Psychology Wiki
Register
Advertisement

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


Distance perception is a form of spatial perception using primarily distance receptors.



See also[]

References[]

  • Abraham, A. (1994). Reproduction of angular distances in the congenitally blind: Psycho-Lingua Vol 24(2) Jul 1994, 99-104.
  • Abrams, R. A., Van Dillen, L., & Stemmons, V. (1994). Multiple sources of spatial information for aimed limb movements. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Acredolo, C. (1989). Assessing children's understanding of time, speed and distance interrelations. Oxford, England: North-Holland.
  • Acredolo, C., & Schmid, J. (1981). The understanding of relative speeds, distances, and durations of movement: Developmental Psychology Vol 17(4) Jul 1981, 490-493.
  • Actis-Grosso, R., & Stucchi, N. (2003). Shifting the start: Backward mislocation of the initial position of a motion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 29(3) Jun 2003, 675-691.
  • Adam, J. J., Mol, R., Pratt, J., & Fischer, M. H. (2006). Moving Farther but Faster: An Exception to Fitts's Law: Psychological Science Vol 17(9) Sep 2006, 794-798.
  • Adelson, D. (1963). The relational influence of size on judgments of distance: Journal of General Psychology 69(2) 1963, 319-333.
  • Agnew, N. M., Pyke, S., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1966). Absolute judgment of distance as a function of induced muscle tension, exposure time, and feedback: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 71(5) May 1966, 649-654.
  • Aleksander, A. K. (1996). Glare mitigation in night driving using partially tinted lenses. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Allen, B. P. (1999). Shadows as sources of cues for distance of shadow-casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 89(2) Oct 1999, 571-584.
  • Allen, B. P. (2000). Angles of shadows as cues for judging the distance of shadow casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 90(3,Pt1) Jun 2000, 864-866.
  • Allen, B. P. (2001). Lighting position and judgments of distance of shadow-casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 93(1) Aug 2001, 127-130.
  • Allen, G. L. (1988). The acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity: Psychological Research Vol 50(3) 1988, 183-190.
  • Allen, G. L., & Rashotte, M. A. (2006). Training metric accuracy in distance estimation skill: Pictures versus words: Applied Cognitive Psychology Vol 20(2) Mar 2006, 171-186.
  • Allen, G. L., Siegel, A. W., & Rosinski, R. R. (1978). The role of perceptual context in structuring spatial knowledge: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 4(6) Nov 1978, 617-630.
  • Alyan, S. H., & Jander, R. (1997). Interplay of directional navigation mechanisms as a function of near-goal distance: Experiments with the house mouse: Behavioural Processes Vol 41(3) Dec 1997, 245-255.
  • Anderson, J. R., Montant, M., & Schmitt, D. (1996). Rhesus monkeys fail to use gaze direction as an experimenter-given cue in an object-choice task: Behavioural Processes Vol 37(1) Aug 1996, 47-55.
  • Ankomah, P. K., Crompton, J. L., & Baker, D. (1996). Influence of cognitive distance in vacation choice: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 23(1) 1996, 138-150.
  • Anooshian, L. J., & Wilson, K. L. (1977). Distance distortions in memory for spatial locations: Child Development Vol 48(4) Dec 1977, 1704-1707.
  • Armstrong, L., & Marks, L. E. (1999). Haptic perception of linear extent: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(6) Aug 1999, 1211-1226.
  • Ashley, A., & Carlson, L. A. (2007). Encoding direction when interpreting proximal terms: Language and Cognitive Processes Vol 22(7) Nov 2007, 1-24.
  • Ashley, M. L. (1903). An Investigation of the Process of Judgment as Involved in Estimating Distances: Psychological Review Vol 10(3) May 1903, 283-295.
  • Ashmead, D. H., Davis, D. L., & Northington, A. (1995). Contribution of listeners' approaching motion to auditory distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 21(2) Apr 1995, 239-256.
  • Atkins, J. E., Jacobs, R. A., & Knill, D. C. (2003). Experience-dependent visual cue recalibration based on discrepancies between visual and haptic percepts: Vision Research Vol 43(25) Nov 2003, 2603-2613.
  • Avraamides, M. N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2004). Functional Equivalence of Spatial Representations Derived From Vision and Language: Evidence From Allocentric Judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 30(4) Jul 2004, 801-814.
  • Aznar-Casanova, J. A., Matsushima, E. H., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2006). One-Dimensional and Multi-Dimensional Studies of the Exocentric Distance Estimates in Frontoparallel Plane, Virtual Space, and Outdoor Open Field: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 273-284.
  • Backus, B. T., & Banks, M. S. (1999). Estimator reliability and distance scaling in stereoscopic slant perception: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 217-242.
  • Baden, D. A., Warwick-Evans, L., & Lakomy, J. (2004). Am I nearly there? The effect of anticipated running distance on perceived exertion and attentional focus: Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology Vol 26(2) Jun 2004, 215-231.
  • Bahcall, D. O., & Kowler, E. (1999). Attentional interference at small spatial separations: Vision Research Vol 39(1) Jan 1999, 71-86.
  • Bailes, S. M. (1996). Effects of processing time and stimulus density on apparent width of the Oppel-Kundt illusion. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Baird, J. C. (1963). Retinal and assumed size cues as determinants of size and distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 66(2) Aug 1963, 155-162.
  • Baird, J. C. (1982). The moon illusion: II. A reference theory: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 111(3) Sep 1982, 304-315.
  • Baird, J. C., & Wagner, M. (1982). The moon illusion: I. How high is the sky? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 111(3) Sep 1982, 296-303.
  • Baird, J. C., Wagner, M., & Fuld, K. (1990). A simple but powerful theory of the moon illusion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 16(3) Aug 1990, 675-677.
  • Baker, C. A. (1954). Interpolation accuracy as a function of visual angle between scale marks: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(6) Jun 1954, 433-436.
  • Balakrishnan, J. D., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Lederman, S. J. (1989). Length distortion of temporally extended visual displays: Similarity to haptic spatial perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 46(4) Oct 1989, 387-394.
  • Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., & Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements: Vision Research Vol 36(3) Feb 1996, 431-443.
  • Banton, P., Thompson, P., & Quinlan, P. T. (2001). Effect of geometric field of view on stereoscopic spatial judgments: Human Factors Vol 43(3) Fal 2001, 405-414.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Haptically perceiving size at a distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 122(3) Sep 1993, 347-370.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1995). Does perceived size depend on perceived distance? An argument from extended haptic perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 57(2) Feb 1995, 216-224.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). Anisotropy in the extended haptic perception of longitudinal distances: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(8) Nov 1999, 1522-1536.
  • Barber, A. V. (1990). Visual mechanisms and predictors of far field visual task performance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1988). Young children's conception of distance: Developmental Psychology Vol 24(4) Jul 1988, 532-541.
  • Baumberger, B., & Fluckiger, M. (2004). The development of distance estimation in optic flow: Perception Vol 33(9) 2004, 1081-1099.
  • Baumberger, B., Fluckiger, M., Paquette, M., Bergeron, J., & Delorme, A. (2005). Perception of relative distance in a driving simulator: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 47(3) Sep 2005, 230-237.
  • Beebe-Center, J. G., Mead, L. C., Wagoner, K. S., & Hoffman, A. C. (1945). Visual acuity and distance of observation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 35(6) Dec 1945, 473-484.
  • Begault, D. R. (1991). Preferred sound intensity increase for sensation of half distance: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 72(3, Pt 1) Jun 1991, 1019-1029.
  • Beggs, W. D., & et al. (1972). The accuracy of non-visual aiming: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 24(4) Nov 1972, 515-523.
  • Berberian, B., Sarrazin, J.-C., & Giraudo, M.-D. (2007). Tau and Kappa effects: The case of space-like-extent frequencies: Ecological Psychology Vol 19(3) 2007, 215-238.
  • Bereby-Meyer, Y., Leiser, D., & Meyer, J. (1999). Perception of artificial stereoscopic stimuli from an incorrect viewing point: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(8) Nov 1999, 1555-1563.
  • Berkeley, G. (1948). An essay toward a new theory of vision, 1709. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Bertamini, M., & Parks, T. E. (2005). On what people know about images on mirrors: Cognition Vol 98(1) Nov 2005, 85-104.
  • Bertamini, M., Yang, T. L., & Proffitt, D. R. (1998). Relative size perception at a distance is best at eye level: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(4) May 1998, 673-682.
  • Berthoz, A., Israel, I., Georges-Francois, P., Grasso, R., & et al. (1995). Spatial memory of body linear displacement: What is being stored? : Science Vol 269(5220) Jul 1995, 95-98.
  • Berti, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Coding near and far space. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2005). The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(5) Jul 2005, 802-815.
  • Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2006). The ground dominance effect in the perception of relative distance in 3-D scenes is mainly due to characteristics of the ground surface: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(8) Nov 2006, 1297-1309.
  • Bigel, M. G., & Ellard, C. G. (2000). The contribution of nonvisual information to simple place navigation and distance estimation: An examination of path integration: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale Vol 54(3) Sep 2000, 172-185.
  • Bigelow, A. E. (1996). Blind and sighted children's spatial knowledge of their home environments: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 19(4) Dec 1996, 797-816.
  • Bilandzic, H. (2006). The Perception of Distance in the Cultivation Process: A Theoretical Consideration of the Relationship Between Television Content, Processing Experience, and Perceived Distance: Communication Theory Vol 16(3) Aug 2006, 333-355.
  • Bilderback, L. G., Taylor, R. E., & Thor, D. H. (1964). Distance perception in darkness: Science 145(Whole No 3629) 1964, 294-295.
  • Bingham, G. P. (2005). Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision: Ecological Psychology Vol 17(2) 2005, 55-74.
  • Bingham, G. P., Crowell, J. A., & Todd, J. T. (2004). Distortions of distance and shape are not produced by a single continuous transformation of reach space: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(1) Jan 2004, 152-169.
  • Bingham, G. P., McConnell, D. S., & Muchisky, M. M. (2001). Commentary on Jacobs and Michaels (2001): Calibration and perceptual learning in event perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(3) Apr 2001, 572-574.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Pagano, C. C. (1998). The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: Monocular distance perception to guide reaching: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 24(1) Feb 1998, 145-168.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Stassen, M. G. (1994). Monocular egocentric distance information generated by head movement: Ecological Psychology Vol 6(3) 1994, 219-238.
  • Bingham, G. P., Zaal, F., Robin, D., & Shull, J. A. (2000). Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 26(4) Aug 2000, 1436-1460.
  • Birnbaum, M. H., Anderson, C. J., & Hynan, L. G. (1989). Two operations for "ratios" and "differences" of distances on the mental map: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(4) Nov 1989, 785-796.
  • Block, M. E. (1993). Can children with mild mental retardation perceive affordances for action? : Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly Vol 10(2) Apr 1993, 137-145.
  • Bondarko, B. M. (1989). Comparison of metrics of spatial characteristics of images with errors in recognition of the images: Sensory Systems Vol 3(1) Jan-Mar 1989, 44-54.
  • Bondarko, V. M., & Danilova, M. V. (1996). Crowding effects with different surroundings and interstimulus distances: Sensory Systems Vol 10(3) Jul-Sept 1996, 170-177.
  • Bongers, R. M., Michaels, C. F., & Smitsman, A. W. (2004). Variations of tool and task characteristics reveal that tool-use postures are anticipated: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 36(3) Sep 2004, 305-315.
  • Bongers, R. M., Smitsman, A. W., & Michaels, C. F. (2004). Geometric, But Not Kinetic, Properties of Tools Affect the Affordances Perceived by Toddlers: Ecological Psychology Vol 16(2) Apr 2004, 129-158.
  • Bonnet, C. (1964). Perceived speed and the relation between space covered and time required: L'annee Psychologique 64(1) 1964, 47-60.
  • Bonnet, C., & Pouthas, V. (1972). Apparent size and duration of a movement after-effect: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 24(3) Aug 1972, 275-281.
  • Bootsma, R. J., Bakker, F. C., Van Snippenberg, F. J., & Tdlohreg, C. W. (1992). The effects of anxiety on perceiving the reachability of passing objects: Ecological Psychology Vol 4(1) 1992, 1-16.
  • Boring, E. G. (1964). Size constancy in a picture: American Journal of Psychology 77(3) 1964, 494-498.
  • Bourdy, C., Cottin, F., & Monot, A. (1991). Errors in distance appreciation and binocular night vision: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 11(4) Oct 1991, 340-349.
  • Boyd, J. N., III. (2000). A theory of the interaction of affect and cognition: It's about time. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Brackhane, R., Behr, M., & Tschernuth, E. (1987). Absolute judgments on geographical distances: Psychologische Beitrage Vol 29(4) 1987, 653-661.
  • Bradshaw, M. F., & Glennerster, A. (2006). Stereoscopic acuity and observation distance: Spatial Vision Vol 19(1) 2006, 21-36.
  • Braithwaite, V. A., & DePerera, T. B. (2006). Short-range orientation in fish: How fish map space: Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology Vol 39(1) Mar 2006, 37-47.
  • Braunstein, M. L. (1972). Perception of rotation in depth: A process model: Psychological Review Vol 79(6) Nov 1972, 510-524.
  • Bremner, J. G., Johnson, S. P., Slater, A., Mason, U., Foster, K., Cheshire, A., et al. (2005). Conditions for Young Infants' Perception of Object Trajectories: Child Development Vol 76(5) Sep-Oct 2005, 1029-1043.
  • Brenner, E., & Landy, M. S. (1999). Interaction between the perceived shape of two objects: Vision Research Vol 39(23) Nov 1999, 3834-3848.
  • Brenner, E., Smeets, J. B. J., & Landy, M. S. (2001). How vertical disparities assist judgements of distance: Vision Research Vol 41(25-26) 2001, 3455-3465.
  • Brenner, E., & van Damme, W. J. M. (1999). Perceived distance, shape and size: Vision Research Vol 39(5) Mar 1999, 975-986.
  • Brown, C. A. (2004). The perception of auditory distance in reverberant rooms. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Brown, W. (1905). Review of Fresh Light on Molyneux' Problem -- Dr. Ramsay's Case: Psychological Bulletin Vol 2(6) Jun 1905, 217-218.
  • Brungart, D. S., Durlach, N. I., & Rabinowitz, W. M. (1999). Auditory localization of nearby sources: II. Localization of a broadband source: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(4, Pt 1) Oct 1999, 1956-1968.
  • Brungart, D. S., & Scott, K. R. (2001). The effects of production and presentation level on the auditory distance perception of speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(1) Jul 2001, 425-440.
  • Bruno, P., & van den Berg, A. V. (1997). Relative orientation of primary positions of the two eyes: Vision Research Vol 37(7) Apr 1997, 935-947.
  • Bubb, H. (2004). Challenges in the application of anthropometric measurements: Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science Vol 5(2) Mar-Apr 2004, 154-168.
