Psychology Wiki
Register
Advertisement

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Biological: Behavioural genetics · Evolutionary psychology · Neuroanatomy · Neurochemistry · Neuroendocrinology · Neuroscience · Psychoneuroimmunology · Physiological Psychology · Psychopharmacology (Index, Outline)


The question of heritability of IQ, concerns the proportion of the variance of a population's IQ level that is attributable to genetic variation within that population. This proportion shows the level of importance of genetics versus environment for phenotypic variation in intelligence quotient (IQ) in a population. "Heritability", in this sense, "refers to the genetic contribution to variance within a population and in a specific environment".[1] There has been significant controversy in the academic community about the heritability of IQ ever since research began in the 19th century.[2]

Intelligence is a polygenic trait under normal circumstances according to recent research.[3] However, certain single gene genetic disorders can severely affect intelligence, with phenylketonuria as an example.[4]

Estimates in the academic research of the heritability of IQ have varied from below 0.5[2] to a high of 0.9 (of a maximum of 1.0).[5]IQ heritability increases during early childhood, but it is unclear whether it stabilizes thereafter.[6] A 1996 statement by the American Psychological Association gave about .45 for children and about .75 during and after adolescence.[7] A 2004 meta-analysis of reports in Current Directions in Psychological Science gave an overall estimate of around .85 for 18-year-olds and older.[8] The New York Times Magazine has listed about three quarters as a figure held by the majority of studies.[9]

Heritability and caveats[]

Main article: Heritability

"Heritability" is defined as the proportion of variance in a trait which is attributable to genotype within a defined population in a specific environment.[1] Heritability takes a value ranging from 0 to 1; a heritability of 1 indicates that all variation in the trait in question is genetic in origin and a heritability of 0 indicates that none of the variation is genetic. The determination of many traits can be considered primarily genetic under similar environmental backgrounds. For example, a 2006 study found that adult height has a heritability estimated at 0.80 when looking only at the height variation within families where the environment should be very similar.[10] Other traits have lower heritabilities, which indicate a relatively larger environmental influence. For example, a twin study on the heritability of depression in men calculated it as 0.29, while it was 0.42 for women in the same study.[11] Contrary to popular belief, two parents of higher IQ will not necessarily produce offspring of equal or higher intelligence. In fact, according to the concept of Regression toward the mean, parents of IQ at either extreme are more likely to produce offspring closer to the mean (or average)[12]

Caveats[]

There are a number of points to consider when interpreting heritability:

  • Heritability measures the proportion of variation in a trait that can be attributed to genes, and not the proportion of a trait caused by genes. Thus, if the environment relevant to a given trait changes in a way that affects all members of the population equally, the mean value of the trait will change without any change in its heritability (because the variation or differences among individuals in the population will stay the same). This has evidently happened for height: the heritability of stature is high, but average heights continue to increase.[7] Thus, even in developed nations, a high heritability of a trait does not necessarily mean that average group differences are due to genes.[7][13] Some have gone further, and used height as an example in order to argue that "even highly heritable traits can be strongly manipulated by the environment, so heritability has little if anything to do with controllability."[14] However, others argue that IQ is highly stable during life and has been largely resistant to interventions aimed to change it long-term and substantially.[15][16][17]
  • A common error is to assume that a heritability figure is necessarily unchangeable. The value of heritability can change if the impact of environment (or of genes) in the population is substantially altered.[7] If the environmental variation encountered by different individuals increases, then the heritability figure would decrease. On the other hand, if everyone had the same environment, then heritability would be 100%. The population in developing nations often has more diverse environments than in developed nations.[citation needed] This would mean that heritability figures would be lower in developing nations. Another example is phenylketonuria which previously caused mental retardation for everyone who had this genetic disorder and thus had a heritability of 100%. Today, this can be prevented by following a modified diet, resulting in a lowered heritability.
  • A high heritability of a trait does not mean that environmental effects such as learning are not involved. Vocabulary size, for example, is very substantially heritable (and highly correlated with general intelligence) although every word in an individual's vocabulary is learned. In a society in which plenty of words are available in everyone's environment, especially for individuals who are motivated to seek them out, the number of words that individuals actually learn depends to a considerable extent on their genetic predispositions and thus heritability is high.[7]
  • Since heritability increases during childhood and adolescence, and even increases greatly between 16–20 years of age and adulthood, one should be cautious drawing conclusions regarding the role of genetics and environment from studies where the participants are not followed until they are adults. Furthermore, there may be differences regarding the effects on the g-factor and on non-g factors, with g possibly being harder to affect and environmental interventions disproportionately affecting non-g factors.[17]

