Fandom

Psychology Wiki

Distance perception

Redirected from Distance discrimination

34,203pages on
this wiki
Add New Page
Talk1 Share

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


Distance perception is a form of spatial perception using primarily distance receptors.



See alsoEdit

ReferencesEdit

  • Abraham, A. (1994). Reproduction of angular distances in the congenitally blind: Psycho-Lingua Vol 24(2) Jul 1994, 99-104.
  • Abrams, R. A., Van Dillen, L., & Stemmons, V. (1994). Multiple sources of spatial information for aimed limb movements. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Acredolo, C. (1989). Assessing children's understanding of time, speed and distance interrelations. Oxford, England: North-Holland.
  • Acredolo, C., & Schmid, J. (1981). The understanding of relative speeds, distances, and durations of movement: Developmental Psychology Vol 17(4) Jul 1981, 490-493.
  • Actis-Grosso, R., & Stucchi, N. (2003). Shifting the start: Backward mislocation of the initial position of a motion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 29(3) Jun 2003, 675-691.
  • Adam, J. J., Mol, R., Pratt, J., & Fischer, M. H. (2006). Moving Farther but Faster: An Exception to Fitts's Law: Psychological Science Vol 17(9) Sep 2006, 794-798.
  • Adelson, D. (1963). The relational influence of size on judgments of distance: Journal of General Psychology 69(2) 1963, 319-333.
  • Agnew, N. M., Pyke, S., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1966). Absolute judgment of distance as a function of induced muscle tension, exposure time, and feedback: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 71(5) May 1966, 649-654.
  • Aleksander, A. K. (1996). Glare mitigation in night driving using partially tinted lenses. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Allen, B. P. (1999). Shadows as sources of cues for distance of shadow-casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 89(2) Oct 1999, 571-584.
  • Allen, B. P. (2000). Angles of shadows as cues for judging the distance of shadow casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 90(3,Pt1) Jun 2000, 864-866.
  • Allen, B. P. (2001). Lighting position and judgments of distance of shadow-casting objects: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 93(1) Aug 2001, 127-130.
  • Allen, G. L. (1988). The acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity: Psychological Research Vol 50(3) 1988, 183-190.
  • Allen, G. L., & Rashotte, M. A. (2006). Training metric accuracy in distance estimation skill: Pictures versus words: Applied Cognitive Psychology Vol 20(2) Mar 2006, 171-186.
  • Allen, G. L., Siegel, A. W., & Rosinski, R. R. (1978). The role of perceptual context in structuring spatial knowledge: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 4(6) Nov 1978, 617-630.
  • Alyan, S. H., & Jander, R. (1997). Interplay of directional navigation mechanisms as a function of near-goal distance: Experiments with the house mouse: Behavioural Processes Vol 41(3) Dec 1997, 245-255.
  • Anderson, J. R., Montant, M., & Schmitt, D. (1996). Rhesus monkeys fail to use gaze direction as an experimenter-given cue in an object-choice task: Behavioural Processes Vol 37(1) Aug 1996, 47-55.
  • Ankomah, P. K., Crompton, J. L., & Baker, D. (1996). Influence of cognitive distance in vacation choice: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 23(1) 1996, 138-150.
  • Anooshian, L. J., & Wilson, K. L. (1977). Distance distortions in memory for spatial locations: Child Development Vol 48(4) Dec 1977, 1704-1707.
  • Armstrong, L., & Marks, L. E. (1999). Haptic perception of linear extent: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(6) Aug 1999, 1211-1226.
  • Ashley, A., & Carlson, L. A. (2007). Encoding direction when interpreting proximal terms: Language and Cognitive Processes Vol 22(7) Nov 2007, 1-24.
  • Ashley, M. L. (1903). An Investigation of the Process of Judgment as Involved in Estimating Distances: Psychological Review Vol 10(3) May 1903, 283-295.
  • Ashmead, D. H., Davis, D. L., & Northington, A. (1995). Contribution of listeners' approaching motion to auditory distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 21(2) Apr 1995, 239-256.
  • Atkins, J. E., Jacobs, R. A., & Knill, D. C. (2003). Experience-dependent visual cue recalibration based on discrepancies between visual and haptic percepts: Vision Research Vol 43(25) Nov 2003, 2603-2613.
  • Avraamides, M. N., Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., & Golledge, R. G. (2004). Functional Equivalence of Spatial Representations Derived From Vision and Language: Evidence From Allocentric Judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 30(4) Jul 2004, 801-814.
  • Aznar-Casanova, J. A., Matsushima, E. H., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2006). One-Dimensional and Multi-Dimensional Studies of the Exocentric Distance Estimates in Frontoparallel Plane, Virtual Space, and Outdoor Open Field: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 273-284.
  • Backus, B. T., & Banks, M. S. (1999). Estimator reliability and distance scaling in stereoscopic slant perception: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 217-242.
  • Baden, D. A., Warwick-Evans, L., & Lakomy, J. (2004). Am I nearly there? The effect of anticipated running distance on perceived exertion and attentional focus: Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology Vol 26(2) Jun 2004, 215-231.
  • Bahcall, D. O., & Kowler, E. (1999). Attentional interference at small spatial separations: Vision Research Vol 39(1) Jan 1999, 71-86.
  • Bailes, S. M. (1996). Effects of processing time and stimulus density on apparent width of the Oppel-Kundt illusion. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Baird, J. C. (1963). Retinal and assumed size cues as determinants of size and distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 66(2) Aug 1963, 155-162.
  • Baird, J. C. (1982). The moon illusion: II. A reference theory: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 111(3) Sep 1982, 304-315.
  • Baird, J. C., & Wagner, M. (1982). The moon illusion: I. How high is the sky? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 111(3) Sep 1982, 296-303.
  • Baird, J. C., Wagner, M., & Fuld, K. (1990). A simple but powerful theory of the moon illusion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 16(3) Aug 1990, 675-677.
  • Baker, C. A. (1954). Interpolation accuracy as a function of visual angle between scale marks: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(6) Jun 1954, 433-436.
  • Balakrishnan, J. D., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Lederman, S. J. (1989). Length distortion of temporally extended visual displays: Similarity to haptic spatial perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 46(4) Oct 1989, 387-394.
  • Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., & Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements: Vision Research Vol 36(3) Feb 1996, 431-443.
  • Banton, P., Thompson, P., & Quinlan, P. T. (2001). Effect of geometric field of view on stereoscopic spatial judgments: Human Factors Vol 43(3) Fal 2001, 405-414.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Haptically perceiving size at a distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 122(3) Sep 1993, 347-370.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1995). Does perceived size depend on perceived distance? An argument from extended haptic perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 57(2) Feb 1995, 216-224.
  • Barac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). Anisotropy in the extended haptic perception of longitudinal distances: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(8) Nov 1999, 1522-1536.
  • Barber, A. V. (1990). Visual mechanisms and predictors of far field visual task performance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1988). Young children's conception of distance: Developmental Psychology Vol 24(4) Jul 1988, 532-541.
  • Baumberger, B., & Fluckiger, M. (2004). The development of distance estimation in optic flow: Perception Vol 33(9) 2004, 1081-1099.
  • Baumberger, B., Fluckiger, M., Paquette, M., Bergeron, J., & Delorme, A. (2005). Perception of relative distance in a driving simulator: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 47(3) Sep 2005, 230-237.
  • Beebe-Center, J. G., Mead, L. C., Wagoner, K. S., & Hoffman, A. C. (1945). Visual acuity and distance of observation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 35(6) Dec 1945, 473-484.
  • Begault, D. R. (1991). Preferred sound intensity increase for sensation of half distance: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 72(3, Pt 1) Jun 1991, 1019-1029.
  • Beggs, W. D., & et al. (1972). The accuracy of non-visual aiming: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 24(4) Nov 1972, 515-523.
  • Berberian, B., Sarrazin, J.-C., & Giraudo, M.-D. (2007). Tau and Kappa effects: The case of space-like-extent frequencies: Ecological Psychology Vol 19(3) 2007, 215-238.
  • Bereby-Meyer, Y., Leiser, D., & Meyer, J. (1999). Perception of artificial stereoscopic stimuli from an incorrect viewing point: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(8) Nov 1999, 1555-1563.
  • Berkeley, G. (1948). An essay toward a new theory of vision, 1709. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Bertamini, M., & Parks, T. E. (2005). On what people know about images on mirrors: Cognition Vol 98(1) Nov 2005, 85-104.
  • Bertamini, M., Yang, T. L., & Proffitt, D. R. (1998). Relative size perception at a distance is best at eye level: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(4) May 1998, 673-682.
  • Berthoz, A., Israel, I., Georges-Francois, P., Grasso, R., & et al. (1995). Spatial memory of body linear displacement: What is being stored? : Science Vol 269(5220) Jul 1995, 95-98.
  • Berti, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Coding near and far space. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2005). The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(5) Jul 2005, 802-815.
  • Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2006). The ground dominance effect in the perception of relative distance in 3-D scenes is mainly due to characteristics of the ground surface: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(8) Nov 2006, 1297-1309.
  • Bigel, M. G., & Ellard, C. G. (2000). The contribution of nonvisual information to simple place navigation and distance estimation: An examination of path integration: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale Vol 54(3) Sep 2000, 172-185.
  • Bigelow, A. E. (1996). Blind and sighted children's spatial knowledge of their home environments: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 19(4) Dec 1996, 797-816.
  • Bilandzic, H. (2006). The Perception of Distance in the Cultivation Process: A Theoretical Consideration of the Relationship Between Television Content, Processing Experience, and Perceived Distance: Communication Theory Vol 16(3) Aug 2006, 333-355.
  • Bilderback, L. G., Taylor, R. E., & Thor, D. H. (1964). Distance perception in darkness: Science 145(Whole No 3629) 1964, 294-295.
  • Bingham, G. P. (2005). Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision: Ecological Psychology Vol 17(2) 2005, 55-74.
  • Bingham, G. P., Crowell, J. A., & Todd, J. T. (2004). Distortions of distance and shape are not produced by a single continuous transformation of reach space: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(1) Jan 2004, 152-169.
  • Bingham, G. P., McConnell, D. S., & Muchisky, M. M. (2001). Commentary on Jacobs and Michaels (2001): Calibration and perceptual learning in event perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(3) Apr 2001, 572-574.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Pagano, C. C. (1998). The necessity of a perception-action approach to definite distance perception: Monocular distance perception to guide reaching: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 24(1) Feb 1998, 145-168.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Stassen, M. G. (1994). Monocular egocentric distance information generated by head movement: Ecological Psychology Vol 6(3) 1994, 219-238.
  • Bingham, G. P., Zaal, F., Robin, D., & Shull, J. A. (2000). Distortions in definite distance and shape perception as measured by reaching without and with haptic feedback: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 26(4) Aug 2000, 1436-1460.
  • Birnbaum, M. H., Anderson, C. J., & Hynan, L. G. (1989). Two operations for "ratios" and "differences" of distances on the mental map: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(4) Nov 1989, 785-796.
  • Block, M. E. (1993). Can children with mild mental retardation perceive affordances for action? : Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly Vol 10(2) Apr 1993, 137-145.
  • Bondarko, B. M. (1989). Comparison of metrics of spatial characteristics of images with errors in recognition of the images: Sensory Systems Vol 3(1) Jan-Mar 1989, 44-54.
  • Bondarko, V. M., & Danilova, M. V. (1996). Crowding effects with different surroundings and interstimulus distances: Sensory Systems Vol 10(3) Jul-Sept 1996, 170-177.
  • Bongers, R. M., Michaels, C. F., & Smitsman, A. W. (2004). Variations of tool and task characteristics reveal that tool-use postures are anticipated: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 36(3) Sep 2004, 305-315.
  • Bongers, R. M., Smitsman, A. W., & Michaels, C. F. (2004). Geometric, But Not Kinetic, Properties of Tools Affect the Affordances Perceived by Toddlers: Ecological Psychology Vol 16(2) Apr 2004, 129-158.
  • Bonnet, C. (1964). Perceived speed and the relation between space covered and time required: L'annee Psychologique 64(1) 1964, 47-60.
  • Bonnet, C., & Pouthas, V. (1972). Apparent size and duration of a movement after-effect: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 24(3) Aug 1972, 275-281.
  • Bootsma, R. J., Bakker, F. C., Van Snippenberg, F. J., & Tdlohreg, C. W. (1992). The effects of anxiety on perceiving the reachability of passing objects: Ecological Psychology Vol 4(1) 1992, 1-16.
  • Boring, E. G. (1964). Size constancy in a picture: American Journal of Psychology 77(3) 1964, 494-498.
  • Bourdy, C., Cottin, F., & Monot, A. (1991). Errors in distance appreciation and binocular night vision: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 11(4) Oct 1991, 340-349.
  • Boyd, J. N., III. (2000). A theory of the interaction of affect and cognition: It's about time. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Brackhane, R., Behr, M., & Tschernuth, E. (1987). Absolute judgments on geographical distances: Psychologische Beitrage Vol 29(4) 1987, 653-661.
  • Bradshaw, M. F., & Glennerster, A. (2006). Stereoscopic acuity and observation distance: Spatial Vision Vol 19(1) 2006, 21-36.
  • Braithwaite, V. A., & DePerera, T. B. (2006). Short-range orientation in fish: How fish map space: Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology Vol 39(1) Mar 2006, 37-47.
  • Braunstein, M. L. (1972). Perception of rotation in depth: A process model: Psychological Review Vol 79(6) Nov 1972, 510-524.
  • Bremner, J. G., Johnson, S. P., Slater, A., Mason, U., Foster, K., Cheshire, A., et al. (2005). Conditions for Young Infants' Perception of Object Trajectories: Child Development Vol 76(5) Sep-Oct 2005, 1029-1043.
  • Brenner, E., & Landy, M. S. (1999). Interaction between the perceived shape of two objects: Vision Research Vol 39(23) Nov 1999, 3834-3848.
  • Brenner, E., Smeets, J. B. J., & Landy, M. S. (2001). How vertical disparities assist judgements of distance: Vision Research Vol 41(25-26) 2001, 3455-3465.
  • Brenner, E., & van Damme, W. J. M. (1999). Perceived distance, shape and size: Vision Research Vol 39(5) Mar 1999, 975-986.
  • Brown, C. A. (2004). The perception of auditory distance in reverberant rooms. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Brown, W. (1905). Review of Fresh Light on Molyneux' Problem -- Dr. Ramsay's Case: Psychological Bulletin Vol 2(6) Jun 1905, 217-218.
  • Brungart, D. S., Durlach, N. I., & Rabinowitz, W. M. (1999). Auditory localization of nearby sources: II. Localization of a broadband source: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(4, Pt 1) Oct 1999, 1956-1968.
  • Brungart, D. S., & Scott, K. R. (2001). The effects of production and presentation level on the auditory distance perception of speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(1) Jul 2001, 425-440.
  • Bruno, P., & van den Berg, A. V. (1997). Relative orientation of primary positions of the two eyes: Vision Research Vol 37(7) Apr 1997, 935-947.
  • Bubb, H. (2004). Challenges in the application of anthropometric measurements: Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science Vol 5(2) Mar-Apr 2004, 154-168.
  • Buchner, A., Brandt, M., Bell, R., & Weise, J. (2006). Car Backlight Position and Fog Density Bias Observer-Car Distance Estimates and Time-to-Collision Judgments: Human Factors Vol 48(2) Sum 2006, 300-317.
  • Bulatov, A., & Bertulis, A. (2005). Distracting effects in length matching: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 65(3) 2005, 265-269.
  • Buresova, O., Homuta, L., Krekule, I., & Bures, J. (1988). Does nondirectional signalization of target distance contribute to navigation in the Morris water maze? : Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 49(2) Mar 1988, 240-248.
  • Burris, C. T., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Distorted distance estimation induced by a self-relevant national boundary: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Vol 41(3) May 2005, 305-312.
  • Burton, G. (2000). The role of the sound of tapping for nonvisual judgment of gap crossability: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 26(3) Jun 2000, 900-916.
  • Burton, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Attentionally splitting the mass distribution of hand-held rods: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 50(2) Aug 1991, 129-140.
  • Burtt, H. E. (1918). A pneumograph for inspiration-expiration ratios: Psychological Bulletin Vol 15(10) Oct 1918, 325-328.
  • Byrne, R. W. (1979). Memory for urban geography: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(1) Feb 1979, 147-154.
  • Byrne, R. W., & Salter, E. (1983). Distances and directions in the cognitive maps of the blind: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 37(2) Jun 1983, 293-299.
  • Cabe, P. A., Wright, C. D., & Wright, M. A. (2003). Descartes's blind man revisited: Bimanual triangulation of distance using static hand-held rods: American Journal of Psychology Vol 116(1) Spr 2003, 71-98.
  • Campbell, P. F., Fein, G. G., & Schwartz, S. S. (1991). The effects of Logo experience on first-grade children's ability to estimate distance: Journal of Educational Computing Research Vol 7(3) 1991, 331-349.
  • Candy, T. R., & Bharadwaj, S. R. (2007). The stability of steady state accommodation in human infants: Journal of Vision Vol 7(11) 2007, 1-16.
  • Carbon, C.-C., & Leder, H. (2005). The Wall inside the brain: Overestimation of distances crossing the former Iron Curtain: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(4) Aug 2005, 746-750.
  • Carey, D. P., Dijkerman, H. C., & Milner, A. D. (1998). Perception and action in depth: Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal Vol 7(3) Sep 1998, 438-453.
  • Carey, D. P., Goodale, M. A., & Sprowl, E. G. (1990). Blindsight in rodents: The use of a "high-level" distance cue in gerbils with lesions of primary visual cortex: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 38(3) May 1990, 283-289.
  • Carlson, L. A., & Covey, E. S. (2005). How far is near? Inferring distance from spatial descriptions: Language and Cognitive Processes Vol 20(5) Oct 2005, 617-631.