  • Buchner, A., Brandt, M., Bell, R., & Weise, J. (2006). Car Backlight Position and Fog Density Bias Observer-Car Distance Estimates and Time-to-Collision Judgments: Human Factors Vol 48(2) Sum 2006, 300-317.
  • Bulatov, A., & Bertulis, A. (2005). Distracting effects in length matching: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 65(3) 2005, 265-269.
  • Buresova, O., Homuta, L., Krekule, I., & Bures, J. (1988). Does nondirectional signalization of target distance contribute to navigation in the Morris water maze? : Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 49(2) Mar 1988, 240-248.
  • Burris, C. T., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Distorted distance estimation induced by a self-relevant national boundary: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Vol 41(3) May 2005, 305-312.
  • Burton, G. (2000). The role of the sound of tapping for nonvisual judgment of gap crossability: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 26(3) Jun 2000, 900-916.
  • Burton, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Attentionally splitting the mass distribution of hand-held rods: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 50(2) Aug 1991, 129-140.
  • Burtt, H. E. (1918). A pneumograph for inspiration-expiration ratios: Psychological Bulletin Vol 15(10) Oct 1918, 325-328.
  • Byrne, R. W. (1979). Memory for urban geography: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(1) Feb 1979, 147-154.
  • Byrne, R. W., & Salter, E. (1983). Distances and directions in the cognitive maps of the blind: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 37(2) Jun 1983, 293-299.
  • Cabe, P. A., Wright, C. D., & Wright, M. A. (2003). Descartes's blind man revisited: Bimanual triangulation of distance using static hand-held rods: American Journal of Psychology Vol 116(1) Spr 2003, 71-98.
  • Campbell, P. F., Fein, G. G., & Schwartz, S. S. (1991). The effects of Logo experience on first-grade children's ability to estimate distance: Journal of Educational Computing Research Vol 7(3) 1991, 331-349.
  • Candy, T. R., & Bharadwaj, S. R. (2007). The stability of steady state accommodation in human infants: Journal of Vision Vol 7(11) 2007, 1-16.
  • Carbon, C.-C., & Leder, H. (2005). The Wall inside the brain: Overestimation of distances crossing the former Iron Curtain: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(4) Aug 2005, 746-750.
  • Carey, D. P., Dijkerman, H. C., & Milner, A. D. (1998). Perception and action in depth: Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal Vol 7(3) Sep 1998, 438-453.
  • Carey, D. P., Goodale, M. A., & Sprowl, E. G. (1990). Blindsight in rodents: The use of a "high-level" distance cue in gerbils with lesions of primary visual cortex: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 38(3) May 1990, 283-289.
  • Carlson, L. A., & Covey, E. S. (2005). How far is near? Inferring distance from spatial descriptions: Language and Cognitive Processes Vol 20(5) Oct 2005, 617-631.
  • Carlson, L. A., & Van Deman, S. R. (2004). The space in spatial language: Journal of Memory and Language Vol 51(3) Oct 2004, 418-436.
  • Carr, H. (1908). Voluntary control of the distance location of the visual field: Psychological Review Vol 15(3) May 1908, 139-149.
  • Carson, B. (2007). What are musical paradox and illusion? : American Journal of Psychology Vol 120(1) Spr 2007, 123-140.
  • Castro, C., Martinez, C., Tornay, F. J., Fernandez, P. G., & Martos, F. J. (2005). Vehicle distance estimations in nighttime driving: a real-setting study: Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour Vol 8(1) Jan 2005, 31-45.
  • Cavallo, V. (1989). The role of visual information in the anticipation of a collision: Psychologie Francaise Vol 34(1) Mar 1989, 33-40.
  • Cavallo, V., Colomb, M., & Dore, J. (2001). Distance perception of vehicle rear lights in fog: Human Factors Vol 43(3) Fal 2001, 442-451.
  • Cavallo, V., & Laurent, M. (1988). Visual information and skill level in time-to-collision estimation: Perception Vol 17(5) 1988, 623-632.
  • Celebrini, S., Stricanne, B., Thorpe, S., & et al. (1996). Neural processing of stereopsis as a function of viewing distance in primate visual cortical area V1. 700 M 710 Trotter:Yves: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 76(5) Nov 1996, 2872-2885.
  • Chakraborty, A. (1964). Error in reproduction of tactually perceived distances between two aesthesiometric points greater than the two-point R.L. values: Journal of Psychological Researches 8(2) 1964, 86-88.
  • Chamizo, V. D., & Rodrigo, T. (2004). Effect of absolute spatial proximity between a landmark and a goal: Learning and Motivation Vol 35(2) May 2004, 102-114.
  • Chan, T.-c., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Perceiving the vertical distances of surfaces by means of a hand-held probe: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 17(2) May 1991, 347-358.
  • Chapanis, A., & Leyzorek, M. (1950). Accuracy of visual interpolation between scale markers as a function of the number assigned to the scale interval: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 40(5) Oct 1950, 655-667.
  • Chaston, A. N. (2002). Time and distance estimation while walking in outdoor environments: Effects of route and environmental characteristics. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Chemero, A., Klein, C., & Cordeiro, W. (2003). Events as Changes in the Layout of Affordances: Ecological Psychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2003, 19-28.
  • Cheng, K. (1989). The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 15(4) Oct 1989, 366-375.
  • Cheng, K. (1998). Distances and directions are computed separately by honeybees in landmark-based search: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 26(4) Nov 1998, 455-468.
  • Cheng, K., & Sherry, D. F. (1992). Landmark-based spatial memory in birds (Parus atricapillus and Columba livia): The use of edges and distances to represent spatial positions: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 106(4) Dec 1992, 331-341.
  • Cheng, K., Srinivasan, M. V., & Zhang, S. W. (1999). Error is proportional to distance measured by honeybees: Weber's law in the odometer: Animal Cognition Vol 2(1) 1999, 11-16.
  • Chi-Cheng, C., Jui-Hsiang, P., Chung-Hsien, L., & Yun-Chiu, F. (1964). Distance perception during postural changes: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 4 1964, 303-313.
  • Chieffi, S., & Allport, D. A. (1997). Independent coding of target distance and direction in visuo-spatial working memory: Psychological Research Vol 60(4) 1997, 244-250.
  • Ching, C.-C., & Liu, W.-M. (1965). Observation-distance and interval traversed by stimulus as variables in constancy of visual movement: Acta Psychologica Sinica 32(3) 1965, 248-258.
  • Cho, R. Y., Yang, V., & Hallett, P. E. (2000). Reliability and dimensionality of judgments of visually textured materials: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(4) May 2000, 735-752.
  • Cholewiak, R. W. (1999). The perception of tactile distance: Influences of body site, space, and time: Perception Vol 28(7) 1999, 851-875.
  • Churchill, A. V. (1959). Optimal interval length for visual interpolation: The effect of viewing distance: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 43(2) Apr 1959, 125-128.
  • Churchill, A. V. (1960). The effect of pointer width and mark width on the accuracy of visual interpolation: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 44(5) Oct 1960, 315-318.
  • Clements, F. E. (1928). The effect of time on distance discrimination in the albino rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 8(4) Oct 1928, 317-324.
  • Clifton, R. K., Perris, E. E., & Bullinger, A. (1991). Infants' perception of auditory space: Developmental Psychology Vol 27(2) Mar 1991, 187-197.
  • Coello, Y., Magne, P., & Plenacoste, P. (2000). The contribution of retinal signal to the specification of target distance in a visuo-manual task: Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition No 3 2000, 71-86.
  • Cohen, A. A. (1988). Illusory conjunctions as a function of focus of attention and distance between objects: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Coleman, P. D. (1963). An analysis of cues to auditory depth perception in free space: Psychological Bulletin Vol 60(3) May 1963, 302-315.
  • Collett, T. S. (2002). Insect vision: Controlling actions through optic flow: Current Biology Vol 12(18) Sep 2002, R615-R617.
  • Collett, T. S., & Collett, M. (2000). Path integration in insects: Current Opinion in Neurobiology Vol 10(6) Dec 2000, 757-762.
  • Connelly, M. J., Conaglen, H. M., Parsonson, B. S., & Isler, R. B. (1998). Child pedestrians' crossing gap thresholds: Accident Analysis & Prevention Vol 30(4) Jul 1998, 443-453.
  • Coombs, S. (1999). Signal detection theory, lateral-line excitation patterns and prey capture behaviour of motted sculpin: Animal Behaviour Vol 58(2) Aug 1999, 421-430.
  • Corlett, J. T., Byblow, W., & Taylor, B. (1990). The effect of perceived locomotor constraints on distance estimation: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 22(3) Sep 1990, 347-360.
  • Coules, J. (1955). Effect of photometric brightness on judgments of distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(1) Jul 1955, 19-25.
  • Creem-Regehr, S. H., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., & Thompson, W. B. (2005). The influence of restricted viewing conditions on egocentric distance perception: Implications for real and virtual indoor environments: Perception Vol 34(2) 2005, 191 -204.
  • Crompton, A. (2006). Perceived Distance in the City as a Function of Time: Environment and Behavior Vol 38(2) Mar 2006, 173-182.
  • Crompton, A., & Brown, F. (2006). Distance Estimation in a Small-Scale Environment: Environment and Behavior Vol 38(5) Sep 2006, 656-666.
  • Crompton, J. L., & Kim, S.-S. (2001). The influence of cognitive distance in vacation choice: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 28(2) Apr 2001, 512-515.
  • Crowell, J. A., & Banks, M. S. (1996). Ideal observer for heading judgments: Vision Research Vol 36(3) Feb 1996, 471-490.
  • Curiel, J. M., & Radvansky, G. A. (2004). The accuracy of spatial information from temporally and spatially organized mental maps: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 11(2) Apr 2004, 314-319.
  • Da Silva, J. A., Matsushima, E. H., Aznar-Casanova, J. A., & Ribeiro-Filho, N. P. (2006). Distance perception in a natural outdoor setting: Is there a developmental trend to overconstancy? : The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 285-294.
  • Daniels, V., & Gordon, I. E. (1993). Occlusion and the distortion of alignment in three-dimensional space: Perception Vol 22(9) 1993, 1037-1044.
  • Dawkins, M. S. (2002). What are birds looking at? Head movements and eye use in chickens: Animal Behaviour Vol 63(5) May 2002, 991-998.
  • De Camp, J. E. (1920). Relative distance as a factor in the white rat's selection of a path: Psychobiology Vol 2(3) Jun 1920, 245-253.
  • de Castro, E. M., de Paula, A. I., Tavares, C. P., & Moraes, R. (2004). Effects of Navigation Training on Orientation Tasks by Blind Adults: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 17(2) 2004, 199-210.
  • De Jong, M., Wagenaar, W. A., Wolters, G., & Verstijnen, I. M. (2005). Familiar face recognition as a function of distance and illumination: A practical tool for use in the courtroom: Psychology, Crime & Law Vol 11(1) Mar 2005, 87-97.
  • Dean, G. M., Dewhurst, S. A., Morris, P. E., & Whittaker, A. (2005). Selective Interference With the Use of Visual Images in the Symbolic Distance Paradigm: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 31(5) Sep 2005, 1043-1068.
  • Dees, J. W. (1966). Accuracy of absolute visual distance and size estimation in space as a function of stereopsis and motion parallax: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 72(3) Sep 1966, 466-476.
  • Degelman, D., & Rosinski, R. R. (1979). Motion parallax and children's distance perception: Developmental Psychology Vol 15(2) Mar 1979, 147-152.
  • DeLucia, P. R. (1999). Critique of "distance estimation with night vision goggles: A little feedback goes a long way": Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 507-510.
  • DeLucia, P. R., & Novak, J. B. (1997). Judgments of relative time-to-contact of more than two approaching objects: Toward a method: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 59(6) Aug 1997, 913-928.
  • Denis, M. (2008). Assessing the symbolic distance effect in mental images constructed from verbal descriptions: A study of individual differences in the mental comparison of distances: Acta Psychologica Vol 127(1) Jan 2008, 197-210.
  • Denis, M., & Cocude, M. (1989). Scanning visual images generated from verbal descriptions: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 1(4) Dec 1989, 293-307.
  • Denisova, K., Singh, M., & Kowler, E. (2006). The role of part structure in the perceptual localization of a shape: Perception Vol 35(8) 2006, 1073-1087.
  • Depy, D., Fagot, J., & Vauclair, J. (1998). Comparative assessment of distance processing and hemispheric specialization in humans and baboons (Papio papio): Brain and Cognition Vol 38(2) Nov 1998, 165-182.
  • Deregowski, J. B., & Bentley, A. M. (1988). Distance constancy in Bushmen: An exploratory study. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff Publishing.
  • Diewert, G. L., & Roy, E. A. (1978). Coding strategy for memory of movement extent information: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 4(6) Nov 1978, 666-675.
  • Dijkstra, T. M., Gielen, C. C., & Melis, B. J. (1992). Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene: Human Movement Science Vol 11(1-2) Feb 1992, 195-203.
  • Dobbins, A. C., Jeo, R. M., Fiser, J., & Allman, J. M. (1998). Distance modulation of neural activity in the visual cortex: Science Vol 281(5376) Jul 1998, 552-555.
  • Dodge, R. (1899). Review of Die Pracision der Blickbewegung und der Localisation an der Netzhautperipherie: Psychological Review Vol 6(6) Nov 1899, 668-669.
  • Dodge, R. (1904). Blickrichtung und Grossenschatzung: Psychological Bulletin Vol 1(4) Mar 1904, 131.
  • Doherty, S., Gale, N., Pellegrino, J. W., & Golledge, R. (1989). Children's versus adults' knowledge of places and distances in a familiar neighborhood environment: Children's Environments Quarterly Vol 6(2-3) Sum-Fal 1989, 65-71.
  • Domini, F., & Caudek, C. (2003). 3-D structure perceived from dyamic information: A new theory: Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol 7(10) Oct 2003, 444-449.
  • Donlan, C., Bishop, D. V. M., & Hitch, G. J. (1998). Magnitude comparisons by children with specific language impairments: Evidence of unimpaired symbolic processing: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders Vol 33(2) Apr-Jun 1998, 149-160.
  • Dopkins, S. (2005). Access to dimensional values can be unselective during early perceptual processing: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(3) Apr 2005, 513-530.
  • Dornfeldt, K. (1996). Pigeon homing in the meteorological and solar-geomagnetic environment: What pigeon race data say: Ethology Vol 102(5) May 1996, 413-435.
  • Doumen, M. J. A., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2005). Visual space under free viewing conditions: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(7) Oct 2005, 1177-1189.
  • Doumen, M. J. A., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2006). Horizontal-vertical anisotropy in visual space: Acta Psychologica Vol 123(3) Nov 2006, 219-239.
  • Dumas, C., St-Louis, B., & Routhier, L. (2006). Decision Making and Interference in the Domestic Cat (Felis catus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 120(4) Nov 2006, 367-377.
  • Dunlap, K. (1900). Studies from the psychological laboratory of the University of California. II. The effect of imperceptible shadows on the judgment of distance: Psychological Review Vol 7(5) Sep 1900, 435-453.