Estimates of the heritability of IQ[]

Various studies have found the heritability of IQ to be between 0.7 and 0.8 in adults and 0.45 in childhood in the United States.[7][5][18] It may seem reasonable to expect that genetic influences on traits like IQ should become less important as one gains experiences with age. However, that the opposite occurs is well documented. Heritability measures in infancy are as low as 0.2, around 0.4 in middle childhood, and as high as 0.8 in adulthood.[8][19] One proposed explanation is that people with different genes tend to seek out different environments that reinforce the effects of those genes.[7]

A 1994 review in Behavior Genetics based on identical/fraternal twin studies found that heritability is as high as 0.80 in general cognitive ability but it also varies based on the trait, with .60 for verbal tests, .50 for spatial and speed-of-processing tests, and only .40 for memory tests.[5]

In 2006, The New York Times Magazine listed about three quarters as a figure held by the majority of studies,[9] while a 2004 meta-analysis of reports in Current Directions in Psychological Science gave an overall estimate of around .85 for 18-year-olds and older.[8]

Shared family environment[]

There are some family effects on the IQ of children, accounting for up to a quarter of the variance. However, adoption studies show that by adulthood adoptive siblings aren't more similar in IQ than strangers,[20] while adult full siblings show an IQ correlation of 0.6. Conventional twin studies reinforce this pattern: monozygotic (identical) twins raised separately are highly similar in IQ (0.86), more so than dizygotic (fraternal) twins raised together (0.6) and much more than adoptive siblings (~0.0).[21] However, some studies of twins reared apart (e.g. Bouchard, 1990) find a significant shared environmental influence, of at least 10% going into late adulthood.[18] JR Harris suggests that this might be due to biasing assumptions in the methodology of the classical twin and adoption studies.[22]

There are aspects of environments that family members have in common (for example, characteristics of the home). This shared family environment accounts for 0.25-0.35 of the variation in IQ in childhood. By late adolescence it is quite low (zero in some studies). There is a similar effect for several other psychological traits. These studies have not looked the effects of extreme environments such as in abusive families.[7][20][23][24]

The American Psychological Association's report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" (1995) states that there is no doubt that normal child development requires a certain minimum level of responsible care. Severely deprived, neglectful, or abusive environments must have negative effects on a great many aspects of development, including intellectual aspects. Beyond that minimum, however, the role of family experience is in serious dispute. There is no doubt that such variables as resources of the home and parents' use of language are correlated with children's IQ scores, but such correlations may be mediated by genetic as well as (or instead of) environmental factors. But how much of that variance in IQ results from differences between families, as contrasted with the varying experiences of different children in the same family? Recent twin and adoption studies suggest that while the effect of the shared family environment is substantial in early childhood, it becomes quite small by late adolescence. These findings suggest that differences in the life styles of families whatever their importance may be for many aspects of children's lives make little long-term difference for the skills measured by intelligence tests.[How to reference and link to summary or text]

Non-shared family environment and environment outside the family[]

Although parents treat their children differently, such differential treatment explains only a small amount of non-shared environmental influence. One suggestion is that children react differently to the same environment due to different genes. More likely influences may be the impact of peers and other experiences outside the family.[7][23] For example, siblings grown up in the same household may have different friends and teachers and even contract different illnesses. This factor may be one of the reasons why IQ score correlations between siblings decreases as they get older.[25]

Heritability and socioeconomic status[]

The APA report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" (1995) also stated that:

"We should note, however, that low-income and non-white families are poorly represented in existing adoption studies as well as in most twin samples. Thus it is not yet clear whether these studies apply to the population as a whole. It remains possible that, across the full range of income and ethnicity, between-family differences have more lasting consequences for psychometric intelligence."[7]

A study (1999) by Capron and Duyme of French children adopted between the ages of four and six examined the influence of socioeconomic status (SES). The children's IQs initially averaged 77, putting them near retardation. Most were abused or neglected as infants, then shunted from one foster home or institution to the next. Nine years later after adoption, when they were on average 14 years old, they retook the IQ tests, and all of them did better. The amount they improved was directly related to the adopting family's socioeconomic status. "Children adopted by farmers and laborers had average IQ scores of 85.5; those placed with middle-class families had average scores of 92. The average IQ scores of youngsters placed in well-to-do homes climbed more than 20 points, to 98."[9][26]

Stoolmiller (1999) argued that the range of environments in previous adoption studies were restricted. Adopting families tend to be more similar on, for example, socio-economic status than the general population, which suggests a possible underestimation of the role of the shared family environment in previous studies. Corrections for range restriction to adoption studies indicated that socio-economic status could account for as much as 50% of the variance in IQ.[27]

On the other hand, the effect of this was examined by Matt McGue and colleagues (2007), who wrote that "restriction in range in parent disinhibitory psychopathology and family socio-economic status had no effect on adoptive-sibling correlations [in] IQ"[28]

Turkheimer and colleagues (2003) argued that the proportions of IQ variance attributable to genes and environment vary with socioeconomic status. They found that in a study on seven-year-old twins, in impoverished families, 60% of the variance in early childhood IQ was accounted for by the shared family environment, and the contribution of genes is close to zero; in affluent families, the result is almost exactly the reverse.[29]

A study by Nagoshi and Johnson (2005) failed to replicate Turkheimer and colleagues' findings. They concluded that the heritability of IQ did not vary as a function of parental socioeconomic status in the 949 families of Caucasian and 400 families of Japanese ancestry who took part in the Hawaii Family Study of Cognition.[30]

Asbury and colleagues (2005) studied the effect of environmental risk factors on verbal and non-verbal ability in a nationally representative sample of 4-year-old British twins. There was not any statistically significant interaction for non-verbal ability, but the heritability of verbal ability was found to be higher in low-SES and high-risk environments.[31]

Harden and colleagues (2007) investigated adolescents, most 17 years old, and found that, among higher income families, genetic influences accounted for approximately 55% of the variance in cognitive aptitude and shared environmental influences about 35%. Among lower income families, the proportions were in the reverse direction, 39% genetic and 45% shared environment."[32]

Rushton and Jensen (2010) criticized many of these studies for being done on children or adolescents. They argued that heritability increases during childhood and adolescence, and even increases greatly between 16–20 years of age and adulthood, so one should be cautious drawing conclusions regarding the role of genetics from studies where the participants are not adults. Furthermore, the studies typically did not examine if IQ gains due to adoption were on the general intelligence factor (g). When the studies by Capron and Duyme were re-examined, IQ gains from being adopted into high SES homes were on non-g factors. By contrast, the adopted children's g mainly depended on their biological parents SES, which implied that g is more difficult to environmentally change.[17]

A 2011 study by Tucker-Drob and colleagues reported that at age 2 years, genes accounted for approximately 50% of the variation in mental ability for children being raised in high socioeconomic status families, but genes accounted for negligible variation in mental ability for children being raised in low socioeconomic status families. This gene-environment interaction was not apparent at age 10 months, suggesting that the effect emerges over the course of early development.[33]

A 2012 study based on a representative sample of twins from the United Kingdom, with longitudinal data on IQ from age two to age fourteen, did not find evidence for lower heritability in low-SES families. However, the study indicated that the effects of shared family environment on IQ were generally greater in low-SES families than in high-SES families, resulting in greater variance in IQ in low-SES families. The authors noted that previous research had produced inconsistent results on whether or not SES moderates the heritability of IQ. They suggested three explanations for the inconsistency. First, some studies may have lacked statistical power to detect interactions. Second, the age range investigated has varied between studies. Third, the effect of SES may vary in different demographics and different countries.[34]