  • Carlson, L. A., & Van Deman, S. R. (2004). The space in spatial language: Journal of Memory and Language Vol 51(3) Oct 2004, 418-436.
  • Carr, H. (1908). Voluntary control of the distance location of the visual field: Psychological Review Vol 15(3) May 1908, 139-149.
  • Carson, B. (2007). What are musical paradox and illusion? : American Journal of Psychology Vol 120(1) Spr 2007, 123-140.
  • Castro, C., Martinez, C., Tornay, F. J., Fernandez, P. G., & Martos, F. J. (2005). Vehicle distance estimations in nighttime driving: a real-setting study: Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour Vol 8(1) Jan 2005, 31-45.
  • Cavallo, V. (1989). The role of visual information in the anticipation of a collision: Psychologie Francaise Vol 34(1) Mar 1989, 33-40.
  • Cavallo, V., Colomb, M., & Dore, J. (2001). Distance perception of vehicle rear lights in fog: Human Factors Vol 43(3) Fal 2001, 442-451.
  • Cavallo, V., & Laurent, M. (1988). Visual information and skill level in time-to-collision estimation: Perception Vol 17(5) 1988, 623-632.
  • Celebrini, S., Stricanne, B., Thorpe, S., & et al. (1996). Neural processing of stereopsis as a function of viewing distance in primate visual cortical area V1. 700 M 710 Trotter:Yves: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 76(5) Nov 1996, 2872-2885.
  • Chakraborty, A. (1964). Error in reproduction of tactually perceived distances between two aesthesiometric points greater than the two-point R.L. values: Journal of Psychological Researches 8(2) 1964, 86-88.
  • Chamizo, V. D., & Rodrigo, T. (2004). Effect of absolute spatial proximity between a landmark and a goal: Learning and Motivation Vol 35(2) May 2004, 102-114.
  • Chan, T.-c., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Perceiving the vertical distances of surfaces by means of a hand-held probe: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 17(2) May 1991, 347-358.
  • Chapanis, A., & Leyzorek, M. (1950). Accuracy of visual interpolation between scale markers as a function of the number assigned to the scale interval: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 40(5) Oct 1950, 655-667.
  • Chaston, A. N. (2002). Time and distance estimation while walking in outdoor environments: Effects of route and environmental characteristics. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Chemero, A., Klein, C., & Cordeiro, W. (2003). Events as Changes in the Layout of Affordances: Ecological Psychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2003, 19-28.
  • Cheng, K. (1989). The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 15(4) Oct 1989, 366-375.
  • Cheng, K. (1998). Distances and directions are computed separately by honeybees in landmark-based search: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 26(4) Nov 1998, 455-468.
  • Cheng, K., & Sherry, D. F. (1992). Landmark-based spatial memory in birds (Parus atricapillus and Columba livia): The use of edges and distances to represent spatial positions: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 106(4) Dec 1992, 331-341.
  • Cheng, K., Srinivasan, M. V., & Zhang, S. W. (1999). Error is proportional to distance measured by honeybees: Weber's law in the odometer: Animal Cognition Vol 2(1) 1999, 11-16.
  • Chi-Cheng, C., Jui-Hsiang, P., Chung-Hsien, L., & Yun-Chiu, F. (1964). Distance perception during postural changes: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 4 1964, 303-313.
  • Chieffi, S., & Allport, D. A. (1997). Independent coding of target distance and direction in visuo-spatial working memory: Psychological Research Vol 60(4) 1997, 244-250.
  • Ching, C.-C., & Liu, W.-M. (1965). Observation-distance and interval traversed by stimulus as variables in constancy of visual movement: Acta Psychologica Sinica 32(3) 1965, 248-258.
  • Cho, R. Y., Yang, V., & Hallett, P. E. (2000). Reliability and dimensionality of judgments of visually textured materials: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(4) May 2000, 735-752.
  • Cholewiak, R. W. (1999). The perception of tactile distance: Influences of body site, space, and time: Perception Vol 28(7) 1999, 851-875.
  • Churchill, A. V. (1959). Optimal interval length for visual interpolation: The effect of viewing distance: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 43(2) Apr 1959, 125-128.
  • Churchill, A. V. (1960). The effect of pointer width and mark width on the accuracy of visual interpolation: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 44(5) Oct 1960, 315-318.
  • Clements, F. E. (1928). The effect of time on distance discrimination in the albino rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 8(4) Oct 1928, 317-324.
  • Clifton, R. K., Perris, E. E., & Bullinger, A. (1991). Infants' perception of auditory space: Developmental Psychology Vol 27(2) Mar 1991, 187-197.
  • Coello, Y., Magne, P., & Plenacoste, P. (2000). The contribution of retinal signal to the specification of target distance in a visuo-manual task: Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition No 3 2000, 71-86.
  • Cohen, A. A. (1988). Illusory conjunctions as a function of focus of attention and distance between objects: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Coleman, P. D. (1963). An analysis of cues to auditory depth perception in free space: Psychological Bulletin Vol 60(3) May 1963, 302-315.
  • Collett, T. S. (2002). Insect vision: Controlling actions through optic flow: Current Biology Vol 12(18) Sep 2002, R615-R617.
  • Collett, T. S., & Collett, M. (2000). Path integration in insects: Current Opinion in Neurobiology Vol 10(6) Dec 2000, 757-762.
  • Connelly, M. J., Conaglen, H. M., Parsonson, B. S., & Isler, R. B. (1998). Child pedestrians' crossing gap thresholds: Accident Analysis & Prevention Vol 30(4) Jul 1998, 443-453.
  • Coombs, S. (1999). Signal detection theory, lateral-line excitation patterns and prey capture behaviour of motted sculpin: Animal Behaviour Vol 58(2) Aug 1999, 421-430.
  • Corlett, J. T., Byblow, W., & Taylor, B. (1990). The effect of perceived locomotor constraints on distance estimation: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 22(3) Sep 1990, 347-360.
  • Coules, J. (1955). Effect of photometric brightness on judgments of distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(1) Jul 1955, 19-25.
  • Creem-Regehr, S. H., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., & Thompson, W. B. (2005). The influence of restricted viewing conditions on egocentric distance perception: Implications for real and virtual indoor environments: Perception Vol 34(2) 2005, 191 -204.
  • Crompton, A. (2006). Perceived Distance in the City as a Function of Time: Environment and Behavior Vol 38(2) Mar 2006, 173-182.
  • Crompton, A., & Brown, F. (2006). Distance Estimation in a Small-Scale Environment: Environment and Behavior Vol 38(5) Sep 2006, 656-666.
  • Crompton, J. L., & Kim, S.-S. (2001). The influence of cognitive distance in vacation choice: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 28(2) Apr 2001, 512-515.
  • Crowell, J. A., & Banks, M. S. (1996). Ideal observer for heading judgments: Vision Research Vol 36(3) Feb 1996, 471-490.
  • Curiel, J. M., & Radvansky, G. A. (2004). The accuracy of spatial information from temporally and spatially organized mental maps: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 11(2) Apr 2004, 314-319.
  • Da Silva, J. A., Matsushima, E. H., Aznar-Casanova, J. A., & Ribeiro-Filho, N. P. (2006). Distance perception in a natural outdoor setting: Is there a developmental trend to overconstancy? : The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 285-294.
  • Daniels, V., & Gordon, I. E. (1993). Occlusion and the distortion of alignment in three-dimensional space: Perception Vol 22(9) 1993, 1037-1044.
  • Dawkins, M. S. (2002). What are birds looking at? Head movements and eye use in chickens: Animal Behaviour Vol 63(5) May 2002, 991-998.
  • De Camp, J. E. (1920). Relative distance as a factor in the white rat's selection of a path: Psychobiology Vol 2(3) Jun 1920, 245-253.
  • de Castro, E. M., de Paula, A. I., Tavares, C. P., & Moraes, R. (2004). Effects of Navigation Training on Orientation Tasks by Blind Adults: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 17(2) 2004, 199-210.
  • De Jong, M., Wagenaar, W. A., Wolters, G., & Verstijnen, I. M. (2005). Familiar face recognition as a function of distance and illumination: A practical tool for use in the courtroom: Psychology, Crime & Law Vol 11(1) Mar 2005, 87-97.
  • Dean, G. M., Dewhurst, S. A., Morris, P. E., & Whittaker, A. (2005). Selective Interference With the Use of Visual Images in the Symbolic Distance Paradigm: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 31(5) Sep 2005, 1043-1068.
  • Dees, J. W. (1966). Accuracy of absolute visual distance and size estimation in space as a function of stereopsis and motion parallax: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 72(3) Sep 1966, 466-476.
  • Degelman, D., & Rosinski, R. R. (1979). Motion parallax and children's distance perception: Developmental Psychology Vol 15(2) Mar 1979, 147-152.
  • DeLucia, P. R. (1999). Critique of "distance estimation with night vision goggles: A little feedback goes a long way": Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 507-510.