  • Dunlap, K. (1906). Review of Malebranche's Theory of the Perception of Distance and Magnitude: Psychological Bulletin Vol 3(4) Apr 1906, 143.
  • Durier, V., & Rivault, C. (1999). Path integration in cockroach larvae, Blatella germanica (L.) (insect: Dictyoptera): Direction and distance estimation: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 27(1) Feb 1999, 108-118.
  • Dutke, S., Ribback, S., & Wagner, B. (2003). Direct Distance, Route Distance, and Implied Temporal Distance: Effects on the Accessibility of Situation Model Components: Scientific Studies of Reading Vol 7(4) Oct 2003, 357-381.
  • Dzhafarov, E. N. (2002). Multidimensional Fechnerian Scaling: Probability-distance hypothesis: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 46(3) Jun 2002, 352-374.
  • Ebenholtz, S. M., & Ebenholtz, J. M. (2003). Distance perception for points at equiconvergence and equidistance loci: Perception Vol 32(6) 2003, 707-716.
  • Ebersbach, M., & Resing, W. C. M. (2007). Shedding new light on an old problem: The estimation of shadow sizes in children and adults: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 97(4) Aug 2007, 265-285.
  • Econopouly, J. (1996). Binocular disparities and KDE are combined to rescale binocular disparities. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ehrlich, J. A. (1999). The effect of viewing conditions on visual stress, sickness, and distance estimation in a helmet-mounted display. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ellard, C. G., & Bigel, M. G. (1996). The use of local features and global spatial context for object recognition in a visuomotor task in the Mongolian gerbil: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 24(3) Aug 1996, 310-317.
  • Ellard, C. G., Chapman, D. G., & Cameron, K. A. (1991). Calibration of retinal image size with distance in the Mongolian gerbil: Rapid adjustment of calibrations in different contexts: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 49(1) Jan 1991, 38-42.
  • Ellard, C. G., & Sharma, L. (1996). The effects of cortical lesions on recognition of object context in a visumotor task in the Mongolian gerbil: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 82(1) Dec 1996, 13-22.
  • Elliott, D. (1987). The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 19(4) Dec 1987, 476-485.
  • Enns, J. T., & Girgus, J. S. (1985). Perceptual grouping and spatial distortion: A developmental study: Developmental Psychology Vol 21(2) Mar 1985, 241-246.
  • Enright, J. T. (1989). Manipulating stereopsis and vergence in an outdoor setting: Moon, sky and horizon: Vision Research Vol 29(12) 1989, 1815-1824.
  • Epstein, W. (1963). Attitudes of judgment and the size-distance invariance hypothesis: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 66(1) Jul 1963, 78-83.
  • Epstein, W. (1964). Effective difference in influence of relative size on perceived relative distance: Perceptual and Motor Skills 18(2) 1964, 383-384.
  • Epstein, W. (1965). Nonrelational judgment of size and distance: American Journal of Psychology 78(1) 1965, 120-123.
  • Epstein, W., Park, J., & Casey, A. (1961). The current status of the size-distance hypotheses: Psychological Bulletin Vol 58(6) Nov 1961, 491-514.
  • Ewert, K. K. (1998). Time judgments in uniform motion events: The effects of visual distance and visual speed information. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Fabricius, W. V., & Wellman, H. M. (1993). Two roads diverged: Young children's ability to judge distance: Child Development Vol 64(2) Apr 1993, 399-414.
  • Fagard, J. (2000). Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behavior of 5-to 12-month old human infants grasping objects of different sizes: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 23(3-4) 2000, 317-329.
  • Faro, D., Leclerc, F., & Hastie, R. (2005). Perceived Causality as a Cue to Temporal Distance: Psychological Science Vol 16(9) Sep 2005, 673-677.
  • Feldman, J. (1997). Curvilinearity, covariance and regularity in perceptual groups: Vision Research Vol 37(20) Oct 1997, 2835-2848.
  • Feria, C. S. (2004). Effects of surface texture discontinuities and surface curvature on distance perception. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Feria, C. S., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2003). Judging distance across texture discontinuities: Perception Vol 32(12) 2003, 1423-1440.
  • Ferris, S. H. (1972). Motion parallax and absolute distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 258-263.
  • Field, J. (1976). Relation of young infants' reaching behavior to stimulus distance and solidity: Developmental Psychology Vol 12(5) Sep 1976, 444-448.
  • Fitt, A. B. (1917). The estimation of distances by sight and passive touch: Some investigations into the evolution of the sense of touch: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 2(4) Aug 1917, 264-288.
  • Flamme, G. A. (2000). The relationships between direction and distance hearing and other auditory traits: A multitrait-multimethod evaluation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Foley, J. M. (1966). Locus of perceived equidistance as a function of viewing distance: Journal of the Optical Society of America 56(6) 1966, 822-827.
  • Foley, J. M. (1980). Binocular distance perception: Psychological Review Vol 87(5) Sep 1980, 411-434.
  • Foley, J. M. (1985). Binocular distance perception: Egocentric distance tasks: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 11(2) Apr 1985, 133-149.
  • Foley, J. M. (1993). Stereoscopic distance perception. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
  • Foley, J. M., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2004). Visual perception of extent and the geometry of visual space: Vision Research Vol 44(2) Jan 2004, 147-156.
  • Fontes, S., Fontes, A. I., & Merino, J. M. (1990). Comparison of two psychophysical techniques on Stevens's function: Revista Latinoamericana de Psicologia Vol 22(3) 1990, 373-383.
  • Fontes, S., Garriga, A. J., & Barbero, I. (1993). Psychophysical functions of distance estimation between two lines: Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada Vol 46(1) Jan 1993, -.
  • Foreman, N., Sandamas, G., & Newson, D. (2004). Distance Underestimation in Virtual Space Is Sensitive to Gender But Not Activity-Passivity or Mode of Interaction: CyberPsychology & Behavior Vol 7(4) Aug 2004, 451-457.
  • Foster, P. S., Crucian, G. P., Drago, V., Burks, D. W., Mielke, J., Shenal, B. V., et al. (2007). The effects of movement direction and hemispace on estimates of distance traveled: Brain and Cognition Vol 64(2) Jul 2007, 184-188.
  • Fotheringham, J. R., Martin, P. R., & Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Song transmission and auditory perception of distance in wood warblers (Parulinae): Animal Behaviour Vol 53(6) Jun 1997, 1271-1285.
  • Fox, B. H. (1951). Figural after-effects: "satiation" and adaptation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 42(5) Nov 1951, 317-326.
  • Freiberg, K., Tually, K., & Crassini, B. (2001). Use of an auditory looming task to test infants' sensitivity to sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue: British Journal of Developmental Psychology Vol 19(Pt1) Mar 2001, 1-10.
  • Frenz, H., Bremmer, F., & Lappe, M. (2003). Discrimination of travel distances from 'situated' optic flow: Vision Research Vol 43(20) Sep 2003, 2173-2183.
  • Frenz, H., & Lappe, M. (2005). Absolute travel distance from optic flow: Vision Research Vol 45(13) Jun 2005, 1679-1692.
  • Frenz, H., & Lappe, M. (2006). Visual Distance Estimation in Static Compared to Moving Virtual Scenes: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 321-331.
  • Frenz, H., Lappe, M., Kolesnik, M., & Buhrmann, T. (2007). Estimation of travel distance from visual motion in virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 4(1) Jan 2007, 1-18.
  • Friedenberg, J., & Liby, B. (2002). Perception of two-body center of mass: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(4) May 2002, 531-539.
  • Friedenberg, J. D. (1996). Grouping by proximity and similarity in dot lattices: Effects of luminance and color. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Friedman, A., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Global-Scale Location and Distance Estimates: Common Representations and Strategies in Absolute and Relative Judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 32(2) Mar 2006, 333-346.
  • Friedman, A., & Pilon, D. J. (1994). Effects of distance between objects and distance from the vertical axis on shape identity judgments: Memory & Cognition Vol 22(5) Sep 1994, 552-564.
  • Fry, G. A. (1952). Gilinsky's equations for perceived size and distance: Psychological Review Vol 59(3) May 1952, 244-245.
  • Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events: Psychological Science Vol 17(4) Apr 2006, 278-282.
  • Fujita, N., Shiihara, T., & Noji, T. (2005). Accurate motion-produced distance and direction under systematically distorted perception: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 47(3) Sep 2005, 163-174.
  • Fukusima, S. S., & Aparecido da Silva, J. (1994). Judgment of egocentric distances in a full cue indoor setting: No effect of instructions: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 10(2) May-Aug 1994, 159-166.
  • Fukusima, S. S., da Silva, J. A., & Ribeiro Filho, N. P. (1990). Perception of size and distance of familiar objects: Toward a bifactorial theory: Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia Vol 42(2) Mar-May 1990, 3-25.
  • Fukusima, S. S., Loomis, J. M., & Da Silva, J. A. (1997). Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 86-100.
  • Fukusima, S. S., Ribeiro, G., & da Silva, J. A. (1988). Calculation of Stevens' psychophysical function by microcomputer: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 4(2) May-Aug 1988, 96-101.
  • Garling, T. (1989). The role of cognitive maps in spatial decisions: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 9(4) Dec 1989, 269-278.
  • Garling, T., & Loukopoulos, P. (2007). Choice of Driving Versus Walking Related to Cognitive Distance. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Garrett, S., Barac-Cikoja, D., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). A parallel between visual and haptic perception of size at a distance: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(1) 1996, 25-42.
  • Genovesio, A., & Ferraina, S. (2004). Integration of Retinal Disparity and Fixation-Distance Related Signals Toward an Egocentric Coding of Distance in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of Primates: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 91(6) Jun 2004, 2670-2684.
  • Gentilucci, M., & Negrotti, A. (1994). Dissociation between perception and visuomotor transformation during reproduction of remembered distances: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 72(4) Oct 1994, 2026-2030.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The Effect of Training on Absolute Estimation of Distance over the Ground: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 48(6) Dec 1954, 473-482.
  • Gibson, E. J., Bergman, R., & Purdy, J. (1955). The effect of prior training with a scale of distance on absolute and relative judgments of distance over ground: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(2) Aug 1955, 97-105.
  • Gilhousen, H. C. (1933). Fixation of excess distance patterns in the white rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 16(1) Aug 1933, 1-23.
  • Gilhousen, H. C. (1941). Final goal versus subgoal distance discrimination: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 31(1) Feb 1941, 35-42.
  • Gilinsky, A. S. (1951). Perceived size and distance in visual space: Psychological Review Vol 58(6) Nov 1951, 460-482.
  • Gillam, B. (1995). The perception of spatial layout from static optical information. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Gillam, B., Chambers, D., & Lawergren, B. (1988). The role of vertical disparity in the scaling of stereoscopic depth perception: An empirical and theoretical study: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 44(5) Nov 1988, 473-483.
  • Gillan, D. J., Schmidt, W., & Hanowski, R. J. (1999). The effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on map reading: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(6) Aug 1999, 1154-1167.
  • Girgus, J. S., Coren, S., & Agdern, M. (1972). The interrelationship between the Ebbinghaus and Delboeuf illusions: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 453-455.
  • Gogel, W. C. (1954). Perception of the relative distance position of objects as a function of other objects in the field: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(5) May 1954, 335-342.
  • Gogel, W. C. (1997). The organization of perceived space. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Gogel, W. C., Wist, E. K., & Harker, G. S. (1963). A test of the invariance of the ratio of perceived size to perceived distance: American Journal of Psychology 76(4) 1963, 537-553.
  • Goldfarb, L., & Tzelgov, J. (2005). Is size perception based on monocular distance cues computed automatically? : Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(4) Aug 2005, 751-754.
  • Goldman, M., Skolnick, A. J., Hernandez, T. P., & Tobach, E. (1992). Distance perception in the spiny mouse Acomys cahirinus: Vertical jumping: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 75(3, Pt 1) Dec 1992, 883-895.
  • Goodale, M. A., Ellard, C. G., & Booth, L. (1990). The role of image size and retinal motion in the computation of absolute distance by the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): Vision Research Vol 30(3) 1990, 399-413.
  • Gordon, A. D., Jupp, P. E., & Byrne, R. W. (1989). The construction and assessment of mental maps: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology Vol 42(2) Nov 1989, 169-182.
  • Gordon, I. E., Day, R. H., & Dorwood, F. (1993). A Muller-Lyer-type illusion of egocentric distance with 3D convex and concave objects: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology Vol 34(2) Jun 1993, 97-106.
  • Graham, B. J., & Northmore, D. P. M. (2007). A spiking neural network model of midbrain visuomotor mechanisms that avoids objects by estimating size and distance monocularly: Neurocomputing: An International Journal Vol 70(10-12) Jun 2007, 1983-1987.
  • Grant, V. W. (1942). Accommodation and convergence in visual space perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(2) Aug 1942, 89-104.
  • Gray, E. R., & Spetch, M. L. (2006). Pigeons Encode Absolute Distance but Relational Direction From Landmarks and Walls: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 32(4) Oct 2006, 474-480.
  • Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1999). Do monocular time-to-collision estimates necessarily involve perceived distance? : Perception Vol 28(10) 1999, 1257-1264.
  • Greenberg, G., McCarthy, T., & Bergamo, P. (1990). Depth/distance perception in gerbils and spiny mice: Ecological considerations: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 3(2) Win 1990, 131-136.
  • Greene, E., & Fraser, S. C. (2002). Observation distance and recognition of photographs of celebrities' faces: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(2) Oct 2002, 637-651.
  • Greene, R. T., Lawson, R. B., & Godek, C. L. (1972). The Ponzo illusion in stereoscopic space: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 358-364.
  • Greenhut, A. (1954). Visual distance discrimination in the rat: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(3) Mar 1954, 148-152.
  • Gregson, R. A. (1985). Comparison of several theories and distance models: Similar data from Broderson (1968): Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(4) 1985, 573-587.
  • Grice, G. R. (1951). A note on the basis of discrimination in the Grice Size Discrimination apparatus: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 44(1) Feb 1951, 99.
  • Haber, R. N., Haber, L. R., Levin, C. A., & Hollyfield, R. (1993). Properties of spatial representations: Data from sighted and blind subjects: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(1) Jul 1993, 1-13.
  • Haber, R. N., & Levin, C. A. (2001). The independence of size perception and distance perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(7) Oct 2001, 1140-1152.
  • Haburcakova, C., Fuhry, L., Nedvidek, J., Tchelidze, T., & Buttner, U. (2000). Modulation of VOR gain during centric and eccentric rotation in the Rhesus monkey: Homeostasis in Health and Disease Vol 40(3-4) 2000, 88-90.
  • Hagen, M. A., & Giorgi, R. (1993). Where's the camera? : Ecological Psychology Vol 5(1) 1993, 65-84.
  • Hamill, R. (1931). Sequence of turns versus distance as essential pattern-elements in the maze problem: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 11(4) Apr 1931, 367-382.
