forced to spend a certain amount of time dealing with that confusion before they even begin to use the term “morality” in their discussions.

One example of that helpful clarification process is found in Walter Terence Stace’s book, The Concept of Morals, in which he clarifies his own usage of the terms "ethics" and "morality," and their differences.[5]

  • Moral realism or moral objectivism holds that moral codes exist outside of human opinion -- that certain things are right or wrong regardless of human opinion on the topic. Objective morality may be seen as stemming from the inherent nature of humanity, divine command, or both.
  • Moral subjectivism holds that moral codes depend on human opinion.
  • Moral relativism holds that moral codes are a function of human values and social structures, and hold no meaning outside social convention.
  • Moral absolutism is the view that certain acts are right or wrong regardless of context.
  • Moral universalism compromises between moral relativism and moral absolutism and holds that there is, or should be, a common universal core of morality.
  • Moral nihilism is the view that no morality exists.
  • Amoralism is the view that the concepts of moral right and wrong do not have meaning.

There are many other examples.

Anthropological Perspectives


Tribal and territorial moralities

Celia Green has made a distinction between tribal and territorial morality.[6] She characterizes the latter as predominantly negative and proscriptive: it defines a person’s territory, including his or her property and dependents, which is not to be damaged or interfered with. Apart from these proscriptions, territorial morality is permissive, allowing the individual whatever behaviour does not interfere with the territory of another. By contrast, tribal morality is prescriptive, imposing the norms of the collective on the individual. These norms will be arbitrary, culturally dependent and ‘flexible’, whereas territorial morality aims at rules which are universal and absolute, such as Kant’s ‘categorical imperative’. Green relates the development of territorial morality to the rise of the concept of private property, and the ascendancy of contract over status.

In-group and out-group


Some observers hold that individuals have distinct sets of moral rules that they apply to different groups of people. There is the "ingroup," which includes the individual and those they believe to be of the same culture or race, and there is the "outgroup," whose members are not entitled to be treated according to the same rules. Some biologists, anthropologists and evolutionary psychologists believe this ingroup/outgroup difference is an evolutionary mechanism, one which evolved due to its enhanced survival aspects. Gary R. Johnson and V.S. Falger have argued that nationalism and patriotism are forms of this ingroup/outgroup boundary.

Comparing cultures

Peterson and Seligman [7] approach the anthropological view looking across cultures and across millennia. The conclude that certain virtues have prevailed in all cultures they examined. The major virtues they identified include wisdom / knowledge; courage; humanity; justice; temperance; and transcendence. Each of these includes several divisions. For instance humanity includes love, kindness, and social intelligence.

Fons Trompenaars, author of Did the Pedestrian Die?, tested members of different cultures with various moral dilemmas. One of these was whether the driver of a car would have his friend, a passenger riding in the car, lie in order to protect the driver from the consequences of driving too fast and hitting a pedestrian. Trompenaars found that different cultures had quite different expectations (from none to almost certain).

Evolutionary perspectives

Further:Altruism

Some evolutionary biologists, particularly sociobiologists, believe that morality is a product of evolutionary forces acting at an individual level and also at the group level through group selection (though whether "group selection" actually occurs is a controversial topic in evolutionary theory). Some sociobiologists contend that the set of behaviors that constitute morality evolved largely because they provided possible survival and/or reproductive benefits (i.e. increased evolutionary success). Humans consequently evolved "pro-social" emotions, such as feelings of empathy or guilt, in response to these moral behaviors.

In this respect, morality is not absolute, but relative and constitutes any set of behaviors that encourage human cooperation based on their ideology. Biologists contend that all social animals, from ants to elephants, have modified their behaviors, by restraining selfishness in order to make group living worthwhile. Human morality, though sophisticated and complex relative to other animals, is essentially a natural phenomenon that evolved to restrict excessive individualism and foster human cooperation. [8]

On this view, moral codes are ultimately founded on emotional instincts and intuitions that were selected for in the past because they aided survival and reproduction (inclusive fitness). The strength of the maternal bond is one example. Another is the Westermarck effect, seen as underpinning taboos against incest, which decreases the likelihood of inbreeding depression.