  • DeLucia, P. R., & Novak, J. B. (1997). Judgments of relative time-to-contact of more than two approaching objects: Toward a method: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 59(6) Aug 1997, 913-928.
  • Denis, M. (2008). Assessing the symbolic distance effect in mental images constructed from verbal descriptions: A study of individual differences in the mental comparison of distances: Acta Psychologica Vol 127(1) Jan 2008, 197-210.
  • Denis, M., & Cocude, M. (1989). Scanning visual images generated from verbal descriptions: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 1(4) Dec 1989, 293-307.
  • Denisova, K., Singh, M., & Kowler, E. (2006). The role of part structure in the perceptual localization of a shape: Perception Vol 35(8) 2006, 1073-1087.
  • Depy, D., Fagot, J., & Vauclair, J. (1998). Comparative assessment of distance processing and hemispheric specialization in humans and baboons (Papio papio): Brain and Cognition Vol 38(2) Nov 1998, 165-182.
  • Deregowski, J. B., & Bentley, A. M. (1988). Distance constancy in Bushmen: An exploratory study. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff Publishing.
  • Diewert, G. L., & Roy, E. A. (1978). Coding strategy for memory of movement extent information: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 4(6) Nov 1978, 666-675.
  • Dijkstra, T. M., Gielen, C. C., & Melis, B. J. (1992). Postural responses to stationary and moving scenes as a function of distance to the scene: Human Movement Science Vol 11(1-2) Feb 1992, 195-203.
  • Dobbins, A. C., Jeo, R. M., Fiser, J., & Allman, J. M. (1998). Distance modulation of neural activity in the visual cortex: Science Vol 281(5376) Jul 1998, 552-555.
  • Dodge, R. (1899). Review of Die Pracision der Blickbewegung und der Localisation an der Netzhautperipherie: Psychological Review Vol 6(6) Nov 1899, 668-669.
  • Dodge, R. (1904). Blickrichtung und Grossenschatzung: Psychological Bulletin Vol 1(4) Mar 1904, 131.
  • Doherty, S., Gale, N., Pellegrino, J. W., & Golledge, R. (1989). Children's versus adults' knowledge of places and distances in a familiar neighborhood environment: Children's Environments Quarterly Vol 6(2-3) Sum-Fal 1989, 65-71.
  • Domini, F., & Caudek, C. (2003). 3-D structure perceived from dyamic information: A new theory: Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol 7(10) Oct 2003, 444-449.
  • Donlan, C., Bishop, D. V. M., & Hitch, G. J. (1998). Magnitude comparisons by children with specific language impairments: Evidence of unimpaired symbolic processing: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders Vol 33(2) Apr-Jun 1998, 149-160.
  • Dopkins, S. (2005). Access to dimensional values can be unselective during early perceptual processing: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(3) Apr 2005, 513-530.
  • Dornfeldt, K. (1996). Pigeon homing in the meteorological and solar-geomagnetic environment: What pigeon race data say: Ethology Vol 102(5) May 1996, 413-435.
  • Doumen, M. J. A., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2005). Visual space under free viewing conditions: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(7) Oct 2005, 1177-1189.
  • Doumen, M. J. A., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2006). Horizontal-vertical anisotropy in visual space: Acta Psychologica Vol 123(3) Nov 2006, 219-239.
  • Dumas, C., St-Louis, B., & Routhier, L. (2006). Decision Making and Interference in the Domestic Cat (Felis catus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 120(4) Nov 2006, 367-377.
  • Dunlap, K. (1900). Studies from the psychological laboratory of the University of California. II. The effect of imperceptible shadows on the judgment of distance: Psychological Review Vol 7(5) Sep 1900, 435-453.
  • Dunlap, K. (1906). Review of Malebranche's Theory of the Perception of Distance and Magnitude: Psychological Bulletin Vol 3(4) Apr 1906, 143.
  • Durier, V., & Rivault, C. (1999). Path integration in cockroach larvae, Blatella germanica (L.) (insect: Dictyoptera): Direction and distance estimation: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 27(1) Feb 1999, 108-118.
  • Dutke, S., Ribback, S., & Wagner, B. (2003). Direct Distance, Route Distance, and Implied Temporal Distance: Effects on the Accessibility of Situation Model Components: Scientific Studies of Reading Vol 7(4) Oct 2003, 357-381.
  • Dzhafarov, E. N. (2002). Multidimensional Fechnerian Scaling: Probability-distance hypothesis: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 46(3) Jun 2002, 352-374.
  • Ebenholtz, S. M., & Ebenholtz, J. M. (2003). Distance perception for points at equiconvergence and equidistance loci: Perception Vol 32(6) 2003, 707-716.
  • Ebersbach, M., & Resing, W. C. M. (2007). Shedding new light on an old problem: The estimation of shadow sizes in children and adults: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 97(4) Aug 2007, 265-285.
  • Econopouly, J. (1996). Binocular disparities and KDE are combined to rescale binocular disparities. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ehrlich, J. A. (1999). The effect of viewing conditions on visual stress, sickness, and distance estimation in a helmet-mounted display. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ellard, C. G., & Bigel, M. G. (1996). The use of local features and global spatial context for object recognition in a visuomotor task in the Mongolian gerbil: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 24(3) Aug 1996, 310-317.
  • Ellard, C. G., Chapman, D. G., & Cameron, K. A. (1991). Calibration of retinal image size with distance in the Mongolian gerbil: Rapid adjustment of calibrations in different contexts: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 49(1) Jan 1991, 38-42.
  • Ellard, C. G., & Sharma, L. (1996). The effects of cortical lesions on recognition of object context in a visumotor task in the Mongolian gerbil: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 82(1) Dec 1996, 13-22.
  • Elliott, D. (1987). The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 19(4) Dec 1987, 476-485.
  • Enns, J. T., & Girgus, J. S. (1985). Perceptual grouping and spatial distortion: A developmental study: Developmental Psychology Vol 21(2) Mar 1985, 241-246.
  • Enright, J. T. (1989). Manipulating stereopsis and vergence in an outdoor setting: Moon, sky and horizon: Vision Research Vol 29(12) 1989, 1815-1824.
  • Epstein, W. (1963). Attitudes of judgment and the size-distance invariance hypothesis: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 66(1) Jul 1963, 78-83.
  • Epstein, W. (1964). Effective difference in influence of relative size on perceived relative distance: Perceptual and Motor Skills 18(2) 1964, 383-384.
  • Epstein, W. (1965). Nonrelational judgment of size and distance: American Journal of Psychology 78(1) 1965, 120-123.
  • Epstein, W., Park, J., & Casey, A. (1961). The current status of the size-distance hypotheses: Psychological Bulletin Vol 58(6) Nov 1961, 491-514.
  • Ewert, K. K. (1998). Time judgments in uniform motion events: The effects of visual distance and visual speed information. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Fabricius, W. V., & Wellman, H. M. (1993). Two roads diverged: Young children's ability to judge distance: Child Development Vol 64(2) Apr 1993, 399-414.
  • Fagard, J. (2000). Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behavior of 5-to 12-month old human infants grasping objects of different sizes: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 23(3-4) 2000, 317-329.
  • Faro, D., Leclerc, F., & Hastie, R. (2005). Perceived Causality as a Cue to Temporal Distance: Psychological Science Vol 16(9) Sep 2005, 673-677.
  • Feldman, J. (1997). Curvilinearity, covariance and regularity in perceptual groups: Vision Research Vol 37(20) Oct 1997, 2835-2848.
  • Feria, C. S. (2004). Effects of surface texture discontinuities and surface curvature on distance perception. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Feria, C. S., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2003). Judging distance across texture discontinuities: Perception Vol 32(12) 2003, 1423-1440.
  • Ferris, S. H. (1972). Motion parallax and absolute distance: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 258-263.
  • Field, J. (1976). Relation of young infants' reaching behavior to stimulus distance and solidity: Developmental Psychology Vol 12(5) Sep 1976, 444-448.
  • Fitt, A. B. (1917). The estimation of distances by sight and passive touch: Some investigations into the evolution of the sense of touch: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 2(4) Aug 1917, 264-288.
  • Flamme, G. A. (2000). The relationships between direction and distance hearing and other auditory traits: A multitrait-multimethod evaluation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Foley, J. M. (1966). Locus of perceived equidistance as a function of viewing distance: Journal of the Optical Society of America 56(6) 1966, 822-827.
  • Foley, J. M. (1980). Binocular distance perception: Psychological Review Vol 87(5) Sep 1980, 411-434.
  • Foley, J. M. (1985). Binocular distance perception: Egocentric distance tasks: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 11(2) Apr 1985, 133-149.
  • Foley, J. M. (1993). Stereoscopic distance perception. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
  • Foley, J. M., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2004). Visual perception of extent and the geometry of visual space: Vision Research Vol 44(2) Jan 2004, 147-156.
  • Fontes, S., Fontes, A. I., & Merino, J. M. (1990). Comparison of two psychophysical techniques on Stevens's function: Revista Latinoamericana de Psicologia Vol 22(3) 1990, 373-383.