  • Hamilton, W. F., & Goldstein, J. L. (1933). Visual acuity and accommodation in the pigeon: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 15(1) Feb 1933, 193-197.
  • Hancock, P. A., & Manser, M. P. (1997). Time-to-contact: More than tau alone: Ecological Psychology Vol 9(4) 1997, 265-297.
  • Hanyu, K., & Itsukushima, Y. (2000). Cognitive distance of stairways: A multi-stairway investigation: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology Vol 41(1) Mar 2000, 63-69.
  • Harris, J. M., & Sumnall, J. H. (2000). Detecting binocular 3D motion in stataic 3D noise: No effect of viewing distance: Spatial Vision Vol 14(1) 2000, 11-19.
  • Hartman, A. M. (1964). Effect of reduction on the relationship between apparent size and distance: American Journal of Psychology 77(3) 1964, 353-366.
  • Harway, N. I. (1963). Judgment of distance in children and adults: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 65(4) Apr 1963, 385-390.
  • Hatta, T. (2000). Implications of the symbol figure representations. Information from the direction and spatial location used by Japanese students: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 90(1) Feb 2000, 180-186.
  • Haussmann, R. E. (1996). The perceptual construction of visual velocity: Mechanism and model. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Hayashibe, K. (2002). Apparent distance in actual, three-dimensional video-recorded, and virtual reality: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(2) Oct 2002, 573-582.
  • Hazlett, C. J., & Woldorff, M. G. (2004). Mechanisms of Moving the Mind's Eye: Planning and Execution of Spatial Shifts of Attention: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Vol 16(5) Jun 2004, 742-750.
  • He, Z. J., Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., Yarbrough, G., & Wu, J. (2004). Judging egocentric distance on the ground: Occlusion and surface integration: Perception Vol 33(7) 2004, 789-806.
  • Healey, E. C., Jones, R., & Berky, R. (1997). Effects of Perceived Listeners on Speakers' Vocal Intensity: Journal of Voice Vol 11(1) Mar 1997, 67-73.
  • Hebert, J. A., & Krantz, D. L. (1965). Transposition: A reevaluation: Psychological Bulletin Vol 63(4) Apr 1965, 244-257.
  • Hecht, H., & Brauer, J. (2007). Convex rear view mirrors compromise distance and time-to-contact judgements: Ergonomics Vol 50(4) Apr 2007, 601-614.
  • Heft, H. (1993). A methodological note on overestimates of reaching distance: Distinguishing between perceptual and analytical judgments: Ecological Psychology Vol 5(3) 1993, 255-271.
  • Heller, J. (2004). The locus of perceived equidistance in binocular vision: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(7) Oct 2004, 1162-1170.
  • Heller, J., & Reyersbach, R. (1995). The influence of luminous intensity on perceived binocular distance: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie Vol 42(2) 1995, 237-255.
  • Hellige, J. B., & Michimata, C. (1989). Categorization versus distance: Hemispheric differences for processing spatial information: Memory & Cognition Vol 17(6) Nov 1989, 770-776.
  • Helson, H., & King, S. M. (1931). The tau effect: an example of psychological relativity: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 14(3) Jun 1931, 202-217.
  • Henderson, M. D., Fujita, K., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Transcending the "Here": The Effect of Spatial Distance on Social Judgment: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Vol 91(5) Nov 2006, 845-856.
  • Hernandez, C., Domenech, B., Segui, M. M., & Illueca, C. (1996). The effect of pupil and observation distance on the contrast sensitivity function: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 16(4) Jul 1996, 336-341.
  • Heron, G., Furby, H. P., Walker, R. J., Lane, C. S., & et al. (1995). Relationship between visual acuity and observation distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 15(1) Jan 1995, 23-30.
  • Hershman, D. P. (1998). Pitch discrimination across melodic transpositions in adults and young children: Psychomusicology Vol 17(1-2) Spr-Fal 1998-2001, 3-18.
  • Hess, J. A. (2002). Distance regulation in personal relationships: The development of a conceptual model and a test of representational validity: Journal of Social and Personal Relationships Vol 19(5) Oct 2002, 663-683.
  • Heuer, H. (1985). After effects of long fixation in distance perception: A test of motor interpretations: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(1) 1985, 33-54.
  • Heuer, H., Bruwer, M., Romer, T., Kroger, H., & et al. (1991). Preferred vertical gaze direction and observation distance: Ergonomics Vol 34(3) Mar 1991, 379-392.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Adachi, K. (2006). Perceived size and perceived distance of targets viewed from between the legs: Evidence for proprioceptive theory: Vision Research Vol 46(23) Nov 2006, 3961-3976.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Kitano, S. (1991). Perceived size and distance of persons in natural outdoor settings: The effects of familiar size: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 34(3) Sep 1991, 188-199.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Shimono, K. (1994). How accurate is size and distance perception for very far terrestrial objects? Function and causality: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 55(4) Apr 1994, 429-442.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Shimono, K. (2004). Mirror vision: Perceived size and perceived distance of virtual images: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(4) Apr 2004, 679-691.
  • Higashiyama, A., Shimono, K., & Zaitsu, W. (2005). Contraction of perceived size and perceived depth in mirrors: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 81-95.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Ueyama, E. (1988). The perception of vertical and horizontal distances in outdoor settings: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 44(2) Aug 1988, 151-156.
  • Higashiyama, A., Yokoyama, Y., & Shimono, K. (2001). Perceived distance of targets in convex mirrors: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 43(1) Mar 2001, 13-24.
  • Hill, G. W. (1989). Attentional demands and distance judgments: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Hiro, O. (1996). Distance perception in driving: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 55 1996, 92-100.
  • Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2007). Eye vergence is susceptible to the hollow-face illusion: Perception Vol 36(3) 2007, 461-470.
  • Holding, C. S. (1994). Further evidence for the hierarchical representation of spatial information: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 14(2) Jun 1994, 137-147.
  • Holding, C. S., & Holding, D. H. (1989). Acquisition of route network knowledge by males and females: Journal of General Psychology Vol 116(1) Jan 1989, 29-41.
  • Holland, J., Dabelsteen, T., Pedersen, S. B., & Larsen, O. N. (1998). Degradation of wren Troglodytes troglodytes song: Implications for information transfer and ranging: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2154-2166.
  • Hollins, M., & Goble, A. K. (1988). Perception of the length of voluntary movements: Somatosensory Research Vol 5(4) 1988, 335-348.
  • Holmberg, L., Kuller, R., & Tidblom, I. (1966). The perception of volume content of rectangular rooms as a function of the ratio between depth and width: Psychological Research Bulletin 6(1) 1966, 15.
  • Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Numerical reference points reexamined: A reply to Shoben, Cech, and Schwanenflugel: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 9(3) Jun 1983, 487-490.
  • Holyoak, K. J., & Patterson, K. K. (1981). A positional discriminability model of linear-order judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 7(6) Dec 1981, 1283-1302.
  • Holzkamp, K. (1966). On the problem of the relation between perceptual size and perceptual distance: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 13(1) 1966, 39-72.
  • Honda, H. (1990). The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: Its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization systems: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 48(5) Nov 1990, 509-515.
  • Hosokawa, T., Mikami, K., & Saito, K. (1997). Basic study of the Portable Fatigue Meter: Effects of illumination, distance from eyes and age: Ergonomics Vol 40(9) Sep 1997, 887-894.
  • Hsieh, K.-C., Hsu, C.-S., Chin, C., & Tan, C.-M. (1965). The position of seats and visual effect in gymnasiums: Acta Psychologica Sinica 32(3) 1965, 241-247.
  • Hubbard, T. L., Kall, D., & Baird, J. C. (1989). Imagery, memory, and size-distance invariance: Memory & Cognition Vol 17(1) Jan 1989, 87-94.
  • Huertas, J. A., & Ochaita, E. (1992). The externalization of spatial representation by blind persons: Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness Vol 86(9) Nov 1992, 398-402.
  • Hund, A. M., & Plumert, J. M. (2007). What Counts as By? Young Children's Use of Relative Distance to Judge Nearbyness: Developmental Psychology Vol 43(1) Jan 2007, 121-133.
  • Hutchison, J. J., & Loomis, J. M. (2006). Does Energy Expenditure Affect the Perception of Egocentric Distance? A Failure to Replicate Experiment 1 of Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein (2003): The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 332-339.
  • Hutchison, J. J., & Loomis, J. M. (2006). Reply to Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 343-345.
  • Hutchison, K. C. (1987). Graphic and verbal/numeric feedback in computer-assisted instruction: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Vasilyeva, M. (1999). Spatial scaling in young children: Psychological Science Vol 10(5) Sep 1999, 393-398.
  • Hyslop, J. H. (1894). Experiments in space perception: (I): Psychological Review Vol 1(3) May 1894, 257-273.
  • Ichihara, S., Kitagawa, N., & Akutsu, H. (2007). Contrast and depth perception: Effects of texture contrast and area contrast: Perception Vol 36(5) 2007, 686-695.
  • Ichikawa, M., Kimura, T., Egusa, H., Nakatsuka, M., Amano, J., Ueda, T., et al. (2003). Modification of depth and distance perception caused by long-term wearing of left-right reversing spectacles: Perception Vol 32(2) 2003, 131-153.
  • Ido, K., Ohtani, Y., & Ejima, Y. (2000). Summation between nearby motion signals and facilitative/inhibitory interactions between distant motion signals: Vision Research Vol 40(5) 2000, 503-516.
  • Imanaka, K. (1989). Effect of starting position on reproduction of movement: Further evidence of interference between location and distance information: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 68(2) Apr 1989, 423-434.
  • Imanaka, K., & Abernethy, B. (1992). Cognitive strategies and short-term memory for movement distance and location: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 45A(4) Nov 1992, 669-700.
  • Imanaka, K., & Abernethy, B. (2000). Distance-location interference in movement reproduction: An interaction between conscious and unconscious processing? Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Inoue, H., & Watanabe, I. (2005). The effect of location of a highlight in a shaded circle on depth perception: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 76(1) Apr 2005, 51-56.
  • Israel, I., Capelli, A., Sable, D., Laurent, C., Lecoq, C., & Bredin, J. (2004). Multifactorial interactions involved in linear self-transport distance estimate: A place for time: International Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 53(1) Jun 2004, 21-28.
  • Ito, H., & Matsunaga, K. (1991). The perception of an environment through expanding and contracting motion and the role of self-motion: Japanese Psychological Review Vol 34(1) 1991, 5-16.
  • Ittelson, W. H. (1961). Size as a cue to distance. New York, NY: New York University Press.
  • Jackson, R. E., & Cormack, L. K. (2007). Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 69(3) Apr 2007, 353-362.
  • Jacobs, D. M., & Michaels, C. F. (2001). Individual differences and the use of nonspecifying variables in learning to perceive distance and size: Comments on McConnell, Muchisky, and Bingham (1998): Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(3) Apr 2001, 563-571.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P. (2001). The influence of environmental features of a route on distance knowledge: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie Vol 48(4) 2001, 327-338.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2002). Investigating distance knowledge using virtual environments: Environment and Behavior Vol 34(2) Mar 2002, 178-193.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2005). What makes a route appear longer? An experimental perspective on features, route segmentation, and distance knowledge: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 58A(8) 2005, 1390-1414.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2004). The influence of turns on distance cognition: New experimental approaches to clarify the route-angularity effect: Environment and Behavior Vol 36(6) Nov 2004, 790-813.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2006). Distance cognition in virtual environmental space: Further investigations to clarify the route-angularity effect: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 70(1) Jan 2006, 43-51.
  • Jarymowicz, M. (1989). An attempted operationalization of the concepts "social identity" and "individual identity": Schematic separateness of we others as an identity attribute: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 73-84.
  • Jaschinski-Kruza, W. (1990). Effects of stimulus distance on measurements of dark convergence: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(3) Jul 1990, 243-251.
  • Jenkin, N. (1960). Size-distance judgment in organic mental defectives: Journal of Consulting Psychology Vol 24(2) Apr 1960, 139-143.
  • Jimenez, A. C., Sicilia, A. O., & Vera, J. G. (2007). Improving spatial perception in 5-yr.-old Spanish children: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 104(3, Pt2) Jun 2007, 1223-1235.
  • Johnston, E. B. (1991). Systematic distortions of shape from stereopsis: Vision Research Vol 31(7-8) 1991, 1351-1360.
  • Jones, L. A., & Bertamini, M. (2007). Through the looking glass: How the relationship between an object and its reflection affects the perception of distance and size: Perception Vol 36(11) 2007, 1572-1594.
  • Jones-Buxton, R. A., & Wall, H. M. (2001). The Poggendorff illusion: Effect of distance between the parallel lines: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 92(3,Pt1) Jun 2001, 706-710.
  • Jou, J., & Aldridge, J. W. (1999). Memory representation of alphabetic position and interval information: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 25(3) May 1999, 680-701.
  • Judd, C. H. (1899). A study of geometrical illusions: Psychological Review Vol 6(3) May 1899, 241-261.
  • Jui-Hseung, P., & Chung-Hsien, L. (1964). The influence of certain factors on the judgment of distance: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 1 1964, 9-19.
  • Jurowski, C., & Gursoy, D. (2004). Distance Effects on Residents' Attitudes Toward Tourism: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 31(2) Apr 2004, 296-312.
  • Kaan, E. (2002). Investigating the effects of distance and number interference in processing subject-verb dependencies: An ERP study: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Vol 31(2) Mar 2002, 165-181.
  • Kaess, D. W., & et al. (1974). Effect of distance and size of standard object on the development of shape constancy: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 102(1) Jan 1974, 17-21.
  • Kamenchuk, O. (2004). Power distance perceptions in Post-Soviet Russia: Understanding the workplace environment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kaminska-Feldman, M. (1989). Estimating self-others distance: The phenomenon of ego- and allo-centric asymmetry: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 27-39.
  • Kaminska-Feldman, M. (1997). Does asymmetry bias in the self-others distance estimation reflect implicit stereotypes? : Studia Psychologiczne Vol 35(1) 1997, 97-116.
  • Kang, Y.-S., Herr, P. M., & Page, C. M. (2003). Time and distance: Asymmetries in consumer trip knowledge and judgments: Journal of Consumer Research Vol 30(3) Dec 2003, 420-429.
  • Kapoula, Z., Evdokimidis, I., Smyrnis, N., Bucci, M.-P., & Constantinidis, T. S. (2002). EEG cortical potentials preceding vergence and combined saccade-vergence eye movements: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 13(15) Oct 2002, 1893-1897.
  • Karpiuk, P. (1995). Sensation and feeling: A Wave Theory analysis. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kaufman, L., Kaufman, J. H., Noble, R., Edlund, S., Bai, S., & King, T. (2006). Perceptual distance and the constancy of size and stereoscopic depth: Spatial Vision Vol 19(5) 2006, 439-457.
  • Kaufman, L., Vassiliades, V., Noble, R., Alexander, R., Kaufman, J., & Edlund, S. (2007). Perceptual distance and the moon illusion: Spatial Vision Vol 20(1-2) 2007, 155-175.