Maternal (fetal) environment[]

A meta-analysis by Devlin and colleagues (1997) of 212 previous studies evaluated an alternative model for environmental influence and found that it fits the data better than the 'family-environments' model commonly used. The shared maternal (fetal) environment effects, often assumed to be negligible, account for 20% of covariance between twins and 5% between siblings, and the effects of genes are correspondingly reduced, with two measures of heritability being less than 50%. They argue that the shared maternal environment may explain the striking correlation between the IQs of twins, especially those of adult twins that were reared apart.[2] IQ heritability increases during early childhood, but whether it stabilizes thereafter remains unclear.[2] These results have two implications: a new model may be required regarding the influence of genes and environment on cognitive function; and interventions aimed at improving the prenatal environment could lead to a significant boost in the population's IQ.[2]

Bouchard and McGue reviewed the literature in 2003, arguing that Devlin's conclusions about the magnitude of heritability is not substantially different than previous reports and that their conclusions regarding prenatal effects stands in contradiction to many previous reports.[35] They write that:

Chipuer et al. and Loehlin conclude that the postnatal rather than the prenatal environment is most important. The Devlin et al. (1997a) conclusion that the prenatal environment contributes to twin IQ similarity is especially remarkable given the existence of an extensive empirical literature on prenatal effects. Price (1950), in a comprehensive review published over 50 years ago, argued that almost all MZ twin prenatal effects produced differences rather than similarities. As of 1950 the literature on the topic was so large that the entire bibliography was not published. It was finally published in 1978 with an additional 260 references. At that time Price reiterated his earlier conclusion (Price, 1978). Research subsequent to the 1978 review largely reinforces Price’s hypothesis (Bryan, 1993; Macdonald et al., 1993; Hall and Lopez-Rangel, 1996; see also Martin et al., 1997, box 2; Machin, 1996).[35]

Dickens and Flynn model[]

Dickens and Flynn (2001) argued that the "heritability" figure includes both a direct effect of the genotype on IQ and also indirect effects where the genotype changes the environment, in turn affecting IQ. That is, those with a higher IQ tend to seek out stimulating environments that further increase IQ. The direct effect can initially have been very small but feedback loops can create large differences in IQ. In their model an environmental stimulus can have a very large effect on IQ, even in adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues. This model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition in early childhood, that may cause permanent effects.

The Flynn effect is the increase in average intelligence test scores by about 0.3% annually, resulting in the average person today scoring 15 points higher in IQ compared to the generation 50 years ago.[36] This effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that programs aiming to increase IQ would be most likely to produce long-term IQ gains if they taught children how to replicate outside the program the kinds of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains while they are in the program and motivate them to persist in that replication long after they have left the program.[37][38] Most of the improvements have allowed for better abstract reasoning, spatial relations, and comprehension. Some scientists have suggested that such enhancements are due to better nutrition, better parenting and schooling, as well as exclusion of the least intelligent people from reproduction. However, Flynn and a group of other scientists share the viewpoint that modern life implies solving many abstract problems which leads to a rise in their IQ scores.[36]

Influence of genes on IQ stability[]

Recent research has illuminated genetic factors underlying IQ stability and change. Modern Genome Wide Association Studies have demonstrated that the genes involved in intelligence remain fairly stable over time.[39] Specifically, in terms of IQ stability, "genetic factors mediated phenotypic stability throughout this entire period [age 0 to 16], whereas most age-to-age instability appeared to be due to nonshared environmental influences".[40][41] These findings have been replicated extensively and observed in the United Kingdom,[42] the United States,[40][43] and the Netherlands.[44][45][46][47] Additionally, researchers have shown that naturalistic changes in IQ occur in individuals at variable times.[48]

Heritability of giftedness[]

Main article: Intellectual giftedness

High intelligence is a form of intellectual giftedness. The study of giftedness, whether it has multiple forms, their heritability and their relation to each other is also a subject of major study.