The phenomenon of 'reciprocity' in nature is seen by evolutionary biologists as one way to begin to understand human morality. Its function is typically to ensure a reliable supply of essential resources, especially for animals living in a habitat where food quantity or quality fluctuates unpredictably. For example, on any given night for vampire bats, some individuals fail to feed on prey while others consume a surplus of blood. Bats that have successfully fed then regurgitate part of their blood meal to save a conspecific from starvation. Since these animals live in close-knit groups over many years, an individual can count on other group members to return the favor on nights when it goes hungry (Wilkinson, 1984)

It has been convincingly demonstrated that chimpanzees show empathy for each other in a wide variety of contexts.[9] They also possess the ability to engage in deception, and a level of social 'politics'[10] prototypical of our own tendencies for gossip and reputation management.

Christopher Boehm (1982) has hypothesized that the incremental development of moral complexity throughout hominid evolution was due to the increasing need to avoid disputes and injuries in moving to open savanna and developing stone weapons. Other theories are that increasing complexity was simply a correlate of increasing group size and brain size, and in particular the development of theory of mind abilities. Richard Dawkins in The God Delusion suggested that our morality is a result of our biological evolutionary history and that the Moral Zeitgeist helps describe how morality evolves from biological and cultural origins and evolves with time within a culture.

Neuroscientific and psychiatric perspectives

Mirror-neurons

Research on mirror neurons, since their discovery in 1996[11], suggests that they may have a strong role to play in empathy. Social neuro-scientist Jean Decety thinks that the ability to recognize and vicariously experience what another creature is undergoing was a key step forward in the evolution of social behavior, and ultimately, morality.[12] The inability to feel empathy is one of the defining characteristic of psychopathy, and this would appear to lend support to Decety's view.[13] [14]

Psychological perspectives

Further information: Jean_Piaget#Education and development of morality
Further information: Kohlberg's stages of moral development
Further information: Ethics of care

In modern psychology, morality is considered to change through personal development. A number of psychologists have produced theories on the development of morals, usually going through stages of different morals. Lawrence Kohlberg, Jean Piaget, and Elliot Turiel have cognitive-developmental approaches to moral development; to these theorists morality forms in a series of constructive stages or domains. Social psychologists such as Martin Hoffman and Jonathan Haidt emphasize social and emotional development based on biology, such as empathy. Moral identity theorists, such as William Damon and Mordechai Nisan, see moral commitment as arising from the development of a self-identity that is defined by moral purposes: this moral self-identity leads to a sense of responsibility to pursue such purposes. Of historical interest in psychology are the theories of psychoanalysts such as Sigmund Freud, who believe that moral development is the product of aspects of the super-ego as guilt-shame avoidance.

Morality in judicial systems

In most systems, the lack of morality of the individual can also be a sufficient cause for punishment[How to reference and link to summary or text], or can be an element for the grading of the punishment.

Especially in the systems where modesty (i.e., with reference to sexual crimes) is legally protected or otherwise regulated, the definition of morality as a legal element and in order to determine the cases of infringement, is usually left to the vision and appreciation of the single judge and hardly ever precisely specified. In such cases, it is common to verify an application of the prevalent common morality of the interested community, that consequently becomes enforced by the law for further reference.

The government of South Africa is attempting to create a Moral Regeneration movement. Part of this is a proposed Bill of Morals, which will bring a biblical-based "moral code" into the realm of law. This move by a nominally secular democracy has attracted relatively little criticism.

Morality and politics

If morality is the answer to the question 'how ought we to live' at the individual level, politics can be seen as addressing the same question at the social level. It is therefore unsurprising that evidence has been found of a relationship between attitudes in morality and politics. Jonathan Haidt and Jesse Graham have studied the differences between liberals and conservatives, in this regard.[15][16][17] According to their model, political conservatives make their moral choices using five moral variables (harm/care, fairness/reciprocity, ingroup loyalty, authority/respect, purity/sanctity), whereas liberals use only two (harm/care and fairness/reciprocity). Haidt also hypothesizes that the origin of this division in the United States can be traced to geohistorical factors, with conservatism strongest in closely knit, ethnically homogenous communities, in contrast to port-cities, where the cultural mix is greater, thus requiring more liberalism.