  • Kebeck, G., Figge, S., & Schreiber, K. (1990). The relationship between physical and imagined distances: An experiment on the foundations of mental training: Gestalt Theory Vol 12(2) Jun 1990, 96-115.
  • Kebeck, G., & Huppe, A. (1985). Possibility of a direct perception of distance: The relationship of objective and phenomenal centerpoints: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(2) 1985, 250-262.
  • Kebeck, G., & Landwehr, K. (1992). Optical magnification as event information: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 54(3) Aug 1992, 146-159.
  • Kellman, P., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). Extracting object motion during observer motion: Combining constraints from optic flow and binocular disparity: Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science & Vision Vol 12(3) Mar 1995, 623-625.
  • Kelly, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Judgments of exocentric direction in large-scale space: Perception Vol 33(4) 2004, 443-454.
  • Kelso, J. A. (1977). Motor control mechanisms underlying human movement reproduction: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 3(4) Nov 1977, 529-543.
  • Kendler, H. H. (1943). The influence of a sub-goal on maze behavior: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 36(2) Oct 1943, 67-73.
  • Kennedy, F. (1900). Memory: Psychological Review Vol 7(3) May 1900, 305-309.
  • Kennedy, J. M. (2002). Touch and the observer's vantage point. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Kennedy, R. S., Berbaum, K. S., Collyer, S. C., May, J. G., & et al. (1988). Spatial requirements for visual simulation of aircraft at real-world distances: Human Factors Vol 30(2) Apr 1988, 153-161.
  • Kesner, R. P., & Hopkins, R. O. (2001). Short-term memory for duration and distance in humans: Role of the hippocampus: Neuropsychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2001, 58-68.
  • Kidd, A. H. (1964). Monocular distance perception in schizophrenics: The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology Vol 68(1) Jan 1964, 100-103.
  • Kiefer, R. J., Flannagan, C. A., & Jerome, C. J. (2006). Time-to-Collision Judgments Under Realistic Driving Conditions: Human Factors Vol 48(2) Sum 2006, 334-345.
  • Kiesow, F. (1895). Review of Studien zur Blinden-Psychologie: Psychological Review Vol 2(5) Sep 1895, 531-533.
  • Kilpatrick, F. P., & Ittelson, W. H. (1953). The size-distance invariance hypothesis: Psychological Review Vol 60(4) Jul 1953, 223-231.
  • Kimura, T., & Miura, T. (2003). Attention in depth modulates spatial perception--A comparison between depth perception and distance perception: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 22(1) Sep 2003, 39-40.
  • King, W. L., & Hayes, M. C. (1966). The sun illusion: Individual differences in remembered size and distance judgments: Psychonomic Science Vol 5(2) 1966, 65-66.
  • Kinsella-Shaw, J. M. (1992). Haptic perception of object distance in a single-strand vibratory web: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Haptic perception of object distance in a single-strand vibratory web: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 52(6) Dec 1992, 625-638.
  • Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Haggerty, D. (1992). Age-related differences in adults' macrospatial cognitive processes: Experimental Aging Research Vol 18(1-2) Spr-Sum 1992, 33-39.
  • Kirasic, K. C., & Bernicki, M. R. (1990). Acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity in young and elderly adults: Psychological Research Vol 52(1) Jun 1990, 76-79.
  • Kirkley, S. E. H., Jr. (2003). Augmented reality performance assessment battery (ARPAB): Object recognition, distance estimation and size estimation using optical see-through head-worn displays. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kjelldahl, L., & Schenkman, B. N. (2007). Colour induction on computer displays--Adjacency and shape effects: Behaviour & Information Technology Vol 26(3) May 2007, 261-272.
  • Klein, O. (2004). Social perception of time, distance and high-speed transportation: Time & Society Vol 13(2-3) Sep 2004, 245-263.
  • Kleiss, J. A., & Hubbard, D. C. (1993). Effects of three types of flight simulator visual scene detail on detection of altitude change: Human Factors Vol 35(4) Dec 1993, 653-671.
  • Kline, P. B. (2000). Estimation of traversed distance in a virtual environment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Knapp, J. M. (2001). The visual perception of egocentric distance in virtual environments. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Koenderink, J. J., Van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2003). Exocentric pointing to opposite targets: Acta Psychologica Vol 112(1) Jan 2003, 71-87.
  • Kohler, M., & Wehner, R. (2005). Idiosyncratic route-based memories in desert ants, Melophorus bagoti: How do they interact with path-integration vectors? : Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 83(1) Jan 2005, 1-12.
  • Kohly, R. P., & Ono, H. (2002). Fixating on the wallpaper illusion: A commentary on "The role of vergence in the perception of distance: A fair test of Bishop Berkeley's claim' by Logvinenko et. al. (2001): Spatial Vision Vol 15(3) 2002, 377-386.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 4(1) Feb 1978, 47-60.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C. B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(4) Nov 1989, 723-735.
  • Kotulak, J. C., & Morse, S. E. (1995). The effect of perceived distance on accommodation under binocular steady-state conditions: Vision Research Vol 35(6) Mar 1995, 791-795.
  • Kotulak, J. C., Morse, S. E., & Wiley, R. W. (1994). The effect of knowledge of object distance on accommodation during instrument viewing: Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 671-679.
  • Kozhevnikova, E. V. (1990). Auditory estimate of the distance of a sound source: Sensory Systems Vol 4(2), Trans Apr-Jun 1990, 156-164.
  • Kraft, R. N., & Green, J. S. (1989). Distance perception as a function of photographic area of view: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(5) May 1989, 459-466.
  • Kral, K., & Devetak, D. (1999). The visual orientation strategies of Mantis reliogosa and Empusa fasciata reflect differences in the structure of their visual surroundings: Journal of Insect Behavior Vol 12(6) Nov 1999, 737-752.
  • Kral, K., & Poteser, M. (1997). Motion parallax as a source of distance information in locusts and mantids: Journal of Insect Behavior Vol 10(1) Jan 1997, 145-163.
  • Krause, W. (1985). Component analysis of the symbolic-distance effect by means of eye movement measurement: Zeitschrift fur Psychologie mit Zeitschrift fur angewandte Psychologie Vol 193(3) 1985, 259-272.
  • Kuroki, D., & Nakamizo, S. (2006). Depth scaling in phantom and monocular gap stereograms using absolute distance information: Vision Research Vol 46(25) Nov 2006, 4206-4216.
  • Labbe, C., & Labbe, D. (2001). Inter-Textual Distance and Authorship Attribution: Corneille and Molire: Journal of Quantitative Linguistics Vol 8(3) Dec 2001, 213-231.
  • Labhart, T., & Meyer, E. P. (2002). Neural mechanisms in insect navigation: Polarization compass and odometer: Current Opinion in Neurobiology Vol 12(6) Dec 2002, 707-714.
  • Lan, W.-C., & Matsuda, F. (1997). The temporal and spatial judgments of junior high school students: Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology Vol 8(3) Dec 1997, 176-185.
  • Lappin, J. S., Shelton, A. L., & Rieser, J. J. (2006). Environmental context influences visually perceived distance: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(4) May 2006, 571-581.
  • Law, D. J., Pellegrino, J. W., Mitchell, S. R., Fischer, S. C., McDonald, T. P., & Hunt, E. B. (1993). Perceptual and cognitive factors governing performance in comparative arrival-time judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 19(6) Dec 1993, 1183-1199.
  • Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Collins, A., & Wardell, J. (1987). Exploring environments by hand or foot: Time-based heuristics for encoding distance in movement space: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 13(4) Oct 1987, 606-614.
  • Legg, C. R., & Lambert, S. (1990). Distance estimation in the hooded rat: Experimental evidence for the role of motion cues: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 41(1) Dec 1990, 11-20.
  • Leibowitz, H. W., & Hennessy, R. T. (1975). The laser optometer and some implications for behavioral research: American Psychologist Vol 30(3) Mar 1975, 349-352.
  • Leonard, G., & Milner, B. (1991). Contribution of the right frontal lobe to the encoding and recall of kinesthetic distance information: Neuropsychologia Vol 29(1) 1991, 47-58.
  • Levi, D. M., & Klein, S. A. (1990). The role of separation and eccentricity in encoding position: Vision Research Vol 30(4) 1990, 557-585.
  • Levin, C. A. (1991). The independence of size perception and distance perception: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Levin, C. A., & Haber, R. N. (1993). Visual angle as a determinant of perceived interobject distance: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(2) Aug 1993, 250-259.
  • Levin, I., Siegler, R. S., & Druyan, S. (1990). Misconceptions about motion: Development and training effects: Child Development Vol 61(5) Oct 1990, 1544-1557.
  • Li, W., & Matin, L. (2005). Two wrongs make a right: Linear increase of accuracy of visually-guided manual pointing, reaching, and height-matching with increase in hand-to-body distance: Vision Research Vol 45(5) Mar 2005, 533-550.
  • Liang, Y. H., & Wu, C. J. (2007). The effects of number of turns and attention on the perceived distance and time in children and adults: Chinese Journal of Psychology Vol 49(1) Mar 2007, 19-33.
  • Lintern, G. (2000). An analysis of slant for guidance of landing approaches: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 10(4) 2000, 363-376.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Ashmead, D. H. (1997). Development of binaural and spatial hearing in infants and children. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Clifton, R. K. (1992). Use of sound-pressure level in auditory distance discrimination by 6-month-old infants and adults: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(2, Pt 1) Aug 1992, 794-802.
  • Loftus, G. R. (1985). Size illusion, distance illusion, and terrestrial passage: Comment on Reed: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 114(1) Mar 1985, 119-121.
  • Loftus, G. R., & Harley, E. M. (2005). Why is it easier to identify someone close than far away? : Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(1) Feb 2005, 43-65.
  • Logan, G. D., & Compton, B. J. (1996). Distance and distraction effects in the apprehension of spatial relations: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(1) Feb 1996, 159-172.
  • Logie, R. H., & Vecchi, T. (2006). Motor components and complexity effects in visuo-spatial processes. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., & Belopolskii, V. I. (1994). Convergence as a cue for distance: Perception Vol 23(2) 1994, 207-217.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., Epelboim, J., & Steinman, R. M. (2002). The role of vergence in the perception of distance: A fair test of Bishop Berkeley's claim: Spatial Vision Vol 15(1) 2002, 77-97.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., & Steinman, R. M. (2002). Fixation on fixation impedes cognition: Reply to Kohly and Ono: Spatial Vision Vol 15(3) 2002, 387-391.
  • Lombard, M. (1994). Direct responses to people on the screen: Television and the illusion of nonmediation. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences.
  • Long, J. M., & Kesner, R. P. (1998). Effects of hippocampal and parietal cortex lesions on memory for egocentric distance and spatial location information in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 112(3) Jun 1998, 480-495.
  • Loomis, J., & Knapp, J. (2003). Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Loomis, J. M., da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 18(4) Nov 1992, 906-921.
  • Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., & Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 5(3) Jun 1996, 72-77.
  • Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Philbeck, J. W., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(6) Aug 1998, 966-980.
  • Loukopoulos, L. D., Engelbrecht, S. E., & Berthier, N. E. (2001). Planning of reach-and-grasp movements: Effects of validity and type of object information: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 33(3) Sep 2001, 255-264.
  • Macdonald, W. A., & Hoffmann, E. R. (1973). The recognition of road pavement messages: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 57(3) Jun 1973, 314-319.
  • MacKay, D. B. (1989). Probabilistic multidimensional scaling: An anisotropic model for distance judgments: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 33(2) Jun 1989, 187-205.
  • MacKay, D. B. (2001). Probabilistic multidimensional scaling using a city-block metric: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 45(2) Apr 2001, 249-264.
  • Madsen, J., & Kesner, R. P. (1995). The temporal-distance effect in subjects with dementia of the Alzheimer type: Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders Vol 9(2) Sum 1995, 94-100.
  • Magne, P., & Coello, Y. (2002). Retinal and extra-retinal contribution to position coding: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 136(1) Nov 2002, 277-287.
  • Maher, B., Weisstein, N., & Sylva, K. (1964). The determinants of oscillation points in a temporal decision conflict: Psychonomic Science Vol 1(1) 1964, 13-14.
  • Makany, T., Redhead, E. S., & Dror, I. E. (2007). Spatial exploration patterns determine navigation efficiency: Trade-off between memory demands and distance travelled: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 60(12) Dec 2007, 1594-1602.
  • Maki, R. H. (1981). Categorization and distance effects with spatial linear orders: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 7(1) Jan 1981, 15-32.
  • Maples, W. C. (2005). Polaroid Projection in Near Space: Observations of Size and Distance: Optometry and Vision Development Vol 36(1) 2005, 35-40.
  • Mapp, A. P., Ono, H., & Saida, S. (1986). Extreme eye position and Hering's law of equal innervation: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 40(3) Sep 1986, 251-262.
  • Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis: Child Development Vol 70(6) Nov-Dec 1999, 1297-1313.
  • Martel, L., Grealy, M., & Coello, Y. (2006). Contribution of retinal and non-retinal sources of visual information in egocentric object localisation for action: Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition No 19(2) 2006, No Pagination Specified.
  • Martin, L. L. (1999). The use of language to restructure perceptual input in children with and without language impairment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Mateeff, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (1996). Perception of visual motion with modulated velocity: Effects of viewing distance and aperture size: Vision Research Vol 36(18) Sep 1996, 2873-2882.
  • Mather, G., & Tunley, H. (1995). Temporal filtering enhances direction discrimination in random-dot patterns: Vision Research Vol 35(15) Aug 1995, 2105-2116.
  • Mathiak, K., Hertrich, I., Kincses, W. E., Riecker, A., Lutzenberger, W., & Ackermann, H. (2003). The right supratemporal plane hears the distance of objects: Neuromagnetic correlates of virtual reality: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 14(3) Mar 2003, 307-311.
  • Matsuda, F. (1991). Concepts about relations among time, distance and velocity in children: I. Time and velocity: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 34(1) Mar 1991, 36-46.
  • Matsuda, F. (1992). Concepts about relations among time, distance and velocity in children: II. Distance and velocity, and time and distance: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 35(4) Dec 1992, 222-231.
  • Matsuda, F. (1994). Concepts about interrelations among duration, distance, and speed in young children: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 17(3) Sep 1994, 553-576.
  • Matsuda, F. (1996). Duration, distance, a speed judgments of two moving objects by 4- to 11-year-olds: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 63(2) Nov 1996, 286-311.
  • Matsuda, F. (2001). Development of concepts of interrelationships among duration, distance, and speed: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 25(5) Sep 2001, 466-480.
  • Matsuda, F., Lan, W.-C., & Tanimura, R. (1999). Contribution of stimulus attributes to errors in duration and distance judgments--A developmental study: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 88(1) Feb 1999, 87-98.
  • Matsuda, F., Tanimura, R., & Woei-Chen, L. (2000). Duration and distance judgments of two moving objects by 1st to 6th graders: Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition Vol 19(5-6) Dec 2000, 575-602.