Molecular genetic investigations[]

A 2009 review article identified over 50 genetic polymorphisms that have been reported to be associated with cognitive ability in various studies, but noted that the discovery of small effect sizes and lack of replication have characterized this research so far.[49] Another study attempted to replicate 12 reported associations between specific genetic variants and general cognitive ability in three large datasets, but found that only one of the genotypes was significantly associated with general intelligence in one of the samples, a result expected by chance alone. The authors concluded that most reported genetic associations with general intelligence are probably false positives brought about by inadequate sample sizes. Arguing that common genetic variants explain much of the variation in general intelligence, they suggested that the effects of individual variants are so small that very large samples are required to reliably detect them.[50]

A novel molecular genetic method for estimating heritability calculates the overall genetic similarity (as indexed by the cumulative effects of all genotyped single nucleotide polymorphisms) between all pairs of individuals in a sample of unrelated individuals and then correlates this genetic similarity with phenotypic similarity across all the pairs. A study using this method estimated that the lower bounds for the narrow-sense heritability of crystallized and fluid intelligence are 40% and 51%, respectively. A replication study in an independent sample confirmed these results, reporting a heritability estimate of 47%. These findings are compatible with the view that a large number of genes, each with only a small effect, contribute to differences in intelligence.[50]

Correlations between IQ and degree of genetic relatedness[]

Template:Content

The neutrality of this section is disputed.

The relative influence of genetics and environment for a trait can be calculated by measuring how strongly traits covary in people of a given genetic (unrelated, siblings, fraternal twins, or identical twins) and environmental (reared in the same family or not) relationship. One method is to consider identical twins reared apart, with any similarities which exists between such twin pairs attributed to genotype. In terms of correlation statistics, this means that theoretically the correlation of tests scores between monozygotic twins would be 1.00 if genetics alone accounted for variation in IQ scores; likewise, siblings and dizygotic twins share on average half of their alleles and the correlation of their scores would be 0.50 if IQ were affected by genes alone (or greater if, as is undoubtedly the case, there is a positive correlation between the IQs of spouses in the parental generation). Practically, however, the upper bound of these correlations are given by the reliability of the test, which is 0.90 to 0.95 for typical IQ tests[51]

If there is biological inheritance of IQ, then the relatives of a person with a high IQ should exhibit a comparably high IQ with a much higher probability than the general population. In 1982, Bouchard and McGue reviewed such correlations reported in 111 original studies in the United States. The mean correlation of IQ scores between monozygotic twins was 0.86, between siblings, 0.47, between half-siblings, 0.31, and between cousins, 0.15.[52]

The 2006 edition of Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence by Alan S. Kaufman and Elizabeth O. Lichtenberger reports correlations of 0.86 for identical twins raised together compared to 0.76 for those raised apart and 0.47 for siblings.[53] These number are not necessarily static. When comparing pre-1963 to late 1970s data, researches DeFries and Plomin found that the IQ correlation between parent and child living together fell significantly, from 0.50 to 0.35. The opposite occurred for fraternal twins.[54]

Another summary:

  • Same person (tested twice) .95
  • Identical twins—Reared together .86
  • Identical twins—Reared apart .76
  • Fraternal twins—Reared together .55
  • Fraternal twins—Reared apart .35
  • Biological siblings—Reared together .47
  • Biological siblings—Reared apart .24
  • Unrelated children—Reared together .30
  • Parent-child—Living together .42
  • Parent-child—Living apart .22
  • Adoptive parent–child—Living together .19[55]

Between-group heritability[]

Although IQ differences between individuals are shown to have a large hereditary component, it does not follow that mean group-level disparities (between-group differences) in IQ necessarily have a genetic basis. An analogy, attributed to Richard Lewontin,[56] illustrates this point:

Suppose two handfuls are taken from a sack containing a genetically diverse variety of corn, and each grown under carefully controlled and standardized conditions, except that one batch is lacking in certain nutrients that are supplied to the other. After several weeks, the plants are measured. There is variability of growth within each batch, due to the genetic variability of the corn. Given that the growing conditions are closely controlled, nearly all the variation in the height of the plants within a batch will be due to differences in their genes. Thus, within populations, heritabilities will be very high. Nevertheless, the difference between the two groups is due entirely to an environmental factor - differential nutrition. Lewontin didn't go so far as to have the one set of pots painted white and the other set black, but you get the idea. The point of the example, in any case, is that the causes of between-group differences may in principle be quite different from the causes of within-group variation.[57]

Arthur Jensen has written in agreement that this is technically correct, but he has also stated that a high heritability increases the probability that genetics play a role in average group differences.[58]

.

Notes and references[]

  1. 1.0 1.1 Rose SP (June 2006). Commentary: heritability estimates--long past their sell-by date. Int J Epidemiol 35 (3): 525–7.
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 (1997). The heritability of IQ. Nature 388 (6641): 468–71.
  3. Comings DE, Wu S, Rostamkhani M, et al. (January 2003). Role of the cholinergic muscarinic 2 receptor (CHRM2) gene in cognition. Mol. Psychiatry 8 (1): 10–1.
  4. (2002) The general factor of intelligence, 260–261, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Template:Pn
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 (1994). Variability and stability in cognitive abilities are largely genetic later in life. Behavior Genetics 24 (3): 207–15.
  6. (1997). The heritability of IQ. Nature 388 (6641): 468–71.
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 7.4 7.5 7.6 7.7 7.8 7.9 (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist 51 (2): 77–101.
  8. 8.0 8.1 8.2 Bouchard, TJ (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits - A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13 (4): 148–151. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; name "TJ" defined multiple times with different content
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 includeonly>David L. Kirp. "After the Bell Curve", New York Times Magazine, July 23, 2006. Retrieved on August 6, 2006.
  10. (2006). Assumption-Free Estimation of Heritability from Genome-Wide Identity-by-Descent Sharing between Full Siblings. PLoS Genetics 2 (3): e41.
  11. (2006). A Swedish National Twin Study of Lifetime Major Depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 163 (1): 109–14.
  12. (2010) "Intelligence" Psychology, 2nd, 405–6, New York: Worth Publishers.
  13. (1996). Ethnic Differences in Children's Intelligence Test Scores: Role of Economic Deprivation, Home Environment, and Maternal Characteristics. Child Development 67 (2): 396–408.
  14. (2009). Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 (4): 217–20.
  15. (2005) Making Sense of Heritability.Template:Pn
  16. Gottfredson, Linda S. Flynn, Ceci, and Turkheimer on race and intelligence: Opening moves.
  17. 17.0 17.1 17.2 (2010). Race and IQ: A Theory-Based Review of the Research in Richard Nisbett's Intelligence and How to Get It. The Open Psychology Journal 3: 9–35.
  18. 18.0 18.1 (1990). Sources of Human Psychological Differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science 250 (4978): 223–8.
  19. (2001) Behavioral Genetics, 4th, Worth Publishers.Template:Pn
  20. 20.0 20.1 (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities. Human biology 70 (2): 257–79.
  21. Plomin et al. (2001, 2003)Template:Pn
  22. Harris, JR (2006). No Two Alike.Template:Pn
  23. 23.0 23.1 (2001). Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 46 (3): 225–33.
  24. (Harris 1998)
  25. (2010) Psychology, 2nd, New York: Worth Publishers.
  26. (1999). How can we boost IQs of 'dull children'?: A late adoption study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96 (15): 8790–4.
  27. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin 125 (4): 392–409.
  28. (2007). The Environments of Adopted and Non-adopted Youth: Evidence on Range Restriction From the Sibling Interaction and Behavior Study (SIBS). Behavior Genetics 37 (3): l449–462.