  • Fontes, S., Garriga, A. J., & Barbero, I. (1993). Psychophysical functions of distance estimation between two lines: Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada Vol 46(1) Jan 1993, -.
  • Foreman, N., Sandamas, G., & Newson, D. (2004). Distance Underestimation in Virtual Space Is Sensitive to Gender But Not Activity-Passivity or Mode of Interaction: CyberPsychology & Behavior Vol 7(4) Aug 2004, 451-457.
  • Foster, P. S., Crucian, G. P., Drago, V., Burks, D. W., Mielke, J., Shenal, B. V., et al. (2007). The effects of movement direction and hemispace on estimates of distance traveled: Brain and Cognition Vol 64(2) Jul 2007, 184-188.
  • Fotheringham, J. R., Martin, P. R., & Ratcliffe, L. (1997). Song transmission and auditory perception of distance in wood warblers (Parulinae): Animal Behaviour Vol 53(6) Jun 1997, 1271-1285.
  • Fox, B. H. (1951). Figural after-effects: "satiation" and adaptation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 42(5) Nov 1951, 317-326.
  • Freiberg, K., Tually, K., & Crassini, B. (2001). Use of an auditory looming task to test infants' sensitivity to sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue: British Journal of Developmental Psychology Vol 19(Pt1) Mar 2001, 1-10.
  • Frenz, H., Bremmer, F., & Lappe, M. (2003). Discrimination of travel distances from 'situated' optic flow: Vision Research Vol 43(20) Sep 2003, 2173-2183.
  • Frenz, H., & Lappe, M. (2005). Absolute travel distance from optic flow: Vision Research Vol 45(13) Jun 2005, 1679-1692.
  • Frenz, H., & Lappe, M. (2006). Visual Distance Estimation in Static Compared to Moving Virtual Scenes: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 321-331.
  • Frenz, H., Lappe, M., Kolesnik, M., & Buhrmann, T. (2007). Estimation of travel distance from visual motion in virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 4(1) Jan 2007, 1-18.
  • Friedenberg, J., & Liby, B. (2002). Perception of two-body center of mass: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(4) May 2002, 531-539.
  • Friedenberg, J. D. (1996). Grouping by proximity and similarity in dot lattices: Effects of luminance and color. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Friedman, A., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Global-Scale Location and Distance Estimates: Common Representations and Strategies in Absolute and Relative Judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 32(2) Mar 2006, 333-346.
  • Friedman, A., & Pilon, D. J. (1994). Effects of distance between objects and distance from the vertical axis on shape identity judgments: Memory & Cognition Vol 22(5) Sep 1994, 552-564.
  • Fry, G. A. (1952). Gilinsky's equations for perceived size and distance: Psychological Review Vol 59(3) May 1952, 244-245.
  • Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events: Psychological Science Vol 17(4) Apr 2006, 278-282.
  • Fujita, N., Shiihara, T., & Noji, T. (2005). Accurate motion-produced distance and direction under systematically distorted perception: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 47(3) Sep 2005, 163-174.
  • Fukusima, S. S., & Aparecido da Silva, J. (1994). Judgment of egocentric distances in a full cue indoor setting: No effect of instructions: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 10(2) May-Aug 1994, 159-166.
  • Fukusima, S. S., da Silva, J. A., & Ribeiro Filho, N. P. (1990). Perception of size and distance of familiar objects: Toward a bifactorial theory: Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia Vol 42(2) Mar-May 1990, 3-25.
  • Fukusima, S. S., Loomis, J. M., & Da Silva, J. A. (1997). Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 86-100.
  • Fukusima, S. S., Ribeiro, G., & da Silva, J. A. (1988). Calculation of Stevens' psychophysical function by microcomputer: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 4(2) May-Aug 1988, 96-101.
  • Garling, T. (1989). The role of cognitive maps in spatial decisions: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 9(4) Dec 1989, 269-278.
  • Garling, T., & Loukopoulos, P. (2007). Choice of Driving Versus Walking Related to Cognitive Distance. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Garrett, S., Barac-Cikoja, D., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). A parallel between visual and haptic perception of size at a distance: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(1) 1996, 25-42.
  • Genovesio, A., & Ferraina, S. (2004). Integration of Retinal Disparity and Fixation-Distance Related Signals Toward an Egocentric Coding of Distance in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of Primates: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 91(6) Jun 2004, 2670-2684.
  • Gentilucci, M., & Negrotti, A. (1994). Dissociation between perception and visuomotor transformation during reproduction of remembered distances: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 72(4) Oct 1994, 2026-2030.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The Effect of Training on Absolute Estimation of Distance over the Ground: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 48(6) Dec 1954, 473-482.
  • Gibson, E. J., Bergman, R., & Purdy, J. (1955). The effect of prior training with a scale of distance on absolute and relative judgments of distance over ground: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(2) Aug 1955, 97-105.
  • Gilhousen, H. C. (1933). Fixation of excess distance patterns in the white rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 16(1) Aug 1933, 1-23.
  • Gilhousen, H. C. (1941). Final goal versus subgoal distance discrimination: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 31(1) Feb 1941, 35-42.
  • Gilinsky, A. S. (1951). Perceived size and distance in visual space: Psychological Review Vol 58(6) Nov 1951, 460-482.
  • Gillam, B. (1995). The perception of spatial layout from static optical information. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Gillam, B., Chambers, D., & Lawergren, B. (1988). The role of vertical disparity in the scaling of stereoscopic depth perception: An empirical and theoretical study: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 44(5) Nov 1988, 473-483.
  • Gillan, D. J., Schmidt, W., & Hanowski, R. J. (1999). The effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on map reading: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(6) Aug 1999, 1154-1167.
  • Girgus, J. S., Coren, S., & Agdern, M. (1972). The interrelationship between the Ebbinghaus and Delboeuf illusions: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 453-455.
  • Gogel, W. C. (1954). Perception of the relative distance position of objects as a function of other objects in the field: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(5) May 1954, 335-342.
  • Gogel, W. C. (1997). The organization of perceived space. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Gogel, W. C., Wist, E. K., & Harker, G. S. (1963). A test of the invariance of the ratio of perceived size to perceived distance: American Journal of Psychology 76(4) 1963, 537-553.
  • Goldfarb, L., & Tzelgov, J. (2005). Is size perception based on monocular distance cues computed automatically? : Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(4) Aug 2005, 751-754.
  • Goldman, M., Skolnick, A. J., Hernandez, T. P., & Tobach, E. (1992). Distance perception in the spiny mouse Acomys cahirinus: Vertical jumping: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 75(3, Pt 1) Dec 1992, 883-895.
  • Goodale, M. A., Ellard, C. G., & Booth, L. (1990). The role of image size and retinal motion in the computation of absolute distance by the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus): Vision Research Vol 30(3) 1990, 399-413.
  • Gordon, A. D., Jupp, P. E., & Byrne, R. W. (1989). The construction and assessment of mental maps: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology Vol 42(2) Nov 1989, 169-182.
  • Gordon, I. E., Day, R. H., & Dorwood, F. (1993). A Muller-Lyer-type illusion of egocentric distance with 3D convex and concave objects: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology Vol 34(2) Jun 1993, 97-106.
  • Graham, B. J., & Northmore, D. P. M. (2007). A spiking neural network model of midbrain visuomotor mechanisms that avoids objects by estimating size and distance monocularly: Neurocomputing: An International Journal Vol 70(10-12) Jun 2007, 1983-1987.
  • Grant, V. W. (1942). Accommodation and convergence in visual space perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(2) Aug 1942, 89-104.
  • Gray, E. R., & Spetch, M. L. (2006). Pigeons Encode Absolute Distance but Relational Direction From Landmarks and Walls: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 32(4) Oct 2006, 474-480.
  • Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1999). Do monocular time-to-collision estimates necessarily involve perceived distance? : Perception Vol 28(10) 1999, 1257-1264.
  • Greenberg, G., McCarthy, T., & Bergamo, P. (1990). Depth/distance perception in gerbils and spiny mice: Ecological considerations: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 3(2) Win 1990, 131-136.
  • Greene, E., & Fraser, S. C. (2002). Observation distance and recognition of photographs of celebrities' faces: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(2) Oct 2002, 637-651.
  • Greene, R. T., Lawson, R. B., & Godek, C. L. (1972). The Ponzo illusion in stereoscopic space: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(2) Oct 1972, 358-364.
  • Greenhut, A. (1954). Visual distance discrimination in the rat: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(3) Mar 1954, 148-152.
  • Gregson, R. A. (1985). Comparison of several theories and distance models: Similar data from Broderson (1968): Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(4) 1985, 573-587.
  • Grice, G. R. (1951). A note on the basis of discrimination in the Grice Size Discrimination apparatus: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 44(1) Feb 1951, 99.
  • Haber, R. N., Haber, L. R., Levin, C. A., & Hollyfield, R. (1993). Properties of spatial representations: Data from sighted and blind subjects: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(1) Jul 1993, 1-13.