  • Matsushima, E., Chiaretti, P., Kreling, D., Lima, M., Da Silva, J. A., & Ribeiro-Filho, N. (2004). Perception-action interaction and bissection: Cadernos de Psicologia e Educacao Paideia Vol 14(27) Jan-Apr 2004, 83-88.
  • Matsushima, E. H., de Oliveira, A. P., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2005). Visual angle as determinant factor for relative distance perception: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 97-104.
  • Matsushima, E. H., Gomes, B. C., Oliveira, L. E., Ribeiro, N. P., & Silva, J. A. (2000). Evidence of angular elevation as an absolute distance cue in visual alley: Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia Vol 52(2) 2000, 85-92.
  • Mauerberg-deCastro, E., & Moraes, R. (2002). Distance Perception in Children while Walking: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 15(2) 2002, 373-381.
  • McConnell, D. S., Muchisky, M. M., & Bingham, G. P. (1998). The use of time and trajectory forms as visual information about spatial scale in events: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(7) Oct 1998, 1175-1187.
  • McFadden, S. A. (1990). Eye design for depth and distance perception in the pigeon: An observer orientated perspective: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 3(2) Win 1990, 101-130.
  • McIntosh, R. D., Dijkerman, H. C., Mon-Williams, M., & Milner, A. D. (2004). Grasping what is graspable: Evidence from visual form agnosia: Cortex Vol 40(4-5) Sep-Dec 2004, 695-702.
  • McIntosh, R. D., McClements, K. I., Dijkerman, H. C., & Milner, A. D. (2004). "Mind the gap": The size-distance dissociation in visual neglect is a cueing effect: Cortex Vol 40(2) Apr 2004, 339-346.
  • McKenzie, B. E., Skouteris, H., Day, R. H., Hartman, B., & et al. (1993). Effective action by infants to contact objects by reaching and leaning: Child Development Vol 64(2) Apr 1993, 415-429.
  • McKenzie, B. E., Tootell, H. E., & Day, R. H. (1980). Development of visual size constancy during the 1st year of human infancy: Developmental Psychology Vol 16(3) May 1980, 163-174.
  • McNaughton, B. L., Knierim, J. J., & Wilson, M. A. (1995). Vector encoding and the vestibular foundations of spatial cognition: Neurophysiological and computational mechanisms. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • McNulty, J. A., & St. Claire-Smith, R. (1964). Terrain effects upon perceived distance: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 18(3) 1964, 175-182.
  • Meng, H., & Angelaki, D. E. (2006). Neural Correlates of the Dependence of Compensatory Eye Movements During Translation on Target Distance and Eccentricity: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 95(4) Apr 2006, 2530-2540.
  • Meng, J. C., & Sedgwick, H. A. (2001). Distance perception mediated through nested contact relations among surfaces: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(1) Jan 2001, 1-15.
  • Meng, J. C., & Sedgwick, H. A. (2002). Distance perception across spatial discontinuities: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(1) Jan 2002, 1-14.
  • Menne, D., Kaipf, I., Wagner, I., Ostwald, J., & et al. (1989). Range estimation by echolocation in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: Trading of phase versus time cues: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(6) Jun 1989, 2642-2650.
  • Mershon, D. H. (1997). Phenomenal geometry and the measurement of perceived auditory distance. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Mershon, D. H., Ballenger, W. L., Little, A. D., McMurtry, P. L., & et al. (1989). Effects of room reflectance and background noise on perceived auditory distance: Perception Vol 18(3) 1989, 403-416.
  • Mershon, D. H., & Hutson, W. E. (1991). Toward the indirect measurement of perceived auditory distance: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(2) Mar 1991, 109-112.
  • Mershon, D. H., Jones, T. A., & Taylor, M. E. (1993). Organizational factors and the perception of motion in depth: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(2) Aug 1993, 240-249.
  • Messing, R., & Durgin, F. H. (2005). Distance perception and the visual horizon in head-mounted displays: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(3) Jul 2005, 234-250.
  • Michael, D. D., Siegel, G. M., & Pick, H. L. (1995). Effects of distance on vocal intensity: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 38(5) Oct 1995, 1176-1183.
  • Michaels, C. F., Withagen, R., Jacobs, D. M., Zaal, F. T. J. M., & Bongers, R. M. (2001). Information, perception, and action: A reply to commentators: Ecological Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 227-244.
  • Michalewski, H. J., Mohamed Kamel, A.-W., & Starr, A. (1988). Brain potentials during mental distance judgments: International Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 6(3) Aug 1988, 221-229.
  • Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006). Two kinds of visual perspective taking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(2) Feb 2006, 327-337.
  • Millar, S., & Al-Attar, Z. (2001). Illusions in reading maps by touch: Reducing distance errors: British Journal of Psychology Vol 92(4) Nov 2001, 643-657.
  • Miller, K. F., & Baillargeon, R. (1990). Length and distance: Do preschoolers think that occlusion brings things together? : Developmental Psychology Vol 26(1) Jan 1990, 103-114.
  • Milner, A. D., & McIntosh, R. D. (2002). Perceptual and visuomotor processing in spatial neglect. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Montello, D. R. (1989). Route information and travel time as bases for the perception and cognition of environmental distance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Bingham, G. P. (2007). Calibrating reach distance to visual targets: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 33(3) Jun 2007, 645-656.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Tresilian, J. R. (1999). Some recent studies on the extraretinal contribution to distance perception: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 167-181.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Tresilian, J. R. (2000). Ordinal depth information from accommodation? : Ergonomics Vol 43(3) Mar 2000, 391-404.
  • Moore, D. R., & King, A. J. (1999). Auditory perception: The near and far of sound localization: Current Biology Vol 9(10) May 1999, R361-R363.
  • Morgan, M. J., & Glennerster, A. (1991). Efficiency of locating centres of dot-clusters by human observers: Vision Research Vol 31(12) 1991, 2075-2083.
  • Morris, A., & Temme, L. A. (1989). The time required for U.S. Navy fighter pilots to shift gaze and identify near and far targets: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 60(11) Nov 1989, 1085-1089.
  • Morrongiello, B. A., Hewitt, K. L., & Gotowiec, A. (1991). Infants' discrimination of relative distance in the auditory modality: Approaching versus receding sound sources: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 14(2) Apr-Jun 1991, 187-208.
  • Mortier, F., Robin, S., Lassalvy, S., Baril, C. P., & Bar-Hen, A. (2006). Prediction of Euclidean distances with discrete and continuous outcomes: Journal of Multivariate Analysis Vol 97(8) Sep 2006, 1799-1814.
  • Mossman, C. A., & Drickamer, L. C. (1996). Odor preferences of female house mice (Mus domesticus) in seminatural enclosures: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 110(2) Jun 1996, 131-138.
  • Mowafy, M. K. (1988). Motion perception: The effects of perceived three-dimensional distance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Musseler, J., & Neumann, O. (1992). Apparent distance reduction with moving stimuli (Tandem Effect): Evidence for an attention-shifting model: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 54(4) Dec 1992, 246-266.
  • Naguib, M., Klump, G. M., Hillmann, E., Griessmann, B., & Teige, T. (2000). Assessment of auditory distance in a territorial songbird: Accurate feat or rule of thumb? : Animal Behaviour Vol 59(4) Apr 2000, 715-721.
  • Nakamizo, S., Ono, H., & Ujike, H. (1999). Subjective staircase: A multiple wallpaper illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(1) Jan 1999, 13-22.
  • Nelson, B. S. (2000). Avian dependence on sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue: Animal Behaviour Vol 59(1) Jan 2000, 57-67.
  • Nelson, B. S., & Stoddard, P. K. (1998). Accuracy of auditory distance and azimuth perception by a passerine bird in natural habitat: Animal Behaviour Vol 56(2) Aug 1998, 467-477.
  • Ni, R., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2005). Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact: Visual Cognition Vol 12(6) Aug 2005, 1235-1254.
  • Niall, K. K. (1999). The art of descrying distance: Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 511-514.
  • Niall, K. K., Reising, J. D., & Martin, E. L. (1999). Distance estimation with night vision goggles: A little feedback goes a long way: Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 495-506.
  • Nicol, A. U., Brown, M. W., & Horn, G. (1999). Is neuronal encoding of subject-object distance dependent on learning? : Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 10(8) Jun 1999, 1671-1675.
  • Niers, P. (1987). The ego as the basis of unity: Psychoanalytic considerations concerning Piaget's genetic epistemology: Tijdschrift voor Psychotherapie Vol 13(2) Mar 1987, 92-105.
  • No authorship, i. (1894). The Perception of Distance: Psychological Review Vol 1(5) Sep 1894, 540-541.
  • No authorship, i. (1895). Experimentelle Untersuchungen uber das Gedachtniss: Comment: Psychological Review Vol 2(3) May 1895, 317-318.
  • Noordzij, M. L., & Postma, A. (2005). Categorical and metric distance information in mental representations derived from route and survey descriptions: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 69(3) Jan 2005, 221-232.
  • Norman, J. F., Crabtree, C. E., Clayton, A. M., & Norman, H. F. (2005). The perception of distances and spatial relationships in natural outdoor environments: Perception Vol 34(11) 2005, 1315-1324.
  • Norman, J. F., Lappin, J. S., & Norman, H. F. (2000). The perception of length on curved and flat surfaces: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(6) Aug 2000, 1133-1145.
  • Norman, K. L. (1974). Dynamic processes in stimulus integration theory: Effects of feedback on averaging of motor movements: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 102(3) Mar 1974, 399-408.
  • North, R. V., Sethi, B., & Owen, K. (1990). Prism adaptation and viewing distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(1) Jan 1990, 81-85.
  • Nowe, V., Wuyts, F. L., Hoppenbrouwers, M., van de Heyning, P. H., de Schepper, A. M., & Parizel, P. M. (2003). The interutricular distance determined from external landmarks: Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium & Orientation Vol 13(1) 2003, 17-23.
  • Ochaita, E., & Espinosa, M. A. (1997). Spatial knowledge and spatial structure: A study carried out in two neighbourhoods of Madrid: Revista de Psicologia Social Vol 12(1) 1997, 3-16.
  • Ohtsuka, S., Ujike, H., & Saida, S. (2002). Relative distance cues contribute to scaling depth from motion parallax: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(3) Apr 2002, 405-414.
  • Okada, H., & Wapner, S. (1991). Apparent distance between profiles of faces with dynamic properties that represent interpersonal relationships: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(2) Mar 1991, 150-152.
  • O'Kane, B. L., Biederman, I., Cooper, E. E., & Nystrom, B. (1997). An account of objects identification confusions: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Vol 3(1) Mar 1997, 21-41.
  • Ono, H., Lillakas, L., Grove, P. M., & Suzuki, M. (2003). Leonardo's constraint: Two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 132(2) Jun 2003, 253-265.
  • Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2007). A distance judgment function based on space perception mechanisms: Revisiting Gilinsky's (1951) equation: Psychological Review Vol 114(2) Apr 2007, 441-454.
  • Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon: Nature Vol 414(6860) Nov 2001, 197-200.
  • Oshima, Y., & Okaichi, H. (1990). Effect of campus experience and sex difference on distance judgment in a campus environment: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 61(3) Aug 1990, 170-176.
  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolne, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(4) Aug 1996, 879-891.
  • Pagano, C. C., & Bingham, G. P. (1998). Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 24(4) Aug 1998, 1037-1051.
  • Pagano, C. C., Grutzmacher, R. P., & Jenkins, J. C. (2001). Comparing verbal and reaching responses to visually perceived egocentric distances: Ecological Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 197-226.
  • Panerai, F., Cornilleau-Peres, V., & Droulez, J. (2002). Contribution of extraretinal signals to the scaling of object distance during self-motion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(5) Jul 2002, 717-731.
  • Paquet, N., Rainville, C., Lajoie, Y., & Tremblay, F. (2007). Reproducibility of distance and direction errors associated with forward, backward, and sideway walking in the context of blind navigation: Perception Vol 36(4) 2007, 525-536.
  • Parks, T. E. (2001). The mirror- (and the moon-) illusion: Perception Vol 30(7) 2001, 899.
  • Paterson, S., & Marino, F. E. (2004). Effect of Deception of Distance on Prolonged Cycling Performance: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 98(3,Pt1) Jun 2004, 1017-1026.
  • Peck, A. J., Jeffers, R. G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Haptically perceiving the length of one rod by means of another: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(3) 1996, 237-258.
  • Peh, C.-H., Panerai, F., Droulez, J., Cornilleau-Peres, V., & Cheong, L.-F. (2002). Absolute distance perception during in-depth head movement: Calibrating optic flow with extra-retinal information: Vision Research Vol 42(16) Jul 2002, 1991-2003.
  • Pei, J., Shen, J., & Gong, J. (1995). A preliminary study on the influencing factors on children's ability of motion distance prediction: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 27(4) 1995, 371-378.
  • Perrine, M. W. (1966). Formation of reference scales, discrimination and verbal reinforcement: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 13(3) 1966, 464-483.
  • Peruch, P., Borel, L., Magnan, J., & Lacour, M. (2005). Direction and distance deficits in path integration after unilateral vestibular loss depend on task complexity: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 25(3) Dec 2005, 862-872.
  • Peruch, P., Giraudo, M.-D., & Garling, T. (1989). Distance cognition by taxi drivers and the general public: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 9(3) Sep 1989, 233-239.
  • Philbeck, J. W. (2000). Visually directed walking to briefly glimpsed targets is not biased toward fixation location: Perception Vol 29(3) 2000, 259-272.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & Loomis, J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 72-85.
  • Philbeck, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1997). Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 59(4) May 1997, 601-612.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & Mershon, D. H. (2002). Knowledge about typical source output influences perceived auditory distance (L): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(5,Pt1) May 2002, 1980-1983.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & O'Leary, S. (2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path integration: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 7-24.
  • Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., Ramsay, S. M., & Weisman, R. G. (1998). Discrimination of auditory distance cues by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) and zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 112(3) Sep 1998, 282-291.
  • Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., & Weisman, R. G. (2003). Does reduced social contact affect discrimination of distance cues and individual vocalizations? : Animal Behaviour Vol 65(5) May 2003, 911-922.
  • Pickens, J. N. (1990). Perception of bimodal distance relations by 5-month-old human infants: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Pillsbury, W. B. (1901). Beitrage zur Analyse der Gesichtswahrnehmungen: Psychological Review Vol 8(2) Mar 1901, 212-213.
  • Pippert, E. (1959). Muscularly performed absolute distance judgments: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 6 1959, 372-393.
  • Pizzamiglio, L., Cappa, S., Vallar, G., Zoccolotti, P., & et al. (1989). Visual neglect for far and near extra-personal space in humans: Cortex Vol 25(3) Sep 1989, 471-477.
  • Plug, C. (1989). The registered distance of the celestial sphere: Some historical cross-cultural data: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 68(1) Feb 1989, 211-217.
  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., Cremer, J. F., & Recker, K. (2005). Distance perception in real and virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(3) Jul 2005, 216-233.
