Psychology Wiki
 
(One intermediate revision by the same user not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
  +
{{CogPsy}}
'''George Armitage Miller''' (3rd February [[1920]] in Charleston, West Virginia) is James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology, Emeritus at [[Princeton University]]. He is the author of one of the most highly cited papers in psychology, "[[The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two]]"<ref name="miller">
 
{{cite journal
 
| last=Miller | first=G. A. | ref=harv
 
| author-link=George Armitage Miller
 
| year=1956
 
| title=[[The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two|The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information]]
 
| journal=Psychological Review
 
| volume=63 | issue=2
 
| pages=81–97
 
}} ([http://www.psych.utoronto.ca/users/peterson/psy430s2001/Miller%20GA%20Magical%20Seven%20Psych%20Review%201955.pdf pdf])
 
</ref> published in 1956 in ''[[Psychological Review]]''.<ref>Gorenflo, Daniel W., [[James V. McConnell|McConnell, James V.]] (1991). "The Most Frequently Cited Journal Articles and Authors in Introductory Psychology Textbooks", ''[[Teaching of Psychology]]'', 18: 8 – 12</ref><ref>Kintsch W, Cacioppo JT.(1994). [http://psychology.uchicago.edu/people/faculty/cacioppo/jtcreprints/kc94.pdf Introduction to the 100th anniversary issue of the Psychological Review]. Psychological Review. 101: 195-199</ref><ref>[[Eugene Garfield|Garfied E.]], (1985). [http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/essays/v8p187y1985.pdf Essays of an Information Scientist], 8: 187-196; [[Current Contents]], (#20, p.3-12, May 20)</ref> This paper suggests that seven (plus or minus two) is the magic number that characterizes people's memory performance on random lists of letters, words, numbers, or almost any kind of meaningful familiar item.
 
   
  +
'''George Armitage Miller''' (February 3, 1920 – July 22, 2012) was one of the founders of the [[cognitive psychology]] field. He also contributed to the birth of [[psycholinguistics]] and [[cognitive science]] in general. Miller wrote several books and directed the development of [[WordNet]], an online word-linkage [[database]] usable by [[computer programs]]. He authored the paper, "[[The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two]]," which experimentally discovered an average limit of seven for human [[short-term memory]] capacity. This paper is frequently cited in both [[psychology]] and the wider culture. He also won awards such as the [[National Medal of Science]].
 
   
  +
Miller started his education focusing on speech and language and published papers on these topics, focusing on [[mathematical]], [[Computing|computational]] and psychological aspects of the field. He started his career at a time when the reigning theory in psychology was [[behaviorism]], which eschewed any attempt to study [[mental processes]] and focused only on observable behavior. Working mostly at [[Harvard University]], [[MIT]] and [[Princeton University]], Miller introduced [[experimental techniques]] to study the psychology of mental processes, by linking the new field of cognitive psychology to the broader area of cognitive science, including [[computation theory]] and [[linguistics]]. He collaborated and co-authored work with other figures in cognitive science and psycholinguistics, such as [[Noam Chomsky]]. For moving psychology into the realm of mental processes and for aligning that move with information theory, computation theory, and linguistics, Miller is considered one of the great twentieth-century psychologists.
In 1940 he received a Bachelor of Arts degree from the [[University of Alabama]] and in 1946 he received his Ph.D. in Psychology from [[Harvard University]].
 
   
  +
== Life and education ==
He has served as Professor of Psychology at [[Rockerfeller University]], [[Massachusetts Institute of Technology]] and at [[Harvard University]], where he was Chairman of the Department of Psychology. He was a Fulbright Research Fellow at [[Oxford University]] and served at the President of the [[American Psychological Association]]. His most famous work was ''[[The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two|The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Information]]'', which was published in [[1956]] in ''The Psychological Review.''
 
   
  +
Miller was born on February 3, 1920, at Charleston, West Virginia, the son of an executive at a steel company,{{r|NYTimes}} George E. Miller, and Florence (Armitage) Miller.{{r|Marquis}} Soon after, his parents divorced. He grew up with only his mother during the Great Depression, attended public school, and graduated from Charleston High School in 1937. He relocated with his mother and stepfather to Washington D.C., and was at [[George Washington University]] for a year. His family practiced [[Christian Science]], which required turning to prayer, rather than medical science, for healing. After his stepfather was transferred to Birmingham, Alabama, Miller transferred to the [[University of Alabama]].{{r|PsycNet}}
In 1960, Miller founded the Center for Cognitive Studies at Harvard with [[Jerome Bruner]] (a cognitivist developmentalist). In the same year he published 'Plans and the Structure of Behaviour' (with Eugene Galanter and [[Karl Pribram]]), which outlined their conception of [[Cognitive psychology|Cognitive Psychology]].
 
   
  +
He received his bachelor's degree in history and speech in 1940, and a master's in speech in 1941 from the University of Alabama. He had taken courses in [[phonetics]], voice science, and [[speech pathology]] . Membership in the Drama club fostered his interest in courses in the Speech Department. He was also influenced by Professor [[Donald Ramsdell]], who introduced him both to psychology, and, indirectly through a seminar, to his future wife Katherine James.{{r|PsycNet}} They married on November 29, 1939. Katherine died in January, 1996.{{r|Marquis}}{{r|LATimes}} He married Margaret Ferguson Skutch Page in 2008.{{r|Marquis}}{{r|WashingtonPost}}
He was a Fulbright Research Fellow at [[Oxford University]]. He is also a former President of the [[American Psychological Association]], and in 1991, received the [[National Medal of Science]].
 
   
  +
Miller taught the course "Introduction to Psychology" at Alabama for two years. He enrolled in the Ph.D. program in psychology at Harvard University in 1943, after coming to the university in 1942.{{r|PsycNet}} He received his doctorate in 1946 from Harvard's Psycho-Acoustic Laboratory, under the supervision of [[Stanley Smith Stevens]], researching military voice communications for the Army Signal Corps during World War II. His doctorate thesis, "The Optimal Design of Jamming Signals," was classified top secret by the US Army.{{r|PsycNet}} He was on the faculty at Harvard, MIT and [[Rockefeller University]] before settling at Princeton in 1979.{{r|LATimes}}{{r|Lindzey}}
In the [[linguistics]] community, Miller is well-known for overseeing the development of [[WordNet]], a semantic network for the English language. Development began in 1985 and the project has received about $3 million of funding, mainly from government agencies interested in machine translation. He is also working on [[READER - a lexical aid]] that uses wordnet to help students read computer text. He spent the later part of his career building and expanding the WordNet database. He also worked on a number of commercial applications based on it, most notably, [[Simpli]]. Simpli was an early Internet search and marketing engine created by George Miller and a number of Professors and graduate students at [[Brown University]], including [[Jeff Stibel]], [[James A. Anderson]] and [[Steve Reiss]]. Simpli utilized WordNet to "read" search queries and disambiguate them. It was also used to read webpages and derive representative keywords so that advertising could be presented. Applied Semantics, a competing search engine that was eventually acquired by Google and evolved into Google AdSense, was based on the WordNet lexicon, as well.<ref>{{cite press release|url=http://www.google.com/press/pressrel/applied.html|title=Google Acquires Applied Semantics|publisher=Google|date=April 23, 2003|accessdate=2008-12-02}}</ref>
 