Group morality develops from shared concepts and beliefs and is often codified to regulate behavior within a culture or community. Various defined actions come to be called moral or immoral. Individuals who choose moral action are popularly held to possess "moral fiber", whereas those who indulge in immoral behavior may be labeled as socially degenerate. The continued existence of a group may depend on widespread conformity to codes of morality; an inability to adjust moral codes in response to new challenges is sometimes credited with the demise of a community (a positive example would be the function of Cistercian reform in reviving monasticism; a negative example would be the role of the Dowager Empress in the subjugation of China to European interests). Within nationalist movements, there has been some tendency to feel that a nation will not survive or prosper without acknowledging one common morality, regardless of in what it consists. Political Morality is also relevant to the behaviour internationally of national governments, and to the support they receive from their host population. Noam Chomsky states that [18] [19]

... if we adopt the principle of universality : if an action is right (or wrong) for others, it is right (or wrong) for us. Those who do not rise to the minimal moral level of applying to themselves the standards they apply to others -- more stringent ones, in fact -- plainly cannot be taken seriously when they speak of appropriateness of response; or of right and wrong, good and evil.
In fact, one of the, maybe the most, elementary of moral principles is that of universality, that is, If something's right for me, it's right for you; if it's wrong for you, it's wrong for me. Any moral code that is even worth looking at has that at its core somehow. But that principle is overwhelmingly disregarded all the time. If you want to run through examples we can easily do it. Take, say, George W. Bush, since he happens to be president. If you apply the standards that we applied to Nazi war criminals at Nuremberg, he'd be hanged. Is it an even conceivable possibility? It's not even discussable. Because we don't apply to ourselves the principles we apply to others. There's a lot of talk about 'terror' and how awful it is. Whose terror? Our terror against them? I mean, is that considered reprehensible? No, it's considered highly moral; it's considered self-defense. Now, their terror against us, that's awful, and terrible. But, to try to rise to the level of becoming a minimal moral agent, and just entering into the domain of moral discourse is very difficult. Because that means accepting the principle of universality. And you can experiment for yourself and see how often that's accepted, either in personal or political life. Very rarely.

Moral codes

Codified morality is generally distinguished from custom, another way for a community to define appropriate activity, by the former's derivation from natural or universal principles. In certain religious communities, the Divine is said to provide these principles through revelation, sometimes in great detail. Such codes may be called laws, as in the Law of Moses, or community morality may be defined through commentary on the texts of revelation, as in Islamic law. Such codes are distinguished from legal or judicial right, including civil rights, which are based on the accumulated traditions, decrees and legislation of a political authority, though these latter often invoke the authority of the moral law.

Morality can also be seen as the collection of beliefs as to what constitutes a good life. Since throughout most of human history, religions have provided both visions and regulations for an ideal life, morality is often confused with religious precepts. In secular communities, lifestyle choices, which represent an individual's conception of the good life, are often discussed in terms of "morality." Individuals sometimes feel that making an appropriate lifestyle choice invokes a true morality, and that accepted codes of conduct within their chosen community are fundamentally moral, even when such codes deviate from more general social principles.

Moral codes are often complex definitions of right and wrong that are based upon well-defined value systems. Although some people might think that a moral code is simple, rarely is there anything simple about one's values, ethics, etc. or, for that matter, the judgment of those of others. The difficulty lies in the fact that morals are often part of a religion and more often than not about culture codes. Sometimes, moral codes give way to legal codes, which couple penalties or corrective actions with particular practices. Note that while many legal codes are merely built on a foundation of religious and/or cultural moral codes, ofttimes they are one and the same.

Examples of moral codes include the Golden Rule; the Noble Eightfold Path of Buddhism; the ancient Egyptian code of Ma'at ;the ten commandments of Judaism, Christianity, and Islam; the yamas and niyama of the Hindu scriptures; the ten Indian commandments; and the principle of the Dessek.