  • Polich, J. (1993). Hemispheric differences for feature migrations: Acta Psychologica Vol 83(3) Aug 1993, 179-201.
  • Poucet, B., & Buhot, M. C. (1989). Scopolamine impairs response-to-change based on distal cues in the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 46(3) Sep 1989, 355-359.
  • Pouget, A., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1994). A neural model of the cortical representation of egocentric distance: Cerebral Cortex Vol 4(3) May-Jun 1994, 314-329.
  • Prazdny, K. (1981). A note on "optical motions as information for unsigned depth." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 7(2) Apr 1981, 286-289.
  • Predebon, J. (1990). Relative distance judgments of familiar and unfamiliar objects viewed under representatively natural conditions: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 47(4) Apr 1990, 342-348.
  • Predebon, J. (1993). The familiar-size cue to distance and stereoscopic depth perception: Perception Vol 22(8) 1993, 985-995.
  • Predebon, J. (1994). Convergence responses to monocularly viewed objects: Implications for distance perception: Perception Vol 23(3) 1994, 303-319.
  • Predebon, J. (2002). Viewing distance and estimates of duration: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(1) Aug 2002, 326-328.
  • Prentice, W. C. H., Krimsky, J., & Barker, S. (1951). The roles of pattern and apparent distance in determining the color of areas seen through transparencies: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 42(3) Sep 1951, 201-206.
  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Distance Perception: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 15(3) Jun 2006, 131-135.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance: Psychological Science Vol 14(2) Mar 2003, 106-112.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2006). A Final Reply to Hutchison and Loomis: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 346-348.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2006). Reply to Hutchison and Loomis: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 340-342.
  • Provines, W. F., Rahe, A. J., Block, M. G., Pena, T., & et al. (1992). Yellow lens effects upon visual acquisition performance: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 63(7) Jul 1992, 561-564.
  • Pufall, P. B., & Dunbar, C. (1992). Perceiving whether or not the world affords stepping onto and over: A developmental study: Ecological Psychology Vol 4(1) 1992, 17-38.
  • Purdy, J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Distance judgment by the method of fractionation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(6) Dec 1955, 374-380.
  • Radvansky, G. A., Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Irwin, D. E. (1995). Uncertainty in estimating distances from memory: Memory & Cognition Vol 23(5) Sep 1995, 596-606.
  • Rafter, J. P. (1994). Prediction of time-to-arrival in relation to dual motion and distraction. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Raghubir, P., & Krishna, A. (1996). As the crow flies: Bias in consumers' map-based distance judgments: Journal of Consumer Research Vol 23(1) Jun 1996, 26-39.
  • Redlick, F. P., Jenkin, M., & Harris, L. R. (2001). Humans can use optic flow to estimate distance of travel: Vision Research Vol 41(2) Jan 2001, 213-219.
  • Reed, C. F. (1985). More things in heaven and earth: A reply to Loftus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 114(1) Mar 1985, 122-124.
  • Reed, C. F. (1996). The immediacy of the moon illusion: Perception Vol 25(11) 1996, 1295-1300.
  • Reed, C. F., & Krupinski, E. A. (1992). The target in the celestial (moon) illusion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 18(1) Feb 1992, 247-256.
  • Reese, H. W. (1962). The distance effect in transposition in the intermediate size problem: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 55(4) Aug 1962, 528-531.
  • Reynolds, R. I. (1989). Auditory and visual distance estimation after active or passive visual deprivation: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 27(3) May 1989, 237-238.
  • Richardson, A. R., & Waller, D. (2007). Interaction with an immersive virtual environment corrects users' distance estimates: Human Factors Vol 49(3) Jun 2007, 507-517.
  • Rieser, J. J., Ashmead, D. H., Talor, C. R., & Youngquist, G. A. (1990). Visual perception and the guidance of locomotion without vision to previously seen targets: Perception Vol 19(5) 1990, 675-689.
  • Rinck, M., & Bower, G. H. (2000). Temporal and spatial distance in situation models: Memory & Cognition Vol 28(8) Dec 2000, 1310-1320.
  • Rinck, M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G. H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The metrics of spatial situation models: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 23(3) May 1997, 622-637.
  • Rival, C., Olivier, I., Ceyte, H., & Bard, C. (2004). Age-related differences in the visual processes implied in perception and action: Distance and location parameters: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 87(2) Feb 2004, 107-124.
  • Roazzi, A., & de Castro Filho, J. A. (2001). The development of the concept of time as integration of distance and speed: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 14(3) 2001, 497-503.
  • Roazzi, A., Santos, A. R. R., & de Castro Filho, J. A. (1994). Time as integration of distance and velocity: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 10 (1) Jan-Apr 1994, 49-58.
  • Rochat, P. (1995). Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 59(2) Apr 1995, 317-333.
  • Rochat, P., Goubet, N., & Senders, S. J. (1999). To reach or not to be reach? Perception of body effectivities by young infants: Infant and Child Development Vol 8(3) Sep 1999, 129-148.
  • Rochat, P., & Wraga, M. (1997). An account of the systematic error in judging what is reachable: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 199-212.
  • Rock, I., & Ebenholtz, S. (1959). The relational determination of perceived size: Psychological Review Vol 66(6) Nov 1959, 387-401.
  • Rock, I., & McDermott, W. (1964). The perception of visual angle: Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam 22(2) 1964, 119-134.
  • Roggeman, C., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities: Cognition Vol 105(2) 2007, 380-394.
  • Roscoe, S. N. (1997). Size-distance variance and eye accommodation: The ways of an investigator: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 8(1) 1997, 75-81.
  • Roscoe, S. N., Hasler, S. G., & Dougherty, D. J. (1966). Flight by periscope: Making takeoffs and landings; the influence of image magnification, practice, and various conditions of flight: Human Factors 8(1) 1966, 13-40.
  • Rosenblum, L. D., Wuestefeld, A. P., & Anderson, K. L. (1996). Auditory reachability: An affordance approach to the perception of sound source distance: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(1) 1996, 1-24.
  • Rosenfield, M., & Ciuffreda, K. J. (1990). Proximal and cognitively-induced accommodation: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(3) Jul 1990, 252-256.
  • Ross, J., & Cliff, N. (1964). A generalization of the interpoint distance model: Psychometrika 29(2) 1964, 167-176.
  • Rossano, M. J., & Moak, J. (1998). Spatial representations acquired from computer models: Cognitive load, orientation specificity and the acquisition of survey knowledge: British Journal of Psychology Vol 89(3) Aug 1998, 481-497.
  • Roumes, C., Meehan, J. W., Plantier, J., & Menu, J.-P. (2001). Distance estimation in a 3-D imaging display: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 11(4) Oct 2001, 381-396.
  • Rovamo, J., Franssila, R., & Nasanen, R. (1992). Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency, viewing distance and eccentricity with and without spatial noise: Vision Research Vol 32(4) Apr 1992, 631-637.
  • Royden, C. S. (1994). In defense of "Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or absolute size." Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 733-735.
  • Rozhkova, G. I., Podugolnikova, T. A., & Vasiljeva, N. N. (2005). Visual acuity in 5-7-year-old children: Individual variability and dependence on observation distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 25(1) Jan 2005, 66-80.
  • Ruiguang, L. (2005). The perspective theory of space perception: Psychological Science (China) Vol 28(2) Mar 2005, 509-511.
  • Rushton, S. K., & Duke, P. A. (2007). The use of direction and distance information in the perception of approach trajectory: Vision Research Vol 47(7) Mar 2007, 899-912.
  • Russell, M. K. (1997). Perceiving distances of nearby sound sources: An investigation of Marler's canonical features of locatable sounds: Ecological Psychology Vol 9(4) 1997, 299-322.
  • Saberi, K., Perrott, D. R., & Sadralodabai, T. (1991). Judgments of lateral distance using transients presented with interaural differences of time: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(1) Jan 1991, 59-61.
  • Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, W. J., & Staplin, L. J. (1980). Reference points in spatial cognition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 6(5) Sep 1980, 516-528.
  • Sahm, C. S., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Thompson, W. B., & Willemsen, P. (2005). Throwing versus walking as indicators of distance perception in similar real and virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(1) Jan 2005, 35-45.
  • Sakamoto, T., & Okaichi, H. (1997). The effects of a fimbria-fornix lesion on distance discrimination in rats: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 68(1) Apr 1997, 38-42.
  • Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 6(4) Apr 1998, 351-360.
  • Santini, F., & Rucci, M. (2007). Active estimation of distance in a robotic system that replicates human eye movement: Robotics and Autonomous Systems Vol 55(2) Feb 2007, 107-121.
  • Sasaki, H., & Nagasaki, H. (1989). The mental distance: Its differences in educational circumstances: Journal of Human Development Vol 25 Jan 1989, 1-10.
  • Saunders, J. A. (1999). Perception of slant and distance from texture. (vision). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Sawyerr, E. S. (1992). Conservation of distance in African children from rural and urban schools: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 74(3, Pt 2), Spec Issue Jun 1992, 1019-1025.
  • Scarf, P., & Grehan, P. (2005). An empirical basis for route choice in cycling: Journal of Sports Sciences Vol 23(9) Sep 2005, 919-925.
  • Schmidt, H.-E. (1964). Relation of the narrowing of the visual field with an increase in distance to manifest anxiety: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 68(4) Oct 1964, 334-336.
  • Schmitt, M., Postma, A., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2001). Cross-modal exogenous attention and distance effects in vision and hearing: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 343-368.
  • Schuster, S., Strauss, R., & Gotz, K. G. (2002). Virtual reality techniques resolve the visual cues used by fruit flies to evaluate object distances: Current Biology Vol 12(18) Sep 2002, 1591-1594.
  • Schutte, A. R., Spencer, J. P., & Schoner, G. (2003). Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development: Child Development Vol 74(5) Sep-Oct 2003, 1393-1417.
  • Schwartz, M. (1999). Haptic perception of the distance walked when blindfolded: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 25(3) Jun 1999, 852-865.
  • Segui-Gomez, M., Levy, J., & Graham, J. D. (1998). Airbag safety and the distance of the driver from the steering wheel: New England Journal of Medicine Vol 339(2) Jul 1998, 132-133.
  • Segui-Gomez, M., Levy, J., Roman, H., Thompson, K. M., McCabe, K., & Graham, J. D. (1999). Driver distance from the steering wheel: Perception and objective measurement: American Journal of Public Health Vol 89(7) Jul 1999, 1109-1111.
  • Seizova-Cajic, T. (2003). The role of perceived relative position in pointing to objects apparently shifted by depth-contrast: Spatial Vision Vol 16(3-4) 2003, 325-346.
  • Servos, P. R. (1995). Binocular vision and the kinematics of human prehension. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Shebilske, W. L., Proffitt, D. R., & Fisher, S. K. (1984). Efferent factors in natural event perception can be rationalized and verified: A reply to Turvey and Solomon: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 10(3) Jun 1984, 455-460.
  • Shechter, S., Hochstein, S., & Hillman, P. (1988). Shape similarity and distance disparity as apparent motion correspondence cues: Vision Research Vol 28(9) 1988, 1013-1021.
  • Shieh, K.-K., & Lee, D.-S. (2007). Preferred viewing distance and screen angle of electronic paper displays: Applied Ergonomics Vol 38(5) Sep 2007, 601-608.
  • Shimono, K., Nakamizo, S., & Higashiyama, A. (2000). Distance perception and vergence: Japanese Psychological Review Vol 43(3) 2000, 335-348.
  • Shipley, W. C., Coffin, J. I., & Hadsell, K. C. (1945). Affective distance and other factors determining reaction time in judgments of color preference: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 35(3) Jun 1945, 206-215.
  • Shoben, E. J., Cech, C. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1983). Choosing model of comparative judgment involving numerical reference points: A rejoinder to Holyoak: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 9(3) Jun 1983, 491-495.
  • Shontz, F. C., & McNish, R. D. (1972). The human body as stimulus object: Estimates of distances between body landmarks: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(1) Sep 1972, 20-24.
  • Siegel, A. W., Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1979). Children's ability to make bidirectional distance comparisons: The advantage of thinking ahead: Developmental Psychology Vol 15(6) Nov 1979, 656-657.
  • Simpson, P. J., & Nation, K. J. (1997). Head orientation and binaural depth perception. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Co.
  • Singh, S. J. (1987). Role of visual experience in short term recall of movements: Studies on congenitally blind subjects: Indian Journal of Current Psychological Research Vol 2(1) Apr 1987, 37-46.
  • Sipes, D. E. (1997). Hyperstereopsis as an attenuator for perceptual depth compression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Sirkar, A. K. (1963). Depth perception: Practice-effect and effect of rod-width on distance discrimination: Indian Journal of Psychology 38(3) 1963, 101-105.
  • Smeets, J. B. J., Brenner, E., Trebuchet, S., & Mestre, D. R. (1996). Is judging time-to-contact based on 'tau'? : Perception Vol 25(5) 1996, 583-590.
  • Smith, O. W. (1958). Distance constancy: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 55(4) Apr 1958, 388-389.
  • Smith, W. M. (1952). Gilinsky's theory of visual size and distance: Psychological Review Vol 59(3) May 1952, 239-243.
  • Smyth, M. M., Katamba, J., & Peacock, K. A. (2004). Development of prehension between 5 and 10 years of age: Distance scaling, grip aperture, and sight of the hand: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 36(1) Mar 2004, 91-103.
  • Sobel, E. C. (1990). Distance perception by motion parallax in the locust: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 14(3) Aug 1988, 404-427.
  • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Gravitational and muscular variables in perceiving rod extent by wielding: Ecological Psychology Vol 1(3) 1989, 265-300.
  • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: Haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(1) Feb 1989, 58-68.
  • Solomon, R. L. (1949). The role of effort in the performance of a distance discrimination: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 39(1) Feb 1949, 73-83.
  • Spence, K. W., & Grice, G. R. (1942). The role of final and sub-goals in distance discrimination by the white rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 34(2) Oct 1942, 179-184.
  • Spencer, J. P. (1998). The development dynamics of reaching errors. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Spetch, M. L., Rust, T. B., Kamil, A. C., & Jones, J. E. (2003). Searching by rules: Pigeons' (Columba livia) landmark-based search according to constant bearing or constant distance: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 117(2) Jun 2003, 123-132.
  • Srinivasan, M. B., Zhang, S., Altwein, M., & Tautz, J. (2000). Honeybee navigation: Nature and calibration of the "odometer." Science Vol 287(5454) Feb 2000, 851-853.
  • Srinivasan, M. V. (1992). Distance perception in insects: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 1(1) Feb 1992, 22-26.
  • Srinivasan, M. V., Lehrer, M., Kirchner, W. H., & Zhang, S. W. (1991). Range perception through apparent image speed in freely flying honeybees: Visual Neuroscience Vol 6(5) May 1991, 519-535.
  • Srinivasan, M. V., Zhang, S. W., & Lehrer, M. (1998). Honeybee navigation: Odometry with monocular input: Animal Behaviour Vol 56(5) Nov 1998, 1245-1259.