  +
  +
In his later years, Miller enjoyed playing golf.{{r|NYTimes}} He died in 2012 at his home in Plainsboro, New Jersey of complications of [[pneumonia]] and [[dementia]].{{r|LATimes}} At the time of his passing, he was survived by his wife Margaret; the children from his first marriage: son Donnally James and daughter Nancy Saunders;two stepsons, David Skutch and Christopher Skutch; and three grandchildren: Gavin Murray-Miller, Morgan Murray-Miller and Nathaniel James Miller.{{r|WashingtonPost}}{{r|Princeton}}
  +
  +
== Career ==
  +
  +
After receiving his doctorate, Miller stayed as a research fellow at Harvard, to continue his research on speech and hearing. He was appointed assistant professor of psychology in 1948. The course he developed on language and communication would eventually lead to his first major book, ''Language and communication'' (1951). He took a sabbatical in 1950, and spent a year as a visiting fellow at the [[Institute for Advanced Study]], [[Princeton, New Jersey | Princeton]], to pursue his interest in mathematics. Miller befriended [[J. Robert Oppenheimer]], with whom he played squash. <ref>{{cite book |title=J. Robert Oppenheimer: A life |author=Pais A. |year=2006 |publisher=Oxford University Press |page=89}}</ref> In 1951, Miller joined MIT as an associate professor of psychology. He led the psychology group at [[MIT Lincoln Lab]]. He worked on voice communication and [[human engineering]], whereupon he identified the minimal voice features of speech required for it to be intelligible. Based on this work, in 1955, he was invited to a talk at the [[Eastern Psychological Association]]. That presentation, "The magical number seven, plus or minus two", was later published as a paper which went on to be a legendary one in cognitive psychology.{{r|PsycNet}}
  +
  +
Miller moved back to Harvard as a tenured associate professor in 1955 and became a full professor in 1958, expanding his research into how language affects human cognition.{{r|PsycNet}} At the university he met a young Noam Chomsky, another of the founders of cognitive science. They spent a summer together at Stanford training the faculty, and their two families shared a house. In 1958–59, Miller took leave to join the [[Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences]] at [[Palo Alto, California]], (now at [[Stanford University]]).{{r|APS}} There he collaborated with Eugene Galanter and Karl Pribram on the book ''Plans and the Structure of Behavior''. In 1960, along with Jerome S. Bruner,{{r|NYTimes}}{{r|PsycNet}} he co-founded the Center for Cognitive Sciences at Harvard.{{r|PsycNet}} The cognitive term was a break from the then-dominant school of behaviorism, which insisted cognition was not fit for scientific study.{{r|NYTimes}} The center attracted such notable visitors as [[Jean Piaget]], [[Alexander Luria]] and Chomsky.{{r|APS}} Miller then became the chair of the psychology department.{{r|PsycNet}} Miller was instrumental at the time for recruiting [[Timothy Leary]] to teach at Harvard. Miller knew Leary from the University of Alabama, where Miller was teaching psychology and Leary graduated with an undergraduate degree from the department.
  +
  +
In 1967, Miller taught at Rockefeller University for a year, as a [[visiting professor]],{{r|Marquis}} researching how lexical memory was stored, but declined a position as professor in [[experimental psychology]].{{r|PsycNet}} A new president's selection at Rockefeller made him leave.{{r|APS}} In 1979, he moved to Princeton as the James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology.{{r|PsycNet}} In 1986, he helped in founding the Cognitive Science Laboratory at Princeton. Eventually he became a [[professor emeritus]] and senior research psychologist at Princeton. He also directed the McDonnell-Pew Program in Cognitive Science.{{r|PsycNet}}
  +
  +
Miller had [[honorary doctorates]] from the [[University of Sussex]] (1984), [[Columbia University]] (1980), [[Yale University]] (1979), [[Université catholique de Louvain|Catholic University of Louvain]] (1978),{{r|PsycNet}} [[Carnegie Mellon University]] (in humane letters, 2003),{{r|CMU}} and an honorary [[Doctor of Science|DSC]] from [[Williams College]] (2000).{{r|Honorary}} He was elected to the [[American Academy of Arts and Sciences]] in 1957,{{r|Princeton}} the [[National Academy of Sciences]] in 1962,{{r|Princeton}} the presidency of the Eastern Psychological Association in 1962,{{r|PsycNet}} the presidency of the [[American Psychological Association]] in 1969,{{r|PsycNet}} and to the [[Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences]] in 1985.{{r|Princeton}} Miller was the keynote speaker at the first convention of the Association for Psychological Science in 1989.{{r|HistoryOfAPS}} He was a [[Fulbright research fellow]] at [[Oxford University]] in 1964–65,{{r|APS}} and in 1991, received the National Medal of Science.{{r|Princeton}}
  +
  +
== Major contributions ==
  +
  +
Miller's career started during the reign of behaviorism in psychology. Behaviorists questioned whether mental thought processes were fit for [[scientific study]], not being observable. They focused on working with responses to [[Stimulus (psychology)|stimuli]], particularly among other animals. Miller disagreed. He, Jerome Bruner, and Noam Chomsky are considered the founders of the field of Cognitive Psychology that replaced behaviorism as the framework for analyzing the mind.{{r|LATimes}}
  +
  +
=== Working memory ===
  +
  +
From the days of [[William James]], psychologists had the idea memory consisted of [[short-term memory | short-term]] and [[long-term memory]]. While short-term memory was expected to be limited, its exact limits were not known. In 1956, Miller would quantify its capacity limit in the paper "The magical number seven, plus or minus two". He tested [[immediate memory]] via tasks such as asking a person to repeat a set of digits presented; absolute judgment by presenting a stimulus and a label, and asking them to recall the label later; and [[span of attention]] by asking them to count things in a group of more than a few items quickly. For all three cases, Miller found the average limit to be seven items. He had mixed feelings about the focus on his work on the exact number seven for quantifying short-term memory, and felt it had been misquoted often. He stated, introducing the paper on the research for the first time, that he was being persecuted by an integer.{{r|NYTimes}} Miller also found humans remembered [[Chunking (psychology) | chunks of information]], interrelating bits using some scheme, and the limit applied to chunks. Miller himself saw no relationship among the disparate tasks of immediate memory and absolute judgment, but lumped them to fill a one-hour presentation. The results influenced the budding field of cognitive psychology.{{r|Cowen}}
  +
  +
=== WordNet ===
  +
  +
For many years starting from 1986, Miller directed the development of WordNet, a large computer-readable electronic reference usable in applications such as [[search engines]].{{r|Princeton}} Wordnet is a [[dictionary]] of words showing their linkages by meaning. Its fundamental building block is a [[synset]], which is a collection of synonyms representing a concept or idea. Words can be in multiple synsets. The entire class of synsets is grouped into [[nouns]], [[verbs]], [[adjectives]] and [[adverbs]] separately, with links existing only within these four major groups but not between them. Going beyond a [[thesaurus]], WordNet also included inter-word relationships such as part/whole relationships and hierarchies of inclusion.{{r|Shiffman}} Miller and colleagues had planned the tool to test [[psycholinguistic]] theories on how humans use and understand words.{{r|Sampson}} Miller also later worked closely with the developers at Simpli.com Inc., on a meaning-based keyword search engine based on WordNet.{{r|BeyondKeyword}}
  +
  +
=== Language psychology and computation ===
  +
  +
Miller is considered one of the founders of psycholinguistics, which links language and cognition in psychology, to analyze how people use and create language.{{r|NYTimes}} His 1951 book ''Language and Communication'' is considered seminal in the field.{{r|LATimes}} His later book, ''The Science of Words'' (1991) also focused on language psychology.{{r|Britannica}} He published papers along with [[Noam Chomsky]] on the mathematics and computational aspects of language and its [[syntax]], two new areas of study. Miller also researched how people understood words and sentences, the same problem faced by artificial [[speech-recognition]] technology. The book ''Plans and the Structure of Behavior'' (1960), written with Eugene Galanter and Karl H. Pribram, explored how humans plan and act, trying to extrapolate this to how a robot could be programmed to plan and do things.{{r|NYTimes}} Miller is also known for coining [[Miller's Law]]: "In order to understand what another person is saying, you must assume it is true and try to imagine what it could be true of".{{r|Banis}}