Another related concept is the moral core which is assumed to be innate in each individual, to those who accept that differences between individuals are more important than posited Creators or their rules. This, in some religious systems and beliefs (e.g. Taoism, Moralism and Gnosticism), is assumed to be the basis of all aesthetics and thus moral choice. Moral codes as such are therefore seen as coercive — part of human politics.

Moral psychology

Religiosity and morality

See also: Morality without religion

In the scientific literature, the degree of religiosity is generally found to be associated with higher ethical attitudes.[20] Although a recent study by Gregory S. Paul published in the Journal of Religion and Society argues for a positive correlation between the degree of public religiosity in a society and certain measures of dysfunction.[21] An analysis published later in the same journal contends that a number of methodological problems undermine any findings or conclusions to be taken from the research.[22] In another response, Gary Jensen builds on and refines Paul's study.[23] His conclusion, after carrying out elaborate multivariate statistical studies, is that a complex relationship exists between religiosity and homicide with some dimensions of religiosity encouraging homicide and other dimensions discouraging it." Meanwhile, other studies seem to show positive links in the relationship between religiosity and moral behavior[24] [25] [26] — for example, surveys suggesting a positive connection between faith and altruism.[27] Modern research in criminology also acknowledges an inverse relationship between religion and crime,[28] with many studies establishing this beneficial connection (though some claim it is a modest one).[29] Indeed, a meta-analysis of 60 studies on religion and crime concluded, “religious behaviors and beliefs exert a moderate deterrent effect on individuals’ criminal behavior”.[30]