  • Stamps, A. E., III, & Krishnan, V. V. (2004). Perceived Enclosure of Space, Angle Above Observer, and Distance to Boundary: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 99(3,Part2) Dec 2004, 1187-1192.
  • Stanley, G. (1966). The apparent size of entoptic after-images: Psychonomic Science Vol 4(8) 1966, 289-290.
  • Stapleton, L., Ward, N. J., Parkes, A. M., Kiefer, R. J., Wolffsohn, J. S., Edgar, G. K., et al. (1998). Head up displays. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science Ltd.
  • Starch, D., & Crawford, A. L. (1909). Minor studies from the psychological laboratory of the Wellesley College: The perception of the distance of sound: Psychological Review Vol 16(6) Nov 1909, 427-430.
  • Steenhuis, R. E. (1988). Target-directed locomotion: A critique of the short-term memory for spatial locations proposed by Thomson: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1988). The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: Does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? : Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 20(4) Dec 1988, 399-415.
  • Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1989). "The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: Does an accurate memory for spatial location exist?": Errata: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 21(2) Jun 1989, 177.
  • Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2005). Distances appear different on hills: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(6) Aug 2005, 1052-1060.
  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2003). Affordances Are Enough: Reply to Chemero et al. (2003): Ecological Psychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2003, 29-36.
  • Swanston, M. T., Wade, N. J., Ono, H., & Shibuta, K. (1992). The interaction of perceived distance with the perceived direction of visual motion during movements of the eyes and the head: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 52(6) Dec 1992, 705-713.
  • Szuster-Zbrojewicz, A. (1989). Asymmetry in assessment of we others distances and exocentric engagement in others' problems: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 41-54.
  • Szuster-Zbrojewicz, A. (1989). Cognitive isolation of self and the asymmetry phenomenon in we others distance assessment: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 55-71.
  • Taieb-Maimon, M., & Shinar, D. (2001). Minimum and comfortable driving headways: Reality versus perception: Human Factors Vol 43(1) Spr 2001, 159-172.
  • Taroyan, N. A., Buckley, D., Porrill, J., & Frisby, J. P. (2000). Exploring sequential stereopsis for co-planarity tasks: Vision Research Vol 40(24) 2000, 3373-3390.
  • Tausch, R. (1953). Binocular space perception: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 1 1953, 394-421.
  • Taylor, J. G. (1965). The behavioral basis of perceived size and distance: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 19(1) 1965, 1-14.
  • Taylor, M. M. (1962). The distance paradox of the figural after-effect in aduitory localization: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 16(4) 1962, 278-282.
  • Taylor, M. M. (1965). Non-additivity of perceived distance with the Mueller-Lyer figure: Perceptual and Motor Skills 20(3, Pt 2) 1965, 1964.
  • Taylor, P. E., Scott, G. B., & Rose, P. (2003). The ability of domestic hens to jump between horizontal perches: Effects of light intensity and perch colour: Applied Animal Behaviour Science Vol 83(2) Sep 2003, 99-108.
  • Thompson, P. (2002). Eyes wide apart: Overestimating interpupillary distance: Perception Vol 31(6) 2002, 651-656.
  • Thompson, S. G., McConnell, D. S., Slocum, J. S., & Bohan, M. (2007). Kinematic analysis of multiple constraints on a pointing task: Human Movement Science Vol 26(1) Feb 2007, 11-26.
  • Thompson, W. B., Dilda, V., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2007). Absolute distance perception to locations off the ground plane: Perception Vol 36(11) 2007, 1559-1571.
  • Tibau, S., Willems, B., Van Den Bergh, E., & Wagemans, J. (2001). The role of the centre of projection in the estimation of slant from texture of planar surfaces: Perception Vol 30(2) 2001, 185-193.
  • Todd, J. T., & Norman, J. F. (2003). The visual perception of 3-D shape from multiple cues: Are observers capable of perceiving metric structure: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 65(1) Jan 2003, 31-47.
  • Todorovic, D. (1989). Orientation anisotropy of spatial discrimination on the skin: Revija za Psihologiju Vol 19(1-2) Dec 1989, 9-16.
  • Torrey, H. B. (1917). General reviews and summaries: Tropism and instinctive activities: Psychological Bulletin Vol 14(8) Aug 1917, 265-276.
  • Tozawa, J. (1992). Perceived size and perceived distance of a growing pattern on the frontal plane: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 10(2) Mar 1992, 99-107.
  • Tozawa, J., & Oyama, T. (2006). Effects of motion parallax and perspective cues on perceived size and distance: Perception Vol 35(8) 2006, 1007-1023.
  • Tresilian, J. R. (1994). A note on "Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or size." Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 727-731.
  • Tresilian, J. R., & Mon-Williams, M. (1999). A curious illusion suggests complex cue interactions in distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 25(3) Jun 1999, 677-687.
  • Trotter, Y. (1995). Cortical representation of visual three-dimensional space: Perception Vol 24(3) 1995, 287-298.
  • Trueman, M. T., & Wilson, A. E. (1989). Examination of apparent extent as an explanation of the Poggendorff effect: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(6) Jun 1989, 494-500.
  • Tsuji, K., Hayashibe, K., Hara, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Visuo-motor development which causes detection of visual depth from motion and density cues: Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift fur Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie Vol 59(2) Jun 2000, 102-107.
  • Turatto, M., Galfano, G., Gardini, S., & Mascetti, G. G. (2004). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: An empirical comparison of display-size and distance methods: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 57A(2) 2004, 297-324.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Solomon, J. (1984). Visually perceiving distance: A comment on Shebilske, Karmiohl, and Proffitt (1983): Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 10(3) Jun 1984, 449-454.
  • Ungar, S., Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1997). Teaching visually impaired children to make distance judgments from a tactile map: Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness Vol 91(2) Mar-Apr 1997, 163-174.
  • Ungs, T. J., & Sangal, S. P. (1990). Perception of near earth altitudes by pilots: Ascending vs. descending over both a land and water surface: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 61(12) Dec 1990, 1098-1101.
  • van de Grind, W. A., Verstraten, F. A. J., & van der Smagt, M. J. (2003). Influence of viewing distance on aftereffects of moving random pixel arrays: Vision Research Vol 43(23) Oct 2003, 2413-2426.
  • Van Dillen, L. R. (1995). Memory for spatial information used for aimed limb movements. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • van Ee, R., Banks, M. S., & Backus, B. T. (1999). Perceived visual direction near an occluder: Vision Research Vol 39(24) Dec 1999, 4085-4097.
  • van Ee, R., & Erkelens, C. J. (2000). Is there an interaction between perceived direction and perceived aspect ratio in stereoscopic vision? : Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(5) Jul 2000, 910-926.
  • Van Erp, J. B. F., Van Veen, H. A. H. C., Jansen, C., & Dobbins, T. (2005). Waypoint navigation with a vibrotactile waist belt: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(2) Apr 2005, 106-117.
  • Varnava, A., McCarthy, M., & Beaumont, J. G. (2002). Line bisection in normal adults: Direction of attentional bias for near and far space: Neuropsychologia Vol 40(8) 2002, 1372-1378.
  • Vartanyan, I. A., & Andreeva, I. G. (2007). A psychophysiological study of auditory illusions of approach and withdrawal in the context of the perceptual environment: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 10(2) Nov 2007, 266-276.
  • Vianin, P., Baumberger, B., & Fluckiger, M. (2004). Distance estimation in a dynamic simulated environment: A visual field dependence problem? : Perception Vol 33(5) 2004, 561-568.
  • Viguier, A., Clement, G., & Trotter, Y. (2001). Distance perception within near visual space: Perception Vol 30(1) 2001, 115-124.
  • Virsu, V., & Hari, R. (1996). Cortical magnification, scale invariance and visual ecology: Vision Research Vol 36(18) Sep 1996, 2971-2977.
  • von der Emde, G., Schwarz, S., Gomez, L., Budelli, R., & Grant, K. (1998). Electric fish measure distance in the dark: Nature Vol 395(6705) Oct 1998, 890-894.
  • von Hofsten, C., & Lee, D. N. (1994). Measuring with the optic sphere. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Voss, J., Keary, N., & Bischof, H.-J. (2007). The use of the geomagnetic field for short distance orientation in zebra finches: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 18(10) Jul 2007, 1053-1057.
  • Vujnovich, D. J. (1988). Perception of size and distance: Progressive cue reduction and the size-distance invariance hypothesis: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Wagemans, J., & Tibau, S. (1999). Visual measurement of relative distances between three collinear dots rotating in a slanted plane: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 267-282.
  • Walker, J. T., Rupich, R. C., & Powell, J. L. (1989). The vista paradox: A natural visual illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(1) Jan 1989, 43-48.
  • Wallach, H. (1985). Learned stimulation in space and motion perception: American Psychologist Vol 40(4) Apr 1985, 399-404.
  • Wallach, H., & Norris, C. M. (1963). Accommodation as a distance cue: American Journal of Psychology 76(4) 1963, 659-664.
  • Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., & Beall, A. C. (2000). Place learning in humans: The role of distance and direction information: Spatial Cognition and Computation Vol 2(4) 2000, 333-354.
  • Wang, S., & Zhang, M. (1990). Tactile discrimination of distance between two end-points of a curved line: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 22(2) 1990, 135-140.
  • Wang, S., & Zhang, M. (1995). Tactile discrimination of distance between 2 points under the effect of a nearby extra point: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 27(2) 1995, 113-120.
  • Wann, J. P. (1996). Anticipating arrival: Is the tau margin a specious theory? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(4) Aug 1996, 1031-1048.
  • Watson, J. S., Banks, M. S., von Hofsten, C., & Royden, C. S. (1992). Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or absolute size: Perception Vol 21(1) 1992, 69-76.
  • Weaver, J. A. (2007). Dorsal stream visual regions use monocular depth cues to compute object distance. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Wei, M., & Angelaki, D. E. (2004). Viewing distance dependence of the vestibulo-ocular reflex during translation: Extra-otolith influences: Vision Research Vol 44(9) Apr 2004, 933-942.
  • Whisenand, T. G., & Emurian, H. H. (1996). Effects of angle of approach on cursor movement with a mouse: Consideration of Fitts' Law: Computers in Human Behavior Vol 12(3) Fal 1996, 481-495.
  • Wickelgren, E. A., McConnell, D. S., & Bingham, G. P. (2000). Reaching measures of monocular distance perception: Forward versus side-to-side head movements and haptic feedback: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(5) Jul 2000, 1051-1059.
  • Wiener, J. M., & Mallot, H. A. (2003). 'Fine-to-Coarse' Route Planning and Navigation in Regionalized Environments: Spatial Cognition and Computation Vol 3(4) 2003, 331-358.
  • Wiest, W. M., & Bell, B. (1985). Stevens's exponent for psychophysical scaling of perceived, remembered, and inferred distance: Psychological Bulletin Vol 98(3) Nov 1985, 457-470.
  • Wilberg, R. B., & Girouard, Y. (1976). Information on distances, effect of stimuli length and short-term motor memory: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 30(2) Jun 1976, 63-71.
  • Wilcox, T., Rosser, R., & Nadel, L. (1994). Representation of object location in 6.5-month-old infants: Cognitive Development Vol 9(2) Apr-Jun 1994, 193-209.
  • Wilkening, F., & Martin, C. (2004). How to speed up to be in time: Action-judgment dissociations in children and adults: Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift fur Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie Vol 63(1) Mar 2004, 17-29.
  • Witmer, B. G., & Sadowski, W. J., Jr. (1998). Nonvisually guided locomotion to a previously viewed target in real and virtual environments: Human Factors Vol 40(3) Sep 1998, 478-488.
  • Witt, J. K. (2007). Reachability influences perception via motor simulation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent: Perception Vol 33(5) 2004, 577-590.
  • Wohlgemuth, S., Ronacher, B., & Wehner, R. (2001). Ant odometry in the third dimension: Nature Vol 411(6839) Jun 2001, 795-798.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1964). Changes in distance judgments as a function of corrected and noncorrected practice: Perceptual and Motor Skills 19(2) 1964, 403-413.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1965). Texture of the stimulus field and age as variables in the perception of relative distance in photographic slides: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2(2) 1965, 163-177.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1966). Smith and Smith's developmental studies of spatial judgments: A note: Perceptual and Motor Skills 23(1) 1966, 137-138.
  • Wrinch, F. S. (1907). Review of Ueber die Beziehungen von Zeitschatzung und Bewegungsemp findung: Psychological Bulletin Vol 4(4) Apr 1907, 119-120.
  • Wrinch, F. S. (1909). The Effect of Suggestion upon the Reproduction of Triangles and Point Distances: Psychological Bulletin Vol 6(9) Sep 1909, 319-320.
  • Wu, B. (2004). The visual perception of distance in action space. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Wu, B., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2007). Inaccurate representation of the ground surface beyond a texture boundary: Perception Vol 36(5) 2007, 703-721.
  • Wu, B., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2007). The linear perspective information in ground surface representation and distance judgment: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 69(5) Jul 2007, 654-672.
  • Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2004). Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information: Nature Vol 428(6978) Mar 2004, 73-77.
  • Wu, J. (2007). Construction of the ground surface representation for distance perception in the intermediate distance range. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Yanase, R. (2001). The effects of spatial information for cognitive distance in an architectural space: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 72(1) Apr 2001, 44-50.
  • Yang, Q., & Kapoula, Z. (2003). Binocular coordination of saccades at far and at near in children and in adults: Journal of Vision Vol 3(8) 2003, 554-561.
  • Yela, M. (1996). The perception of causation at a distance: Psicothema Vol 8(Suppl) 1996, 53-71.
  • Yonas, A., Granrud, C. E., & Pettersen, L. (1985). Infants' sensitivity to relative size information for distance: Developmental Psychology Vol 21(1) Jan 1985, 161-167.
  • Yonas, A., & Hartman, B. (1993). Perceiving the affordance of contact in four- and five-month-old infants: Child Development Vol 64(1) Feb 1993, 298-308.
  • Young, P. T. (1931). The role of head movements in auditory localization: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 14(2) Apr 1931, 95-124.
  • Yun-Chiu, F. (1964). Convergence, interocular distance, and perception of distance: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 3 1964, 203-210.
  • Zahorik, P. (2002). Assessing auditory distance perception using virtual acoustics: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(4) Apr 2002, 1832-1846.
  • Zahorik, P. A. (1999). Experiments in auditory distance perception. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Zakay, D., Hayduk, L. A., & Tsal, Y. (1992). Personal space and distance misperception: Implications of a novel observation: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 30(1) Jan 1992, 33-35.
  • Zhou, Z. (1996). The development of American and Chinese children's understanding of distance, time, and speed interrelations. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ziskind, D. (1987). Linear perspective is not linear: Compensation for visual field spansion during movement: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Zollner, P. A., & Lima, S. L. (1999). Illumination and the perception of remote habitat patches by white-footed mice: Animal Behaviour Vol 58(3) Sep 1999, 489-500.
Advertisement