  +
  +
== Books ==
  +
  +
Miller authored several books, many considered the first major works in their respective fields.
  +
  +
=== ''Language and Communication'', 1951 ===
  +
  +
Miller's ''Language and Communication'' was one of the first significant texts in the study of language behavior. The book was a scientific study of language, emphasizing quantitative data, and was based on the mathematical model of [[Claude Shannon]]'s [[information theory]].{{r|Osgood}} It used a probabilistic model imposed on a learning-by-association scheme borrowed from behaviorism, with Miller not yet attached to a pure cognitive perspective.{{r|Smith}} The first part of the book reviewed information theory, the physiology and acoustics of phonetics, speech recognition and comprehension, and [[Statistical technique | statistical techniques]] to analyze language.{{r|Osgood}} The focus was more on speech generation than recognition.{{r|Smith}} The second part had the psychology: idiosyncratic differences across people in language use; developmental linguistics; the structure of word associations in people; use of [[symbol]]ism in language; and social aspects of language use.{{r|Osgood}}
  +
  +
Reviewing the book, [[Charles E. Osgood]] classified the book as a graduate-level text based more on objective facts than on theoretical constructs. He thought the book was verbose on some topics and too brief on others not directly related to the author's expertise area. He was also critical of Miller's use of simple, [[B.F. Skinner | Skinnerian]] single-stage [[stimulus-response theory | stimulus-response learning]] to explain human [[language acquisition]] and use. This approach, per Osgood, made it impossible to analyze the concept of meaning, and the idea of language consisting of representational signs. He did find the book objective in its emphasis on facts over theory, and depicting clearly application of information theory to psychology.{{r|Osgood}}
  +
  +
=== ''Plans and the Structure of Behavior'', 1960 ===
  +
  +
In ''Plans and the Structure of Behavior'', Miller and his co-authors tried to explain through an [[artificial-intelligence]] computational perspective how animals plan and act.{{r|Milner}} This was a radical break from behaviorism which explained behavior as a set or sequence of stimulus-response actions. The authors introduced a planning element controlling such actions.{{r|Wallace}} They saw all plans as being executed based on input using a stored or inherited information of the environment (called the image), and using a strategy called test-operate-test-exit (TOTE). The image was essentially a stored memory of all past context, akin to [[Edward C. Tolman | Tolman]]'s [[cognitive map]]. The TOTE strategy, in its initial test phase, compared the input against the image; if there was incongruity the operate function attempted to reduce it. This cycle would be repeated till the incongruity vanished, and then the exit function would be invoked, passing control to another TOTE unit in a hierarchically arranged scheme.{{r|Milner}}
  +
  +
[[Peter Milner]], in a review in the ''Canadian Journal of Psychology'', noted the book was short on concrete details on implementing the TOTE strategy. He also critically viewed the book as not being able to tie its model to details from [[neurophysiology]] at a [[molecular]] level. Per him, the book covered only the brain at the gross level of [[lesion studies]], showing that some of its regions could possibly implement some TOTE strategies, without giving a reader an indication as to how the region could implement the strategy.{{r|Milner}}
  +
  +
=== ''The Psychology of Communication'', 1967 ===
  +
  +
Miller's 1967 work, ''The Psychology of Communication'', was a collection of seven previously published articles. The first "Information and Memory" dealt with chunking, presenting the idea of separating physical length (the number of items presented to be learned) and psychological length (the number of ideas the recipient manages to categorize and summarize the items with). Capacity of short-term memory was measured in units of psychological length, arguing against a pure behaviorist interpretation since meaning of items, beyond [[reinforcement]] and [[punishment]], was central to psychological length.{{r|Phil}}
  +
  +
The second essay was the paper on magical number seven. The third, 'The human link in communication systems,' used information theory and its idea of [[channel capacity]] to analyze human perception [[Bandwidth (computing) | bandwidth]]. The essay concluded how much of what impinges on us we can absorb as knowledge was limited, for each property of the stimulus, to a handful of items.{{r|Phil}} The paper on "Psycholinguists" described how effort in both speaking or understanding a sentence was related to how much of self-reference to similar-structures-present-inside was there when the sentence was broken down into clauses and phrases.{{r|ABC}} The book, in general, used the Chomskian view of seeing language rules of grammar as having a biological basis—disproving the simple behaviorist idea that language performance improved with reinforcement—and using the tools of information and computation to place hypotheses on a sound theoretical framework and to analyze data practically and efficiently. Miller specifically addressed experimental data refuting the behaviorist framework at concept level in the field of language and cognition. He noted this only qualified behaviorism at the level of cognition, and did not overthrow it in other spheres of psychology.{{r|Phil}}
  +
  +
== Legacy ==
  +
  +
The Cognitive Neuroscience Society established a George A. Miller Prize in 1995 for contributions to the field.{{r|CogNeuro}} The American Psychological Association established a George A. Miller Award in 1995 for an outstanding article on general psychology.{{r|MillerAward}} From 1987 the department of psychology at Princeton University has presented the George A. Miller prize annually to the best interdisciplinary senior thesis in cognitive science.{{r|ThesisPrize}} The paper on the magical number seven continues to be cited by both the popular press to explain the liking for seven-digit phone numbers and to argue against nine-digit zip codes, and by academia, especially modern psychology, to highlight its break with the behaviorist paradigm.{{r|NYTimes}}
  +
  +
Miller was considered the 20th most eminent psychologist of the 20th century in a list{{r|Haggbloom}} republished by, among others, the American Psychological Association.{{r|Sidebar}}
  +
  +
== Awards ==
  +
  +
* Distinguished Scientific Contribution award from the American Psychological Association (APA) in 1963.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Distinguished Service award from the [[American Speech and Hearing Association]], 1976.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Award in Behavioral Sciences from the [[New York Academy of Sciences]], 1982.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* [[Guggenheim fellow]] in 1986.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* William James fellow of the American Psychological Society, 1989.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Hermann von Helmholtz award from the Cognitive Neurosciences Institute, 1989.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Gold Medal from the American Psychological Foundation in 1990.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* National Medal of Science from [[The White House]], 1991.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Louis E. Levy medal from the [[Franklin Institute]], 1991.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* [[International Prize (Fyssen Foundation) | International Prize from the Fyssen Foundation]], 1992.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* William James Book award from the [[Divisions of the American Psychological Association |APA Division of General Psychology]], 1993.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* John P. McGovern award from the [[American Association for the Advancement of Science]], 2000.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology award from the APA in 2003.{{r|Marquis}}
  +
* Antonio Zampolli Prize from the European Languages Research Association, 2006.{{r|LREC}}
   