See also

Footnotes

  1. The Definition of Morality (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy)
  2. Ethics vs morality - the distinction between ethics and morals
  3. Ethics [Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy]
  4. Moral Relativism (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy)
  5. Stace, Walter T. (1937, Reprinted 1975 by permission of MacMillan Publishing Co. Inc.). The Concept of Morals, New York: The MacMillan Company.
  6. Green, Celia (2004). Letters from Exile: Observations on a Culture in Decline. Oxford: Oxford Forum. Chapters I-XX.
  7. Peterson, Christopher, and Martin E. P. Seligman. Character Strengths and Virtues. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2004.
  8. Shermer, Michael. "Transcendent Morality" The Science of Good and Evil.
  9. O’Connell, Sanjida (July 1995). Empathy in chimpanzees: Evidence for theory of mind?. primates 36 (3): 397–410. 0032-8332.
  10. Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals.
  11. Giacomo Rizzolatti et al. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions, Cognitive Brain Research 3 131-141
  12. If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural - washingtonpost.com
  13. de Wied M, Goudena PP, Matthys W (2005). Empathy in boys with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 46 (8): 867–80.
  14. Fernandez YM, Marshall WL (2003). Victim empathy, social self-esteem, and psychopathy in rapists. Sexual abuse : a journal of research and treatment 15 (1): 11–26.
  15. Haidt, Jonathan and Graham, Jesse (2006). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize (DOC) Social Justice Research.
  16. Morality: 2012: Online Only Video: The New Yorker
  17. Why conservatives and liberals talk past each other on moral issues. | Dangerous Intersection
  18. [1] Terror and Just Response, ZNet, 02 July 2002, Noam Chomsky
  19. [2] Arts and Opinion Vol. 6, No. 6, 2007 Gabriel Matthew Schivone interviews Noam Chomsky
  20. As is expressed in the review of literature on this topic by: Conroy, S.J. and Emerson, T.L.N. (2004). Business Ethics and Religion: Religiosity as a Predictor of Ethical Awareness Among Students. Journal of Business Ethics 50: 383--396. DOI:10.1023/B:BUSI.0000025040.41263.09
  21. Paul, Gregory S. (2005). Cross-National Correlations of Quantifiable Societal Health with Popular Religiosity and Secularism in the Prosperous Democracies: A First Look. Journal of Religion and Society 7.
  22. Gerson Moreno-Riaño, Mark Caleb Smith, Thomas Mach (2006). Religiosity, Secularism, and Social Health. Journal of Religion and Society 8.
  23. Gary F. Jensen (2006) Department of Sociology, Vanderbilt University Religious Cosmologies and Homicide Rates among Nations: A Closer Look http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/2006/2006-7.html http://moses.creighton.edu/JRS/pdf/2006-7.pdf Journal of Religion and Society, Volume 8, ISSN 1522-5658 http://purl.org/JRS
  24. KERLEY, KENT R., MATTHEWS, TODD L. & BLANCHARD, TROY C. (2005) Religiosity, Religious Participation, and Negative Prison Behaviors. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 44 (4), 443-457. DOI:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00296.x
  25. SAROGLOU, VASSILIS, PICHON, ISABELLE, TROMPETTE, LAURENCE, VERSCHUEREN, MARIJKE & DERNELLE, REBECCA (2005) Prosocial Behavior and Religion: New Evidence Based on Projective Measures and Peer Ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 44 (3), 323-348. DOI:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2005.00289.x
  26. Regnerus, Mark D. & Burdette, Amy (2006) RELIGIOUS CHANGE AND ADOLESCENT FAMILY DYNAMICS. The Sociological Quarterly 47 (1), 175-194. DOI:10.1111/j.1533-8525.2006.00042.x
  27. eg a survey by Robert Putnam showing that membership of religious groups was positively correlated with membership of voluntary organisations
  28. As is stated in: Doris C. Chu (2007). Religiosity and Desistance From Drug Use. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 2007; 34; 661 originally published online Mar 7, 2007; DOI: 10.1177/0093854806293485
  29. For example:
    • Albrecht, S. I., Chadwick, B. A., & Alcorn, D. S. (1977). Religiosity and deviance:Application of an attitude-behavior contingent consistency model. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 16, 263-274.
    • Burkett, S.,& White, M. (1974). Hellfire and delinquency:Another look. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,13,455-462.
    • Chard-Wierschem, D. (1998). In pursuit of the “true” relationship: A longitudinal study of the effects of religiosity on delinquency and substance abuse. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Dissertation.
    • Cochran, J. K.,& Akers, R. L. (1989). Beyond hellfire:An explanation of the variable effects of religiosity on adolescent marijuana and alcohol use. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 26, 198-225.
    • Evans, T. D.,Cullen, F. T.,Burton, V. S.,Jr.,Dunaway, R. G.,Payne, G. L.,& Kethineni, S. R. (1996). Religion, social bonds, and delinquency. Deviant Behavior, 17, 43-70.
    • Grasmick, H. G., Bursik, R. J., & Cochran, J. K. (1991). “Render unto Caesar what is Caesar’s”: Religiosity and taxpayer’s inclinations to cheat. The Sociological Quarterly, 32, 251-266.
    • Higgins, P. C., & Albrecht, G. L. (1977). Hellfire and delinquency revisited. Social Forces, 55, 952-958.
    • Johnson, B. R.,Larson, D. B.,DeLi,S.,& Jang, S. J. (2000). Escaping from the crime of inner cities:Church attendance and religious salience among disadvantaged youth. Justice Quarterly, 17, 377-391.
    • Johnson, R. E., Marcos, A. C., & Bahr, S. J. (1987). The role of peers in the complex etiology of adolescent drug use. Criminology, 25, 323-340.
    • Powell, K. (1997). Correlates of violent and nonviolent behavior among vulnerable inner-city youths. Family and Community Health, 20, 38-47.
  30. Baier, C. J.,& Wright, B. R. (2001). “If you love me, keep my commandments”:A meta-analysis of the effect of religion on crime. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency,38,3-21.

Bibliography

  • Walker, Martin G. LIFE! Why We Exist...And What We Must Do to Survive ([3] Wiki Book Page) ([4] Web Site), Dog Ear Publishing, 2006, ISBN 1-59858-243-7
  • Trompenaars, Fons. Did the Pedestrian Die? ISBN 1-84112-436-2

External links


This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).