  • Haber, R. N., & Levin, C. A. (2001). The independence of size perception and distance perception: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(7) Oct 2001, 1140-1152.
  • Haburcakova, C., Fuhry, L., Nedvidek, J., Tchelidze, T., & Buttner, U. (2000). Modulation of VOR gain during centric and eccentric rotation in the Rhesus monkey: Homeostasis in Health and Disease Vol 40(3-4) 2000, 88-90.
  • Hagen, M. A., & Giorgi, R. (1993). Where's the camera? : Ecological Psychology Vol 5(1) 1993, 65-84.
  • Hamill, R. (1931). Sequence of turns versus distance as essential pattern-elements in the maze problem: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 11(4) Apr 1931, 367-382.
  • Hamilton, W. F., & Goldstein, J. L. (1933). Visual acuity and accommodation in the pigeon: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 15(1) Feb 1933, 193-197.
  • Hancock, P. A., & Manser, M. P. (1997). Time-to-contact: More than tau alone: Ecological Psychology Vol 9(4) 1997, 265-297.
  • Hanyu, K., & Itsukushima, Y. (2000). Cognitive distance of stairways: A multi-stairway investigation: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology Vol 41(1) Mar 2000, 63-69.
  • Harris, J. M., & Sumnall, J. H. (2000). Detecting binocular 3D motion in stataic 3D noise: No effect of viewing distance: Spatial Vision Vol 14(1) 2000, 11-19.
  • Hartman, A. M. (1964). Effect of reduction on the relationship between apparent size and distance: American Journal of Psychology 77(3) 1964, 353-366.
  • Harway, N. I. (1963). Judgment of distance in children and adults: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 65(4) Apr 1963, 385-390.
  • Hatta, T. (2000). Implications of the symbol figure representations. Information from the direction and spatial location used by Japanese students: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 90(1) Feb 2000, 180-186.
  • Haussmann, R. E. (1996). The perceptual construction of visual velocity: Mechanism and model. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Hayashibe, K. (2002). Apparent distance in actual, three-dimensional video-recorded, and virtual reality: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(2) Oct 2002, 573-582.
  • Hazlett, C. J., & Woldorff, M. G. (2004). Mechanisms of Moving the Mind's Eye: Planning and Execution of Spatial Shifts of Attention: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Vol 16(5) Jun 2004, 742-750.
  • He, Z. J., Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., Yarbrough, G., & Wu, J. (2004). Judging egocentric distance on the ground: Occlusion and surface integration: Perception Vol 33(7) 2004, 789-806.
  • Healey, E. C., Jones, R., & Berky, R. (1997). Effects of Perceived Listeners on Speakers' Vocal Intensity: Journal of Voice Vol 11(1) Mar 1997, 67-73.
  • Hebert, J. A., & Krantz, D. L. (1965). Transposition: A reevaluation: Psychological Bulletin Vol 63(4) Apr 1965, 244-257.
  • Hecht, H., & Brauer, J. (2007). Convex rear view mirrors compromise distance and time-to-contact judgements: Ergonomics Vol 50(4) Apr 2007, 601-614.
  • Heft, H. (1993). A methodological note on overestimates of reaching distance: Distinguishing between perceptual and analytical judgments: Ecological Psychology Vol 5(3) 1993, 255-271.
  • Heller, J. (2004). The locus of perceived equidistance in binocular vision: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(7) Oct 2004, 1162-1170.
  • Heller, J., & Reyersbach, R. (1995). The influence of luminous intensity on perceived binocular distance: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie Vol 42(2) 1995, 237-255.
  • Hellige, J. B., & Michimata, C. (1989). Categorization versus distance: Hemispheric differences for processing spatial information: Memory & Cognition Vol 17(6) Nov 1989, 770-776.
  • Helson, H., & King, S. M. (1931). The tau effect: an example of psychological relativity: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 14(3) Jun 1931, 202-217.
  • Henderson, M. D., Fujita, K., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Transcending the "Here": The Effect of Spatial Distance on Social Judgment: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Vol 91(5) Nov 2006, 845-856.
  • Hernandez, C., Domenech, B., Segui, M. M., & Illueca, C. (1996). The effect of pupil and observation distance on the contrast sensitivity function: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 16(4) Jul 1996, 336-341.
  • Heron, G., Furby, H. P., Walker, R. J., Lane, C. S., & et al. (1995). Relationship between visual acuity and observation distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 15(1) Jan 1995, 23-30.
  • Hershman, D. P. (1998). Pitch discrimination across melodic transpositions in adults and young children: Psychomusicology Vol 17(1-2) Spr-Fal 1998-2001, 3-18.
  • Hess, J. A. (2002). Distance regulation in personal relationships: The development of a conceptual model and a test of representational validity: Journal of Social and Personal Relationships Vol 19(5) Oct 2002, 663-683.
  • Heuer, H. (1985). After effects of long fixation in distance perception: A test of motor interpretations: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(1) 1985, 33-54.
  • Heuer, H., Bruwer, M., Romer, T., Kroger, H., & et al. (1991). Preferred vertical gaze direction and observation distance: Ergonomics Vol 34(3) Mar 1991, 379-392.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Adachi, K. (2006). Perceived size and perceived distance of targets viewed from between the legs: Evidence for proprioceptive theory: Vision Research Vol 46(23) Nov 2006, 3961-3976.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Kitano, S. (1991). Perceived size and distance of persons in natural outdoor settings: The effects of familiar size: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 34(3) Sep 1991, 188-199.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Shimono, K. (1994). How accurate is size and distance perception for very far terrestrial objects? Function and causality: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 55(4) Apr 1994, 429-442.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Shimono, K. (2004). Mirror vision: Perceived size and perceived distance of virtual images: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(4) Apr 2004, 679-691.
  • Higashiyama, A., Shimono, K., & Zaitsu, W. (2005). Contraction of perceived size and perceived depth in mirrors: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 81-95.
  • Higashiyama, A., & Ueyama, E. (1988). The perception of vertical and horizontal distances in outdoor settings: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 44(2) Aug 1988, 151-156.
  • Higashiyama, A., Yokoyama, Y., & Shimono, K. (2001). Perceived distance of targets in convex mirrors: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 43(1) Mar 2001, 13-24.
  • Hill, G. W. (1989). Attentional demands and distance judgments: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Hiro, O. (1996). Distance perception in driving: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 55 1996, 92-100.
  • Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2007). Eye vergence is susceptible to the hollow-face illusion: Perception Vol 36(3) 2007, 461-470.
  • Holding, C. S. (1994). Further evidence for the hierarchical representation of spatial information: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 14(2) Jun 1994, 137-147.
  • Holding, C. S., & Holding, D. H. (1989). Acquisition of route network knowledge by males and females: Journal of General Psychology Vol 116(1) Jan 1989, 29-41.
  • Holland, J., Dabelsteen, T., Pedersen, S. B., & Larsen, O. N. (1998). Degradation of wren Troglodytes troglodytes song: Implications for information transfer and ranging: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2154-2166.
  • Hollins, M., & Goble, A. K. (1988). Perception of the length of voluntary movements: Somatosensory Research Vol 5(4) 1988, 335-348.
  • Holmberg, L., Kuller, R., & Tidblom, I. (1966). The perception of volume content of rectangular rooms as a function of the ratio between depth and width: Psychological Research Bulletin 6(1) 1966, 15.
  • Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Numerical reference points reexamined: A reply to Shoben, Cech, and Schwanenflugel: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 9(3) Jun 1983, 487-490.
  • Holyoak, K. J., & Patterson, K. K. (1981). A positional discriminability model of linear-order judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 7(6) Dec 1981, 1283-1302.
  • Holzkamp, K. (1966). On the problem of the relation between perceptual size and perceptual distance: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 13(1) 1966, 39-72.
  • Honda, H. (1990). The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: Its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization systems: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 48(5) Nov 1990, 509-515.
  • Hosokawa, T., Mikami, K., & Saito, K. (1997). Basic study of the Portable Fatigue Meter: Effects of illumination, distance from eyes and age: Ergonomics Vol 40(9) Sep 1997, 887-894.
  • Hsieh, K.-C., Hsu, C.-S., Chin, C., & Tan, C.-M. (1965). The position of seats and visual effect in gymnasiums: Acta Psychologica Sinica 32(3) 1965, 241-247.
  • Hubbard, T. L., Kall, D., & Baird, J. C. (1989). Imagery, memory, and size-distance invariance: Memory & Cognition Vol 17(1) Jan 1989, 87-94.
  • Huertas, J. A., & Ochaita, E. (1992). The externalization of spatial representation by blind persons: Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness Vol 86(9) Nov 1992, 398-402.
  • Hund, A. M., & Plumert, J. M. (2007). What Counts as By? Young Children's Use of Relative Distance to Judge Nearbyness: Developmental Psychology Vol 43(1) Jan 2007, 121-133.
  • Hutchison, J. J., & Loomis, J. M. (2006). Does Energy Expenditure Affect the Perception of Egocentric Distance? A Failure to Replicate Experiment 1 of Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein (2003): The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 332-339.