   
Line 36: Line 99:
 
===Books===
 
===Books===
 
*Miller, G.A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K.H. (1960) Plans and the Structure of Behaviour, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
 
*Miller, G.A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K.H. (1960) Plans and the Structure of Behaviour, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  +
* {{cite book | title = Language and Communication | year = 1963 | authormask = 1 | publisher = [[McGraw Hill]] | asin =B000SRSOIK | author = George A. Miller }}
 
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = Mathematics and Psychology (Perspectives in Psychology) | year = 1965 | publisher = [[John Wiley & Sons]] | isbn = 9780471604082 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | editors = Frank Smith & George A. Miller | title = The genesis of language; a psycholinguistic approach; proceedings of a conference on language development in children | year = 1966 | publisher = [[The MIT Press]] | isbn = }}
  +
* {{cite book | title = The Genesis of Language: A Psycholinguistic Approach | year = 1968 | publisher = The MIT Press | isbn = 978-0262690225 | author = Frank Smith & George A Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | editor = George A. Miller | authormask = 1 | title = Communication, Language and Meaning (Perspectives in Psychology) | year = 1973 | publisher = [[Basic Books]] | isbn = 9780465128334)}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = Linguistic Communication: Perspectives for Research | publisher = [[International Reading Association]] | year = 1974 | isbn = 978-0872079298 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = The Psychology of Communication | year = 1975 | publisher = Harper Androw-1975 | isbn = 978-0465097074 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | title = Language and Perception | year = 1976 | publisher = [[Harvard University Press]] | isbn = 978-0674509474 | author = George A. Miller & Philip N Johnson-Laird }}
  +
* {{cite book | editors = Morris Halle, Joan Bresnan, & George A. Miller | title = Linguistic theory and psychological reality | year = 1978 | publisher = The MIT Press | isbn = 0262080958 }}
  +
* {{cite book | editors = George A. Miller & Elizabeth Lenneberg | title =Psychology and biology of language and thought : essays in honor of Eric Lenneberg | year = 1978 | publisher = [[Academic Press]] | isbn = 0124977502 }}
  +
* {{cite book | editors = Oscar Grusky & George A. Miller | title = Sociology of Organizations | year = 1981 | edition = 2 | publisher = [[Free Press (publisher) | Free Press]] | isbn = 9780029129302 }}
  +
* {{cite book | editors = Ned Joel Block, Jerrold J. Katz, George A. Miller | title = Readings in Philosophy of Psychology, Volume II | year = 1981 | publisher = Harvard University Press | isbn = 9780674748781 }}
  +
* {{cite book | title = Plans and the Structure of Behavior| year = 1986 | publisher = Adams Bannister Cox Pubs | isbn = 0937431001 | author = George A. Miller, Eugene Galanter, & Karl H. Pribram}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = Spontaneous Apprentices: Children and Language (Tree of Life) | year = 1987 | publisher = Seabury Press | isbn = 978-0816493302 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = Language and Speech | year = 1987 | publisher = [[W.H. Freeman and Company | W H Freeman & Co]] (sd) | isbn = 978-0716712978 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = Psychology: The Science of Mental Life | year = 1991 | publisher = [[Penguin Books Ltd]] | isbn = 9780140134896 | author = George A. Miller}}
  +
* {{cite book | authormask = 1 | title = The Science of Words | year = 1991 | publisher = W H Freeman & Co | isbn = 978-0716750277 | author = George A. Miller }}
   
 
===Book Chapters===
 
===Book Chapters===
Line 51: Line 130:
   
 
*Miller,G.A. and Isard, S. (1964). Free recall of self-embedded English sentences. Information and Control, 7(3):292-303.
 
*Miller,G.A. and Isard, S. (1964). Free recall of self-embedded English sentences. Information and Control, 7(3):292-303.
  +
   
   
 
==References==
 
==References==
  +
{{refs
<references/>
 
  +
| colwidth = 40em
  +
| refs =
   
  +
{{refn|name=ABC|
==External links==
 
  +
{{cite journal
*[http://www.cogsci.princeton.edu/%7Egeo/ George A. Miller's web page at Princeton University]
 
  +
| title = Georage A. Miller: The Psychology of Communication: Seven Essays: Review
*[http://ogch.tripod.com/pmphwgm.html Miller's "Psychology as a Means of Promoting Human Welfare," 1969 Presidential Address to the American Psychological Association]
 
  +
| journal = Journal of Business Communication
*[[George A. Miller|Miller, G.]] (1956): "[[The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two|The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information]]", ''Psychological Review'', vol. 63 pp. 81-97 [http://www.well.com/user/smalin/miller.html]
 
  +
| year = 1968
  +
| volume = 5
  +
| page = 54–55
  +
| doi = 10.1177/002194366800500208
  +
| issue = 2
  +
}}
  +
}}
   