  29. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of iq in young children. Psychological Science 14 (6): 623–8.
  30. (2004). Socioeconomic Status Does Not Moderate the Familiality of Cognitive Abilities in the Hawaii Family Study of Cognition. Journal of Biosocial Science 37 (6): 773–81.
  31. (2005). Environmental moderators of genetic influence on verbal and nonverbal abilities in early childhood. Intelligence 33 (6): 643–61.
  32. (2006). Genotype by Environment Interaction in Adolescents' Cognitive Aptitude. Behavior Genetics 37 (2): 273–83.
  33. (2010). Emergence of a Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Infant Mental Ability Between 10 Months and 2 Years. Psychological Science 22 (1): 125–33.
  34. (2012). Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Children's Intelligence (IQ): In a UK-Representative Sample SES Moderates the Environmental, Not Genetic, Effect on IQ. PLoS ONE 7 (2): e30320.
  35. 35.0 35.1 (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology 54 (1): 4–45.
  36. 36.0 36.1 (2010) Psychology, 2nd, New York: Worth Publishers.
  37. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review 108 (2): 346–69.
  38. (2002). The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved. Psychological Review 109 (4).
  39. (2013). DNA evidence for strong genetic stability and increasing heritability of intelligence from age 7 to 12. Molecular Psychiatry.
  40. 40.0 40.1 (2004). Genetic and Environmental Contributions to General Cognitive Ability Through the First 16 Years of Life. Developmental Psychology 40 (5): 805–12.
  41. (2009). Genes Determine Stability and the Environment Determines Change in Cognitive Ability During 35 Years of Adulthood. Psychological Science 20 (9): 1146–52.
  42. (2007). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 72 (3): vii, 1–144.
  43. (1989). Modeling IQ change: Evidence from the Texas Adoption Project. Child development 60 (4): 993–1004.
  44. (2012). Heritability of Verbal and Performance Intelligence in a Pediatric Longitudinal Sample. Twin Research and Human Genetics 14 (2): 119–28.
  45. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on the development of intelligence. Behavior genetics 32 (4): 237–49.
  46. (2007). Longitudinal genetic study of verbal and nonverbal IQ from early childhood to young adulthood. Learning and Individual Differences 17 (2): 97–114.
  47. (2003). A twin study of differentiation of cognitive abilities in childhood. Behavior genetics 33 (4): 367–81.
  48. (1993). The natural history of change in intellectual performance: Who changes? How much? Is it meaningful?. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 34 (4): 455–506.
  49. (2009). The Impact of Genetic Research on our Understanding of Normal Cognitive Ageing: 1995 to 2009. Neuropsychology Review 19 (4): 451–77.
  50. 50.0 50.1 (2012). Most Reported Genetic Associations with General Intelligence Are Probably False Positives. Psychological Science 23 (11): 1314–23.
  51. Jensen, Arthur (1998). The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability. Westport, Connecticut: Praeger PublishersTemplate:Pn
  52. (1981). Familial Studies of Intelligence: A Review. Science 212 (4498): 1055–9.
  53. (2006) Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence, 3rd, Hoboken (NJ): Wiley.Template:Pn
  54. (1980). Genetics and intelligence: Recent data. Intelligence 4: 15.
  55. IQ Testing 101, Alan S. Kaufman, 2009, Springer Publishing Company, ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2Template:Pn
  56. (1970). Race and intelligence. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists 26 (3): 2–8.
  57. (1992). On Shonemann on Guttman on Jensen, via Lewontin. Multivariate Behavioral Research 27 (2): 261.
  58. (1970). Race and the genetics of intelligence: A reply to Lewontin. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists 26 (5): 17–23.

Further reading[]

  • McGuffin P (2000). The quantitative and molecular genetics of human intelligence. Novartis Found. Symp. 233: 243–55; discussion 255–9, 276–80.
  • G. Meisenberg: Genes for intelligence. A review of recent progress. Mankind Quarterly 36 (Winter 2005) 139-164. abstract
  • Payton A (2006). Investigating cognitive genetics and its implications for the treatment of cognitive deficit. Genes, Brain and Behavior 5 Suppl 1: 44–53.


This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).
Advertisement