  • Hutchison, J. J., & Loomis, J. M. (2006). Reply to Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 343-345.
  • Hutchison, K. C. (1987). Graphic and verbal/numeric feedback in computer-assisted instruction: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Vasilyeva, M. (1999). Spatial scaling in young children: Psychological Science Vol 10(5) Sep 1999, 393-398.
  • Hyslop, J. H. (1894). Experiments in space perception: (I): Psychological Review Vol 1(3) May 1894, 257-273.
  • Ichihara, S., Kitagawa, N., & Akutsu, H. (2007). Contrast and depth perception: Effects of texture contrast and area contrast: Perception Vol 36(5) 2007, 686-695.
  • Ichikawa, M., Kimura, T., Egusa, H., Nakatsuka, M., Amano, J., Ueda, T., et al. (2003). Modification of depth and distance perception caused by long-term wearing of left-right reversing spectacles: Perception Vol 32(2) 2003, 131-153.
  • Ido, K., Ohtani, Y., & Ejima, Y. (2000). Summation between nearby motion signals and facilitative/inhibitory interactions between distant motion signals: Vision Research Vol 40(5) 2000, 503-516.
  • Imanaka, K. (1989). Effect of starting position on reproduction of movement: Further evidence of interference between location and distance information: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 68(2) Apr 1989, 423-434.
  • Imanaka, K., & Abernethy, B. (1992). Cognitive strategies and short-term memory for movement distance and location: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 45A(4) Nov 1992, 669-700.
  • Imanaka, K., & Abernethy, B. (2000). Distance-location interference in movement reproduction: An interaction between conscious and unconscious processing? Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Inoue, H., & Watanabe, I. (2005). The effect of location of a highlight in a shaded circle on depth perception: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 76(1) Apr 2005, 51-56.
  • Israel, I., Capelli, A., Sable, D., Laurent, C., Lecoq, C., & Bredin, J. (2004). Multifactorial interactions involved in linear self-transport distance estimate: A place for time: International Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 53(1) Jun 2004, 21-28.
  • Ito, H., & Matsunaga, K. (1991). The perception of an environment through expanding and contracting motion and the role of self-motion: Japanese Psychological Review Vol 34(1) 1991, 5-16.
  • Ittelson, W. H. (1961). Size as a cue to distance. New York, NY: New York University Press.
  • Jackson, R. E., & Cormack, L. K. (2007). Evolved navigation theory and the descent illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 69(3) Apr 2007, 353-362.
  • Jacobs, D. M., & Michaels, C. F. (2001). Individual differences and the use of nonspecifying variables in learning to perceive distance and size: Comments on McConnell, Muchisky, and Bingham (1998): Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(3) Apr 2001, 563-571.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P. (2001). The influence of environmental features of a route on distance knowledge: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie Vol 48(4) 2001, 327-338.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2002). Investigating distance knowledge using virtual environments: Environment and Behavior Vol 34(2) Mar 2002, 178-193.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Berendt, B. (2005). What makes a route appear longer? An experimental perspective on features, route segmentation, and distance knowledge: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 58A(8) 2005, 1390-1414.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2004). The influence of turns on distance cognition: New experimental approaches to clarify the route-angularity effect: Environment and Behavior Vol 36(6) Nov 2004, 790-813.
  • Jansen-Osmann, P., & Wiedenbauer, G. (2006). Distance cognition in virtual environmental space: Further investigations to clarify the route-angularity effect: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 70(1) Jan 2006, 43-51.
  • Jarymowicz, M. (1989). An attempted operationalization of the concepts "social identity" and "individual identity": Schematic separateness of we others as an identity attribute: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 73-84.
  • Jaschinski-Kruza, W. (1990). Effects of stimulus distance on measurements of dark convergence: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(3) Jul 1990, 243-251.
  • Jenkin, N. (1960). Size-distance judgment in organic mental defectives: Journal of Consulting Psychology Vol 24(2) Apr 1960, 139-143.
  • Jimenez, A. C., Sicilia, A. O., & Vera, J. G. (2007). Improving spatial perception in 5-yr.-old Spanish children: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 104(3, Pt2) Jun 2007, 1223-1235.
  • Johnston, E. B. (1991). Systematic distortions of shape from stereopsis: Vision Research Vol 31(7-8) 1991, 1351-1360.
  • Jones, L. A., & Bertamini, M. (2007). Through the looking glass: How the relationship between an object and its reflection affects the perception of distance and size: Perception Vol 36(11) 2007, 1572-1594.
  • Jones-Buxton, R. A., & Wall, H. M. (2001). The Poggendorff illusion: Effect of distance between the parallel lines: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 92(3,Pt1) Jun 2001, 706-710.
  • Jou, J., & Aldridge, J. W. (1999). Memory representation of alphabetic position and interval information: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 25(3) May 1999, 680-701.
  • Judd, C. H. (1899). A study of geometrical illusions: Psychological Review Vol 6(3) May 1899, 241-261.
  • Jui-Hseung, P., & Chung-Hsien, L. (1964). The influence of certain factors on the judgment of distance: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 1 1964, 9-19.
  • Jurowski, C., & Gursoy, D. (2004). Distance Effects on Residents' Attitudes Toward Tourism: Annals of Tourism Research Vol 31(2) Apr 2004, 296-312.
  • Kaan, E. (2002). Investigating the effects of distance and number interference in processing subject-verb dependencies: An ERP study: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Vol 31(2) Mar 2002, 165-181.
  • Kaess, D. W., & et al. (1974). Effect of distance and size of standard object on the development of shape constancy: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 102(1) Jan 1974, 17-21.
  • Kamenchuk, O. (2004). Power distance perceptions in Post-Soviet Russia: Understanding the workplace environment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kaminska-Feldman, M. (1989). Estimating self-others distance: The phenomenon of ego- and allo-centric asymmetry: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 27-39.
  • Kaminska-Feldman, M. (1997). Does asymmetry bias in the self-others distance estimation reflect implicit stereotypes? : Studia Psychologiczne Vol 35(1) 1997, 97-116.
  • Kang, Y.-S., Herr, P. M., & Page, C. M. (2003). Time and distance: Asymmetries in consumer trip knowledge and judgments: Journal of Consumer Research Vol 30(3) Dec 2003, 420-429.
  • Kapoula, Z., Evdokimidis, I., Smyrnis, N., Bucci, M.-P., & Constantinidis, T. S. (2002). EEG cortical potentials preceding vergence and combined saccade-vergence eye movements: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 13(15) Oct 2002, 1893-1897.
  • Karpiuk, P. (1995). Sensation and feeling: A Wave Theory analysis. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kaufman, L., Kaufman, J. H., Noble, R., Edlund, S., Bai, S., & King, T. (2006). Perceptual distance and the constancy of size and stereoscopic depth: Spatial Vision Vol 19(5) 2006, 439-457.
  • Kaufman, L., Vassiliades, V., Noble, R., Alexander, R., Kaufman, J., & Edlund, S. (2007). Perceptual distance and the moon illusion: Spatial Vision Vol 20(1-2) 2007, 155-175.
  • Kebeck, G., Figge, S., & Schreiber, K. (1990). The relationship between physical and imagined distances: An experiment on the foundations of mental training: Gestalt Theory Vol 12(2) Jun 1990, 96-115.
  • Kebeck, G., & Huppe, A. (1985). Possibility of a direct perception of distance: The relationship of objective and phenomenal centerpoints: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 32(2) 1985, 250-262.
  • Kebeck, G., & Landwehr, K. (1992). Optical magnification as event information: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 54(3) Aug 1992, 146-159.
  • Kellman, P., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). Extracting object motion during observer motion: Combining constraints from optic flow and binocular disparity: Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science & Vision Vol 12(3) Mar 1995, 623-625.
  • Kelly, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Judgments of exocentric direction in large-scale space: Perception Vol 33(4) 2004, 443-454.
  • Kelso, J. A. (1977). Motor control mechanisms underlying human movement reproduction: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 3(4) Nov 1977, 529-543.
  • Kendler, H. H. (1943). The influence of a sub-goal on maze behavior: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 36(2) Oct 1943, 67-73.
  • Kennedy, F. (1900). Memory: Psychological Review Vol 7(3) May 1900, 305-309.
  • Kennedy, J. M. (2002). Touch and the observer's vantage point. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Kennedy, R. S., Berbaum, K. S., Collyer, S. C., May, J. G., & et al. (1988). Spatial requirements for visual simulation of aircraft at real-world distances: Human Factors Vol 30(2) Apr 1988, 153-161.
  • Kesner, R. P., & Hopkins, R. O. (2001). Short-term memory for duration and distance in humans: Role of the hippocampus: Neuropsychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2001, 58-68.
  • Kidd, A. H. (1964). Monocular distance perception in schizophrenics: The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology Vol 68(1) Jan 1964, 100-103.
  • Kiefer, R. J., Flannagan, C. A., & Jerome, C. J. (2006). Time-to-Collision Judgments Under Realistic Driving Conditions: Human Factors Vol 48(2) Sum 2006, 334-345.