  +
{{refn|name=APS|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| title = The Miller's tale
  +
| url = http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2006/june-06/the-millers-tale.html#.UJmnsq5Jt8E
  +
| author = Richard Hébert
  +
| date = July 2006
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| publisher = [[American Psychological Society]]
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Banis|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| title = BA 3320.Introduction to operations management
  +
| date = September 8, 2007
  +
| author = Robert J. Banis
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| url = http://www.umsl.edu/~banisr/3320/docs/tqm.ppt
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=BeyondKeyword|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://newsbreaks.infotoday.com/nbreader.asp?ArticleID=17858
  +
| title = Beyond keyword searching.Oingo and Simpli.com introduce meaning-based searching
  +
| date = December 20, 1999
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Britannica|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| publisher = Encyclopedia Britannica
  +
| url = http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/1382641/George-A-Miller
  +
| title = George A. Miller
  +
| accessdate = August 8, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=CMU|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://www.cmu.edu/cmnews/extra/030513_commence03.html
  +
| title = Preeminent leaders awarded honorary degrees
  +
| publisher = Carnegie Mellon University: Carnegie Mellon Today
  +
| date = May 13, 2003
  +
| accessdate = August 23, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=CogNeuro|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| title = George A. Miller Prize in cognitive neuroscience
  +
| publisher = Cognitive Neuroscience Society
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| url = http://cogneurosociety.org/annual-meeting/awards/george-a.-miller-prize-in-cognitive-neuroscience
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Cowen|
  +
{{Cite book
  +
| last = Cowan
  +
| first = N.
  +
| last2 = Morey
  +
| first2 = C. C.
  +
| last3 = Chen
  +
| first3 = Z.
  +
| chapter = The legend of the magical number seven
  +
| editor = Sergio Della Sala
  +
| title = Tall tales About the Brain: Separating Fact from Fiction
  +
| publisher = [[Oxford University Press]]
  +
| url = http://web.missouri.edu/~cowann/docs/articles/in%20press/Cowan%20et%20al,%20Tall%20tales%20in%20press.pdf
  +
| isbn = 978-0-19-856877-3
  +
| postscript = <!-- Bot inserted parameter. Either remove it; or change its value to "." for the cite to end in a ".", as necessary. -->{{inconsistent citations}}
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Haggbloom|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| last = Haggbloom
  +
| first = S.J.
  +
| last2 = et al.
  +
| year = 2002
  +
| volume = 6
  +
| pages = 139.52
  +
| title = The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century
  +
| journal = [[Review of General Psychology]]
  +
| issue = 2
  +
| doi = 10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139
  +
| first2 = Renee
  +
| last3 = Warnick
  +
| first3 = Jason E.
  +
| last4 = Jones
  +
| first4 = Vinessa K.
  +
| last5 = Yarbrough
  +
| first5 = Gary L.
  +
| last6 = Russell
  +
| first6 = Tenea M.
  +
| last7 = Borecky
  +
| first7 = Chris M.
  +
| last8 = McGahhey
  +
| first8 = Reagan
  +
| last9 = Powell
  +
| first9 = John L., III
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=HistoryOfAPS|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://www.psychologicalscience.org/anniversary/timeline.cfm
  +
| title = The history of APS: A timeline
  +
| accessdate = August 22, 2012
  +
| publisher = Association for Psychological Science
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Honorary|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://president.williams.edu/honorary-degrees/?ffp=2
  +
| title = Honorary degrees
  +
| publisher = Williams University: Office of the President
  +
| accessdate = August 23, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=LATimes|
  +
{{cite news
  +
| work = [[Los Angeles Times]]
  +
| date = August 6, 2012
  +
| accessdate = August 8, 2012
  +
| title = George A. Miller dies at 92; pioneer of cognitive psychology
  +
| author = Thomas M. Haugh II
  +
| url = http://www.latimes.com/news/obituaries/la-me-george-miller-20120806,0,6416902.story
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Lindzey|
  +
{{cite book
  +
| author = Lindzey, G.
  +
|year = 1989
  +
| title = A History of psychology in autobiography.
  +
| publisher = [[Stanford University Press]]
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=LREC|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://www.lrec-conf.org/lrec2006/article.php3?id_article=45
  +
| title = LREC 2006 Conference: Winners of the 2006 Antonio Zampolli Prize
  +
| publisher = LREC
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| year = 2006
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Marquis|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| publisher = [[Marquis Who's Who]]
  +
| accessdate = August 7, 2012
  +
| title = Profie details: George Armitage Miller
  +
| url = http://search.marquiswhoswho.com/profile/100002520559
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=MillerAward|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| title = George A. Miller Award for an Outstanding Recent Article on General Psychology
  +
| url = http://www.apa.org/about/awards/div-1-miller.aspx
  +
| publisher = American Psychological Association
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Milner|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| last = Milner
  +
| first = P. M.
  +
| doi = 10.1037/h0083461
  +
| journal = Canadian Journal of Psychology
  +
| year = 1960
  +
| volume = 14
  +
| issue = 4
  +
| pages = 281.82
  +
| title = Review of Plans and the Structure of Behavior
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=NYTimes|
  +
{{cite news
  +
| work = [[New York Times]]
  +
| accessdate = August 8, 2012
  +
| title = George A. Miller, a pioneer in cognitive psychology, is dead at 92
  +
| date = August 1, 2012
  +
| url = http://www.nytimes.com/2012/08/02/us/george-a-miller-cognitive-psychology-pioneer-dies-at-92.html?pagewanted=all
  +
| author = Paul Vitello
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Osgood|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| journal = Psychological Bulletin
  +
| volume = 49
  +
| issue = 4
  +
| year = 1952
  +
| pages = 361.363
  +
| last = Osgood
  +
| first = C. E.
  +
| doi = 10.1037/h0052690
  +
| title = Language and communication
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Phil|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| journal = The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science
  +
| last = Bunge
  +
| first = Mario
  +
| title = Reviews: George A. Miller: The Psychology of Communication
  +
| year = 1968
  +
| volume = 18
  +
| issue = 4
  +
| pages = 350.52
  +
| doi = 10.1093/bjps/18.4.350
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Princeton|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://www.princeton.edu/main/news/archive/S34/33/10E46/index.xml?section=topstories
  +
| title = George Miller, Princeton psychology professor and cognitive pioneer, dies
  +
| author = Michael Hotchkiss
  +
| date = July 26, 2012
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=PsycNet|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| title = Gold medal awards for life achievement: George Armitage Miller
  +
| journal = [[American Psychologist]]
  +
| volume = 46
  +
| issue = 4
  +
| pages = 326.328
  +
| doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.326
  +
| year = 1991
  +
| last1 = No Authorship Indicated
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Sampson|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| journal = International Journal of Lexicography
  +
| last = Sampson
  +
| first = Geoffrey
  +
| volume = 13
  +
| issue = 1
  +
| pages = 54.9
  +
| year = 2000
  +
| doi = 10.1093/ijl/13.1.54
  +
| title = Reviews
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Shiffman|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| title = Daniel Shiffman: WordNet
  +
| author = Daniel Shiffman
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| url = http://www.shiffman.net/teaching/a2z/wordnet/
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Sidebar|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| journal = [[Monitor on Psychology]]
  +
| title = Sidebar: Eminent psychologists of the 20th century
  +
| year = 2002
  +
| volume = 33
  +
| issue = 7
  +
| page = 29
  +
| url = http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug02/eminent.aspx
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Smith|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| title = Language and Communication
  +
| journal = Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology
  +
| last = Smith
  +
| first = S.M.
  +
| volume = 47
  +
| issue = 3
  +
| year = 1952
  +
| pages = 734–735
  +
| doi = 10.1037/h0052503
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=ThesisPrize|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| url = http://psych.princeton.edu/psychology/related/gmiller/index.php
  +
| title = George A. Miller Sr. Thesis Prize
  +
| accessdate = August 10, 2012
  +
| publisher = Department of Psychology, Princeton University
  +
| year = 2004
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=Wallace|
  +
{{cite journal
  +
| title = Plans and the structure of behavior: Review
  +
| journal = American Anthropologist
  +
| last = Wallace
  +
| first = A.F.C
  +
| volume = 62
  +
| issue = 6
  +
| year = 1960
  +
| pages = 1065–1067
  +
| doi = 10.1525/aa.1960.62.6.02a00190
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
{{refn|name=WashingtonPost|
  +
{{cite web
  +
| work = [[Washington Post]]
  +
|title = George A. Miller; helped transform the study of psychology; at 92
  +
| author = Emily Langer
  +
| date = August 3, 2012
  +
| accessdate = August 8, 2012
  +
| url = http://articles.boston.com/2012-08-03/news/33000658_1_psychology-miller-cognitive-science/2
  +
}}
  +
}}
  +
  +
}}
  +
  +
== External links ==
  +
  +
*[http://isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k69509&pageid=icb.page334500&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent698262&view=watch.do&viewParam_entry=35358&state=maximize#a_icb_pagecontent698262 2007 discussion on the cognitive revolution, with Chomsky, Bruner, Pinker and others: Part I]
  +
*[http://isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k69509&pageid=icb.page334500&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent698262&view=watch.do&viewParam_entry=35369&state=maximize#a_icb_pagecontent698262 2007 discussion on the cognitive revolution, with Chomsky, Bruner, Pinker and others: Part II]
  +
*[http://isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k69509&pageid=icb.page334500&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent698262&view=watch.do&viewParam_entry=35374&state=maximize#a_icb_pagecontent698262 2007 discussion on the cognitive revolution, with Chomsky, Bruner, Pinker and others: Part III]
  +
*[http://isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k69509&pageid=icb.page334500&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent698262&view=watch.do&viewParam_entry=35382&state=maximize#a_icb_pagecontent698262 2007 discussion on the cognitive revolution, with Chomsky, Bruner, Pinker and others: Part IV]
  +
  +
*[http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Miller/ Classics in the history of psychology: The seven plus/minus two paper]
  +
*[http://www.kurzweilai.net/george-a-miller Bio on Kurtzweil.net]
  +
*[http://www.cs.princeton.edu/~rit/geo/ Old faculty page]
  +
*[http://www.questia.com/read/57282051/communication-language-and-meaning-psychological Communication, Language, and Meaning (edited by Miller)]
  +
*[http://www.edwardtufte.com/bboard/q-and-a-fetch-msg?msg_id=0000U6 A blog with links to discussions on the seven-plus-minus-two paper]
  +
*[http://neurotree.org/neurotree/tree.php?pid=597 Neurotree: Miller's academic genealogy]
  +
{{s-start}}
  +
{{s-edu}}
  +
{{succession box
  +
|before=[[Abraham Maslow]]
  +
|title=78th President of the [[American Psychological Association]]
  +
|years=1968–1969
  +
|after=[[George Albee|George W. Albee]]
  +
}}
  +
{{s-end}}
  +
{{Psychology}}
  +
{{Winners of the National Medal of Science|behav-social}}
  +
  +
{{Authority control|VIAF=29595505}}
  +
<!-- Metadata: see [[Wikipedia:Persondata]] -->
  +
{{Persondata
  +
|NAME= Miller, George A.
  +
|ALTERNATIVE NAMES= Miller, George Armitage
  +
|SHORT DESCRIPTION= American psychologist, cognitive scientist
  +
|DATE OF BIRTH= February 3, 1920
  +
|PLACE OF BIRTH= [[Charleston, West Virginia|Charleston]], [[West Virginia]]
  +
|DATE OF DEATH= July 22, 2012
  +
|PLACE OF DEATH=
  +
}}
 
