Psychology Wiki
 
(4 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Measurement theory''' is a branch of applied mathematics that is useful in measurement and data analysis. The fundamental idea of measurement theory is that measurements are not the same as the attribute being measured. Hence, if you want to draw conclusions about the attribute, you must take into account the nature of the correspondence between the attribute and the measurements.
+
'''Measurement theory''' is a branch of applied mathematics that is useful in [[measurement]] and [[data analysis]]. The fundamental idea of measurement theory is that measurements are not the same as the attribute being measured. Hence, if you want to draw conclusions about the attribute, you must take into account the nature of the correspondence between the attribute and the measurements.
   
 
Measurement theory was popularized in psychology by S. S. Stevens, who originated the idea of levels of measurement. His relevant articles include Stevens (1946, 1951, 1959, 1968).
 
Measurement theory was popularized in psychology by S. S. Stevens, who originated the idea of levels of measurement. His relevant articles include Stevens (1946, 1951, 1959, 1968).
   
  +
The basic idea of any measurement theory is that a quantitative scale is a map between some empirical objects and associated numerical values. The prototype of a scale is the mapping of physical bodies to a measure of their physical mass. This mapping, however, is not arbitrary but is supposed to meet some requirements. The most important of them is that the mapping includes also a map between empirical relations between the objects to algebraic relations between the numerical values. Again the simplest example is that of physical mass. In this case the quantitative measurements are constructed in such a way that, for instance, the operation of combining objects corresponds to the addition of the masses of objects with the mass of to obtain the mass of : The physical operation of combining objects corresponds to the mathematical operation of summation. This is the most important aspect of defining a scale, since it implicitly defines the scientific meaning of the concept and determines how to use the measured values for predictions; for instance predicting the mass of a filled container from the masses of the container and that of the cargo.
==References==
 
Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1971), Foundations of measurement, Vol. I: Additive and polynomial representations, New York: Academic Press.
 
   
  +
There are other, more technical aspects of general measurement theory that we will not review here. They concern the types of scales and the uniqueness of scales. Scales are for instance classified as fundamental or derived, depending on whether they are based on existing scales or not. For those interested in these aspects of measurement theory we recommend the excellent summary by Suppes and Zinnes (1963).
Suppes, P., Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., and Tversky, A. (1989), Foundations of measurement, Vol. II: Geometrical, threshold, and probabilistic respresentations, New York: Academic Press.
 
   
  +
==Historical overview==
Luce, R. D., Krantz, D. H., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1990), Foundations of measurement, Vol. III: Representation, axiomatization, and invariance, New York: Academic Press.
 
  +
In the 1930s, the [[British Association for the Advancement of Science]] established the Ferguson Committee to investigate the possibility of psychological attributes being measured scientifically. In its Final Report (Ferguson, ''et al.'', 1940), Campbell and the Committee concluded that because psychological attributes were not capable of sustaining [[concatenation operations]], such attributes could not be continuous quantities. Therefore, they could not be measured scientifically. This had important ramifications for psychology, the most significant of these being the creation in 1946 of the ''operational theory of measurement'' by Harvard psychologist [[Stanley Smith Stevens]]. Stevens' non-scientific theory of measurement is widely held as definitive in psychology and the behavioural sciences generally {{harv|Michell|1999}}.
   
  +
==Types of measurement theory==
Allen, M., Yen, W., (2001), Introduction to Measurement Theory, Waveland Press ISBN-13: 978-1577662303
 
  +
*[[Axiomatic measurement theory]]
  +
*[[Operational theory of measurement]]
  +
*[[Theory of conjoint measurement]]
  +
  +
==See also==
  +
*[[Measurement level]]
  +
*[[Measurement model]]
  +
*[[Multiplicative model]]
  +
*[[Rasch model]]
  +
*[[Scale]]
  +
*[[Scaling]]
  +
*[[System of measurement]]
  +
*[[Units of measurement]]
  +
 
==References==
 
*Allen, M., Yen, W., (2001), Introduction to Measurement Theory, Waveland Press ISBN-13: 978-1577662303
 
*Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1971), Foundations of measurement, Vol. I: Additive and polynomial representations, New York: Academic Press.
 
*Luce, R. D., Krantz, D. H., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1990), Foundations of measurement, Vol. III: Representation, axiomatization, and invariance, New York: Academic Press.
 
*Suppes, P., Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., and Tversky, A. (1989), Foundations of measurement, Vol. II: Geometrical, threshold, and probabilistic respresentations, New York: Academic Press.
  +
* Torgerson, W.S. (1958). Theory and method of measurement. New York: Wiley.

Latest revision as of 23:01, 2 July 2013

Measurement theory is a branch of applied mathematics that is useful in measurement and data analysis. The fundamental idea of measurement theory is that measurements are not the same as the attribute being measured. Hence, if you want to draw conclusions about the attribute, you must take into account the nature of the correspondence between the attribute and the measurements.

Measurement theory was popularized in psychology by S. S. Stevens, who originated the idea of levels of measurement. His relevant articles include Stevens (1946, 1951, 1959, 1968).

The basic idea of any measurement theory is that a quantitative scale is a map between some empirical objects and associated numerical values. The prototype of a scale is the mapping of physical bodies to a measure of their physical mass. This mapping, however, is not arbitrary but is supposed to meet some requirements. The most important of them is that the mapping includes also a map between empirical relations between the objects to algebraic relations between the numerical values. Again the simplest example is that of physical mass. In this case the quantitative measurements are constructed in such a way that, for instance, the operation of combining objects corresponds to the addition of the masses of objects with the mass of to obtain the mass of : The physical operation of combining objects corresponds to the mathematical operation of summation. This is the most important aspect of defining a scale, since it implicitly defines the scientific meaning of the concept and determines how to use the measured values for predictions; for instance predicting the mass of a filled container from the masses of the container and that of the cargo.

There are other, more technical aspects of general measurement theory that we will not review here. They concern the types of scales and the uniqueness of scales. Scales are for instance classified as fundamental or derived, depending on whether they are based on existing scales or not. For those interested in these aspects of measurement theory we recommend the excellent summary by Suppes and Zinnes (1963).

Historical overview

In the 1930s, the British Association for the Advancement of Science established the Ferguson Committee to investigate the possibility of psychological attributes being measured scientifically. In its Final Report (Ferguson, et al., 1940), Campbell and the Committee concluded that because psychological attributes were not capable of sustaining concatenation operations, such attributes could not be continuous quantities. Therefore, they could not be measured scientifically. This had important ramifications for psychology, the most significant of these being the creation in 1946 of the operational theory of measurement by Harvard psychologist Stanley Smith Stevens. Stevens' non-scientific theory of measurement is widely held as definitive in psychology and the behavioural sciences generally (Michell 1999).

Types of measurement theory

See also

References

  • Allen, M., Yen, W., (2001), Introduction to Measurement Theory, Waveland Press ISBN-13: 978-1577662303
  • Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1971), Foundations of measurement, Vol. I: Additive and polynomial representations, New York: Academic Press.
  • Luce, R. D., Krantz, D. H., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. (1990), Foundations of measurement, Vol. III: Representation, axiomatization, and invariance, New York: Academic Press.
  • Suppes, P., Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., and Tversky, A. (1989), Foundations of measurement, Vol. II: Geometrical, threshold, and probabilistic respresentations, New York: Academic Press.
  • Torgerson, W.S. (1958). Theory and method of measurement. New York: Wiley.