{{DEFAULTSORT:Miller, George A.}}
 
{{DEFAULTSORT:Miller, George A.}}
  +
[[Category:1920 births]]
 
 
[[Category:American psychologists]]
 
[[Category:American psychologists]]
  +
[[Category:Cognitive psychologists]]
  +
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]]
 
[[Category:Memory researchers]]
 
[[Category:Memory researchers]]
 
[[Category:Harvard University faculty]]
 
[[Category:Harvard University faculty]]
[[Category:Cognitive scientists]]
+
[[Category:CASBS at Stanford fellows]]
  +
[[Category:National Medal of Science laureates]]
  +
[[Category:Fellows of the Society of Experimental Psychologists]]
  +
[[Category:Fellows of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences]]
  +
[[Category:Fellows of the American Association for the Advancement of Science]]
  +
[[Category:Members of the United States National Academy of Sciences]]
  +
[[Category:Guggenheim Fellows]]
  +
[[Category:Consciousness researchers and theorists]]
  +
[[Category:Princeton University faculty]]
  +
[[Category:Massachusetts Institute of Technology faculty]]
  +
[[Category:University of Alabama faculty]]
  +
[[Category:Harvard University alumni]]
  +
[[Category:University of Alabama alumni]]
  +
[[Category:Charleston High School (West Virginia) alumni]]
  +
[[Category:Scientists from West Virginia]]
  +
[[Category:People from Charleston, West Virginia]]
  +
[[Category:1920 births]]
  +
[[Category:2012 deaths]]
  +
[[Category:Presidents of the American Psychological Association]]
   
 
<!--
 
<!--

Latest revision as of 15:58, 12 August 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


George Armitage Miller (February 3, 1920 – July 22, 2012) was one of the founders of the cognitive psychology field. He also contributed to the birth of psycholinguistics and cognitive science in general. Miller wrote several books and directed the development of WordNet, an online word-linkage database usable by computer programs. He authored the paper, "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two," which experimentally discovered an average limit of seven for human short-term memory capacity. This paper is frequently cited in both psychology and the wider culture. He also won awards such as the National Medal of Science.

Miller started his education focusing on speech and language and published papers on these topics, focusing on mathematical, computational and psychological aspects of the field. He started his career at a time when the reigning theory in psychology was behaviorism, which eschewed any attempt to study mental processes and focused only on observable behavior. Working mostly at Harvard University, MIT and Princeton University, Miller introduced experimental techniques to study the psychology of mental processes, by linking the new field of cognitive psychology to the broader area of cognitive science, including computation theory and linguistics. He collaborated and co-authored work with other figures in cognitive science and psycholinguistics, such as Noam Chomsky. For moving psychology into the realm of mental processes and for aligning that move with information theory, computation theory, and linguistics, Miller is considered one of the great twentieth-century psychologists.

Life and education

Miller was born on February 3, 1920, at Charleston, West Virginia, the son of an executive at a steel company,[1] George E. Miller, and Florence (Armitage) Miller.[2] Soon after, his parents divorced. He grew up with only his mother during the Great Depression, attended public school, and graduated from Charleston High School in 1937. He relocated with his mother and stepfather to Washington D.C., and was at George Washington University for a year. His family practiced Christian Science, which required turning to prayer, rather than medical science, for healing. After his stepfather was transferred to Birmingham, Alabama, Miller transferred to the University of Alabama.[3]

He received his bachelor's degree in history and speech in 1940, and a master's in speech in 1941 from the University of Alabama. He had taken courses in phonetics, voice science, and speech pathology . Membership in the Drama club fostered his interest in courses in the Speech Department. He was also influenced by Professor Donald Ramsdell, who introduced him both to psychology, and, indirectly through a seminar, to his future wife Katherine James.[3] They married on November 29, 1939. Katherine died in January, 1996.[2][4] He married Margaret Ferguson Skutch Page in 2008.[2][5]

Miller taught the course "Introduction to Psychology" at Alabama for two years. He enrolled in the Ph.D. program in psychology at Harvard University in 1943, after coming to the university in 1942.[3] He received his doctorate in 1946 from Harvard's Psycho-Acoustic Laboratory, under the supervision of Stanley Smith Stevens, researching military voice communications for the Army Signal Corps during World War II. His doctorate thesis, "The Optimal Design of Jamming Signals," was classified top secret by the US Army.[3] He was on the faculty at Harvard, MIT and Rockefeller University before settling at Princeton in 1979.[4][6]

In his later years, Miller enjoyed playing golf.[1] He died in 2012 at his home in Plainsboro, New Jersey of complications of pneumonia and dementia.[4] At the time of his passing, he was survived by his wife Margaret; the children from his first marriage: son Donnally James and daughter Nancy Saunders;two stepsons, David Skutch and Christopher Skutch; and three grandchildren: Gavin Murray-Miller, Morgan Murray-Miller and Nathaniel James Miller.[5][7]

Career

After receiving his doctorate, Miller stayed as a research fellow at Harvard, to continue his research on speech and hearing. He was appointed assistant professor of psychology in 1948. The course he developed on language and communication would eventually lead to his first major book, Language and communication (1951). He took a sabbatical in 1950, and spent a year as a visiting fellow at the Institute for Advanced Study, Princeton, to pursue his interest in mathematics. Miller befriended J. Robert Oppenheimer, with whom he played squash. [8] In 1951, Miller joined MIT as an associate professor of psychology. He led the psychology group at MIT Lincoln Lab. He worked on voice communication and human engineering, whereupon he identified the minimal voice features of speech required for it to be intelligible. Based on this work, in 1955, he was invited to a talk at the Eastern Psychological Association. That presentation, "The magical number seven, plus or minus two", was later published as a paper which went on to be a legendary one in cognitive psychology.[3]

Miller moved back to Harvard as a tenured associate professor in 1955 and became a full professor in 1958, expanding his research into how language affects human cognition.[3] At the university he met a young Noam Chomsky, another of the founders of cognitive science. They spent a summer together at Stanford training the faculty, and their two families shared a house. In 1958–59, Miller took leave to join the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences at Palo Alto, California, (now at Stanford University).[9] There he collaborated with Eugene Galanter and Karl Pribram on the book Plans and the Structure of Behavior. In 1960, along with Jerome S. Bruner,[1][3] he co-founded the Center for Cognitive Sciences at Harvard.[3] The cognitive term was a break from the then-dominant school of behaviorism, which insisted cognition was not fit for scientific study.[1] The center attracted such notable visitors as Jean Piaget, Alexander Luria and Chomsky.[9] Miller then became the chair of the psychology department.[3] Miller was instrumental at the time for recruiting Timothy Leary to teach at Harvard. Miller knew Leary from the University of Alabama, where Miller was teaching psychology and Leary graduated with an undergraduate degree from the department.

In 1967, Miller taught at Rockefeller University for a year, as a visiting professor,[2] researching how lexical memory was stored, but declined a position as professor in experimental psychology.[3] A new president's selection at Rockefeller made him leave.[9] In 1979, he moved to Princeton as the James S. McDonnell Distinguished University Professor of Psychology.[3] In 1986, he helped in founding the Cognitive Science Laboratory at Princeton. Eventually he became a professor emeritus and senior research psychologist at Princeton. He also directed the McDonnell-Pew Program in Cognitive Science.[3]

Miller had honorary doctorates from the University of Sussex (1984), Columbia University (1980), Yale University (1979), Catholic University of Louvain (1978),[3] Carnegie Mellon University (in humane letters, 2003),[10] and an honorary DSC from Williams College (2000).[11] He was elected to the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1957,[7] the National Academy of Sciences in 1962,[7] the presidency of the Eastern Psychological Association in 1962,[3] the presidency of the American Psychological Association in 1969,[3] and to the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1985.[7] Miller was the keynote speaker at the first convention of the Association for Psychological Science in 1989.[12] He was a Fulbright research fellow at Oxford University in 1964–65,[9] and in 1991, received the National Medal of Science.[7]

Major contributions

Miller's career started during the reign of behaviorism in psychology. Behaviorists questioned whether mental thought processes were fit for scientific study, not being observable. They focused on working with responses to stimuli, particularly among other animals. Miller disagreed. He, Jerome Bruner, and Noam Chomsky are considered the founders of the field of Cognitive Psychology that replaced behaviorism as the framework for analyzing the mind.[4]

Working memory

From the days of William James, psychologists had the idea memory consisted of short-term and long-term memory. While short-term memory was expected to be limited, its exact limits were not known. In 1956, Miller would quantify its capacity limit in the paper "The magical number seven, plus or minus two". He tested immediate memory via tasks such as asking a person to repeat a set of digits presented; absolute judgment by presenting a stimulus and a label, and asking them to recall the label later; and span of attention by asking them to count things in a group of more than a few items quickly. For all three cases, Miller found the average limit to be seven items. He had mixed feelings about the focus on his work on the exact number seven for quantifying short-term memory, and felt it had been misquoted often. He stated, introducing the paper on the research for the first time, that he was being persecuted by an integer.[1] Miller also found humans remembered chunks of information, interrelating bits using some scheme, and the limit applied to chunks. Miller himself saw no relationship among the disparate tasks of immediate memory and absolute judgment, but lumped them to fill a one-hour presentation. The results influenced the budding field of cognitive psychology.[13]

WordNet

For many years starting from 1986, Miller directed the development of WordNet, a large computer-readable electronic reference usable in applications such as search engines.[7] Wordnet is a dictionary of words showing their linkages by meaning. Its fundamental building block is a synset, which is a collection of synonyms representing a concept or idea. Words can be in multiple synsets. The entire class of synsets is grouped into nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs separately, with links existing only within these four major groups but not between them. Going beyond a thesaurus, WordNet also included inter-word relationships such as part/whole relationships and hierarchies of inclusion.[14] Miller and colleagues had planned the tool to test psycholinguistic theories on how humans use and understand words.[15] Miller also later worked closely with the developers at Simpli.com Inc., on a meaning-based keyword search engine based on WordNet.[16]

Language psychology and computation

Miller is considered one of the founders of psycholinguistics, which links language and cognition in psychology, to analyze how people use and create language.[1] His 1951 book Language and Communication is considered seminal in the field.[4] His later book, The Science of Words (1991) also focused on language psychology.[17] He published papers along with Noam Chomsky on the mathematics and computational aspects of language and its syntax, two new areas of study. Miller also researched how people understood words and sentences, the same problem faced by artificial speech-recognition technology. The book Plans and the Structure of Behavior (1960), written with Eugene Galanter and Karl H. Pribram, explored how humans plan and act, trying to extrapolate this to how a robot could be programmed to plan and do things.[1] Miller is also known for coining Miller's Law: "In order to understand what another person is saying, you must assume it is true and try to imagine what it could be true of".[18]

Books

Miller authored several books, many considered the first major works in their respective fields.

Language and Communication, 1951

Miller's Language and Communication was one of the first significant texts in the study of language behavior. The book was a scientific study of language, emphasizing quantitative data, and was based on the mathematical model of Claude Shannon's information theory.[19] It used a probabilistic model imposed on a learning-by-association scheme borrowed from behaviorism, with Miller not yet attached to a pure cognitive perspective.[20] The first part of the book reviewed information theory, the physiology and acoustics of phonetics, speech recognition and comprehension, and statistical techniques to analyze language.[19] The focus was more on speech generation than recognition.[20] The second part had the psychology: idiosyncratic differences across people in language use; developmental linguistics; the structure of word associations in people; use of symbolism in language; and social aspects of language use.[19]

Reviewing the book, Charles E. Osgood classified the book as a graduate-level text based more on objective facts than on theoretical constructs. He thought the book was verbose on some topics and too brief on others not directly related to the author's expertise area. He was also critical of Miller's use of simple, Skinnerian single-stage stimulus-response learning to explain human language acquisition and use. This approach, per Osgood, made it impossible to analyze the concept of meaning, and the idea of language consisting of representational signs. He did find the book objective in its emphasis on facts over theory, and depicting clearly application of information theory to psychology.[19]

Plans and the Structure of Behavior, 1960

In Plans and the Structure of Behavior, Miller and his co-authors tried to explain through an artificial-intelligence computational perspective how animals plan and act.[21] This was a radical break from behaviorism which explained behavior as a set or sequence of stimulus-response actions. The authors introduced a planning element controlling such actions.[22] They saw all plans as being executed based on input using a stored or inherited information of the environment (called the image), and using a strategy called test-operate-test-exit (TOTE). The image was essentially a stored memory of all past context, akin to Tolman's cognitive map. The TOTE strategy, in its initial test phase, compared the input against the image; if there was incongruity the operate function attempted to reduce it. This cycle would be repeated till the incongruity vanished, and then the exit function would be invoked, passing control to another TOTE unit in a hierarchically arranged scheme.[21]

Peter Milner, in a review in the Canadian Journal of Psychology, noted the book was short on concrete details on implementing the TOTE strategy. He also critically viewed the book as not being able to tie its model to details from neurophysiology at a molecular level. Per him, the book covered only the brain at the gross level of lesion studies, showing that some of its regions could possibly implement some TOTE strategies, without giving a reader an indication as to how the region could implement the strategy.[21]

The Psychology of Communication, 1967

Miller's 1967 work, The Psychology of Communication, was a collection of seven previously published articles. The first "Information and Memory" dealt with chunking, presenting the idea of separating physical length (the number of items presented to be learned) and psychological length (the number of ideas the recipient manages to categorize and summarize the items with). Capacity of short-term memory was measured in units of psychological length, arguing against a pure behaviorist interpretation since meaning of items, beyond reinforcement and punishment, was central to psychological length.[23]

The second essay was the paper on magical number seven. The third, 'The human link in communication systems,' used information theory and its idea of channel capacity to analyze human perception bandwidth. The essay concluded how much of what impinges on us we can absorb as knowledge was limited, for each property of the stimulus, to a handful of items.[23] The paper on "Psycholinguists" described how effort in both speaking or understanding a sentence was related to how much of self-reference to similar-structures-present-inside was there when the sentence was broken down into clauses and phrases.[24] The book, in general, used the Chomskian view of seeing language rules of grammar as having a biological basis—disproving the simple behaviorist idea that language performance improved with reinforcement—and using the tools of information and computation to place hypotheses on a sound theoretical framework and to analyze data practically and efficiently. Miller specifically addressed experimental data refuting the behaviorist framework at concept level in the field of language and cognition. He noted this only qualified behaviorism at the level of cognition, and did not overthrow it in other spheres of psychology.[23]

Legacy

The Cognitive Neuroscience Society established a George A. Miller Prize in 1995 for contributions to the field.[25] The American Psychological Association established a George A. Miller Award in 1995 for an outstanding article on general psychology.[26] From 1987 the department of psychology at Princeton University has presented the George A. Miller prize annually to the best interdisciplinary senior thesis in cognitive science.[27] The paper on the magical number seven continues to be cited by both the popular press to explain the liking for seven-digit phone numbers and to argue against nine-digit zip codes, and by academia, especially modern psychology, to highlight its break with the behaviorist paradigm.[1]

Miller was considered the 20th most eminent psychologist of the 20th century in a list[28] republished by, among others, the American Psychological Association.[29]

Awards

  • Distinguished Scientific Contribution award from the American Psychological Association (APA) in 1963.[2]
  • Distinguished Service award from the American Speech and Hearing Association, 1976.[2]
  • Award in Behavioral Sciences from the New York Academy of Sciences, 1982.[2]
  • Guggenheim fellow in 1986.[2]
  • William James fellow of the American Psychological Society, 1989.[2]
  • Hermann von Helmholtz award from the Cognitive Neurosciences Institute, 1989.[2]
  • Gold Medal from the American Psychological Foundation in 1990.[2]
  • National Medal of Science from The White House, 1991.[2]
  • Louis E. Levy medal from the Franklin Institute, 1991.[2]
  • International Prize from the Fyssen Foundation, 1992.[2]
  • William James Book award from the APA Division of General Psychology, 1993.[2]
  • John P. McGovern award from the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 2000.[2]
  • Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology award from the APA in 2003.[2]
  • Antonio Zampolli Prize from the European Languages Research Association, 2006.[30]



Preceded by:
Abraham Maslow
George A. Miller elected APA President
1969
Succeeded by:
George W. Albee


See also

Publications

Books

  • Miller, G.A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K.H. (1960) Plans and the Structure of Behaviour, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • George A. Miller (1963). Language and Communication, McGraw Hill.
  • George A. Miller (1965). Mathematics and Psychology (Perspectives in Psychology), John Wiley & Sons.
  • (1966) The genesis of language; a psycholinguistic approach; proceedings of a conference on language development in children, The MIT Press.
  • Frank Smith & George A Miller (1968). The Genesis of Language: A Psycholinguistic Approach, The MIT Press.
  • (1973) George A. Miller Communication, Language and Meaning (Perspectives in Psychology), Basic Books.
  • George A. Miller (1974). Linguistic Communication: Perspectives for Research, International Reading Association.
  • George A. Miller (1975). The Psychology of Communication, Harper Androw-1975.
  • George A. Miller & Philip N Johnson-Laird (1976). Language and Perception, Harvard University Press.
  • (1978) Linguistic theory and psychological reality, The MIT Press.
  • (1978) Psychology and biology of language and thought : essays in honor of Eric Lenneberg, Academic Press.
  • (1981) Sociology of Organizations, 2, Free Press.
  • (1981) Readings in Philosophy of Psychology, Volume II, Harvard University Press.
  • George A. Miller, Eugene Galanter, & Karl H. Pribram (1986). Plans and the Structure of Behavior, Adams Bannister Cox Pubs.
  • George A. Miller (1987). Spontaneous Apprentices: Children and Language (Tree of Life), Seabury Press.
  • George A. Miller (1987). Language and Speech, W H Freeman & Co (sd).
  • George A. Miller (1991). Psychology: The Science of Mental Life, Penguin Books Ltd.
  • George A. Miller (1991). The Science of Words, W H Freeman & Co.

Book Chapters

Papers

  • Miller, G.A. (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two; some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review 63; 81-97.
  • Miller,G.A. and Friedman, E.A. (1957)The reconstruction of mutilated English texts. Information and Control, 1(1):38-55,
  • Chomsky, N. and Miller,G.A. (1958) Finite state languages. Information and Control, 1(2):91-112.
  • Miller,G.A. Newman,E.B. and Friedman, E.A. (1958). Length-frequency statistics for written English. Information and Control, 1(4):370-389.
  • Miller,G.A. and Isard, S. (1964). Free recall of self-embedded English sentences. Information and Control, 7(3):292-303.


References

  1. REDIRECT Template:Reflist


External links

Template:S-start Template:S-edu |- style="text-align: center;" |- style="text-align: center;" |width="30%" align="center" rowspan="1"|Preceded by:
Abraham Maslow |width="40%" style="text-align: center;" rowspan="1"|78th President of the American Psychological Association
1968–1969 |width="30%" align="center" rowspan="1"|Succeeded by:
George W. Albee |- Template:S-end

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Psychology: Debates · Journals · Psychologists


Template:Winners of the National Medal of Science



This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).
  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 1.6 1.7 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named NYTimes
  2. 2.00 2.01 2.02 2.03 2.04 2.05 2.06 2.07 2.08 2.09 2.10 2.11 2.12 2.13 2.14 2.15 2.16 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Marquis
  3. 3.00 3.01 3.02 3.03 3.04 3.05 3.06 3.07 3.08 3.09 3.10 3.11 3.12 3.13 3.14 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named PsycNet
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named LATimes
  5. 5.0 5.1 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named WashingtonPost
  6. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Lindzey
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 7.4 7.5 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Princeton
  8. Pais A. (2006). J. Robert Oppenheimer: A life, Oxford University Press.
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named APS
  10. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named CMU
  11. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Honorary
  12. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named HistoryOfAPS
  13. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Cowen
  14. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Shiffman
  15. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Sampson
  16. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named BeyondKeyword
  17. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Britannica
  18. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Banis
  19. 19.0 19.1 19.2 19.3 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Osgood
  20. 20.0 20.1 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Smith
  21. 21.0 21.1 21.2 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Milner
  22. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Wallace
  23. 23.0 23.1 23.2 Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Phil
  24. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named ABC
  25. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named CogNeuro
  26. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named MillerAward
  27. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named ThesisPrize
  28. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Haggbloom
  29. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named Sidebar
  30. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; no text was provided for refs named LREC