  • Kiesow, F. (1895). Review of Studien zur Blinden-Psychologie: Psychological Review Vol 2(5) Sep 1895, 531-533.
  • Kilpatrick, F. P., & Ittelson, W. H. (1953). The size-distance invariance hypothesis: Psychological Review Vol 60(4) Jul 1953, 223-231.
  • Kimura, T., & Miura, T. (2003). Attention in depth modulates spatial perception--A comparison between depth perception and distance perception: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 22(1) Sep 2003, 39-40.
  • King, W. L., & Hayes, M. C. (1966). The sun illusion: Individual differences in remembered size and distance judgments: Psychonomic Science Vol 5(2) 1966, 65-66.
  • Kinsella-Shaw, J. M. (1992). Haptic perception of object distance in a single-strand vibratory web: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Haptic perception of object distance in a single-strand vibratory web: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 52(6) Dec 1992, 625-638.
  • Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Haggerty, D. (1992). Age-related differences in adults' macrospatial cognitive processes: Experimental Aging Research Vol 18(1-2) Spr-Sum 1992, 33-39.
  • Kirasic, K. C., & Bernicki, M. R. (1990). Acquisition of spatial knowledge under conditions of temporospatial discontinuity in young and elderly adults: Psychological Research Vol 52(1) Jun 1990, 76-79.
  • Kirkley, S. E. H., Jr. (2003). Augmented reality performance assessment battery (ARPAB): Object recognition, distance estimation and size estimation using optical see-through head-worn displays. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kjelldahl, L., & Schenkman, B. N. (2007). Colour induction on computer displays--Adjacency and shape effects: Behaviour & Information Technology Vol 26(3) May 2007, 261-272.
  • Klein, O. (2004). Social perception of time, distance and high-speed transportation: Time & Society Vol 13(2-3) Sep 2004, 245-263.
  • Kleiss, J. A., & Hubbard, D. C. (1993). Effects of three types of flight simulator visual scene detail on detection of altitude change: Human Factors Vol 35(4) Dec 1993, 653-671.
  • Kline, P. B. (2000). Estimation of traversed distance in a virtual environment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Knapp, J. M. (2001). The visual perception of egocentric distance in virtual environments. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Koenderink, J. J., Van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2003). Exocentric pointing to opposite targets: Acta Psychologica Vol 112(1) Jan 2003, 71-87.
  • Kohler, M., & Wehner, R. (2005). Idiosyncratic route-based memories in desert ants, Melophorus bagoti: How do they interact with path-integration vectors? : Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 83(1) Jan 2005, 1-12.
  • Kohly, R. P., & Ono, H. (2002). Fixating on the wallpaper illusion: A commentary on "The role of vergence in the perception of distance: A fair test of Bishop Berkeley's claim' by Logvinenko et. al. (2001): Spatial Vision Vol 15(3) 2002, 377-386.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 4(1) Feb 1978, 47-60.
  • Kosslyn, S. M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C. B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(4) Nov 1989, 723-735.
  • Kotulak, J. C., & Morse, S. E. (1995). The effect of perceived distance on accommodation under binocular steady-state conditions: Vision Research Vol 35(6) Mar 1995, 791-795.
  • Kotulak, J. C., Morse, S. E., & Wiley, R. W. (1994). The effect of knowledge of object distance on accommodation during instrument viewing: Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 671-679.
  • Kozhevnikova, E. V. (1990). Auditory estimate of the distance of a sound source: Sensory Systems Vol 4(2), Trans Apr-Jun 1990, 156-164.
  • Kraft, R. N., & Green, J. S. (1989). Distance perception as a function of photographic area of view: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(5) May 1989, 459-466.
  • Kral, K., & Devetak, D. (1999). The visual orientation strategies of Mantis reliogosa and Empusa fasciata reflect differences in the structure of their visual surroundings: Journal of Insect Behavior Vol 12(6) Nov 1999, 737-752.
  • Kral, K., & Poteser, M. (1997). Motion parallax as a source of distance information in locusts and mantids: Journal of Insect Behavior Vol 10(1) Jan 1997, 145-163.
  • Krause, W. (1985). Component analysis of the symbolic-distance effect by means of eye movement measurement: Zeitschrift fur Psychologie mit Zeitschrift fur angewandte Psychologie Vol 193(3) 1985, 259-272.
  • Kuroki, D., & Nakamizo, S. (2006). Depth scaling in phantom and monocular gap stereograms using absolute distance information: Vision Research Vol 46(25) Nov 2006, 4206-4216.
  • Labbe, C., & Labbe, D. (2001). Inter-Textual Distance and Authorship Attribution: Corneille and Molire: Journal of Quantitative Linguistics Vol 8(3) Dec 2001, 213-231.
  • Labhart, T., & Meyer, E. P. (2002). Neural mechanisms in insect navigation: Polarization compass and odometer: Current Opinion in Neurobiology Vol 12(6) Dec 2002, 707-714.
  • Lan, W.-C., & Matsuda, F. (1997). The temporal and spatial judgments of junior high school students: Japanese Journal of Developmental Psychology Vol 8(3) Dec 1997, 176-185.
  • Lappin, J. S., Shelton, A. L., & Rieser, J. J. (2006). Environmental context influences visually perceived distance: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(4) May 2006, 571-581.
  • Law, D. J., Pellegrino, J. W., Mitchell, S. R., Fischer, S. C., McDonald, T. P., & Hunt, E. B. (1993). Perceptual and cognitive factors governing performance in comparative arrival-time judgments: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 19(6) Dec 1993, 1183-1199.
  • Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Collins, A., & Wardell, J. (1987). Exploring environments by hand or foot: Time-based heuristics for encoding distance in movement space: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 13(4) Oct 1987, 606-614.
  • Legg, C. R., & Lambert, S. (1990). Distance estimation in the hooded rat: Experimental evidence for the role of motion cues: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 41(1) Dec 1990, 11-20.
  • Leibowitz, H. W., & Hennessy, R. T. (1975). The laser optometer and some implications for behavioral research: American Psychologist Vol 30(3) Mar 1975, 349-352.
  • Leonard, G., & Milner, B. (1991). Contribution of the right frontal lobe to the encoding and recall of kinesthetic distance information: Neuropsychologia Vol 29(1) 1991, 47-58.
  • Levi, D. M., & Klein, S. A. (1990). The role of separation and eccentricity in encoding position: Vision Research Vol 30(4) 1990, 557-585.
  • Levin, C. A. (1991). The independence of size perception and distance perception: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Levin, C. A., & Haber, R. N. (1993). Visual angle as a determinant of perceived interobject distance: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(2) Aug 1993, 250-259.
  • Levin, I., Siegler, R. S., & Druyan, S. (1990). Misconceptions about motion: Development and training effects: Child Development Vol 61(5) Oct 1990, 1544-1557.
  • Li, W., & Matin, L. (2005). Two wrongs make a right: Linear increase of accuracy of visually-guided manual pointing, reaching, and height-matching with increase in hand-to-body distance: Vision Research Vol 45(5) Mar 2005, 533-550.
  • Liang, Y. H., & Wu, C. J. (2007). The effects of number of turns and attention on the perceived distance and time in children and adults: Chinese Journal of Psychology Vol 49(1) Mar 2007, 19-33.
  • Lintern, G. (2000). An analysis of slant for guidance of landing approaches: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 10(4) 2000, 363-376.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Ashmead, D. H. (1997). Development of binaural and spatial hearing in infants and children. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Clifton, R. K. (1992). Use of sound-pressure level in auditory distance discrimination by 6-month-old infants and adults: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(2, Pt 1) Aug 1992, 794-802.
  • Loftus, G. R. (1985). Size illusion, distance illusion, and terrestrial passage: Comment on Reed: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 114(1) Mar 1985, 119-121.
  • Loftus, G. R., & Harley, E. M. (2005). Why is it easier to identify someone close than far away? : Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 12(1) Feb 2005, 43-65.
  • Logan, G. D., & Compton, B. J. (1996). Distance and distraction effects in the apprehension of spatial relations: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(1) Feb 1996, 159-172.
  • Logie, R. H., & Vecchi, T. (2006). Motor components and complexity effects in visuo-spatial processes. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., & Belopolskii, V. I. (1994). Convergence as a cue for distance: Perception Vol 23(2) 1994, 207-217.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., Epelboim, J., & Steinman, R. M. (2002). The role of vergence in the perception of distance: A fair test of Bishop Berkeley's claim: Spatial Vision Vol 15(1) 2002, 77-97.
  • Logvinenko, A. D., & Steinman, R. M. (2002). Fixation on fixation impedes cognition: Reply to Kohly and Ono: Spatial Vision Vol 15(3) 2002, 387-391.
  • Lombard, M. (1994). Direct responses to people on the screen: Television and the illusion of nonmediation. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences.
  • Long, J. M., & Kesner, R. P. (1998). Effects of hippocampal and parietal cortex lesions on memory for egocentric distance and spatial location information in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 112(3) Jun 1998, 480-495.
  • Loomis, J., & Knapp, J. (2003). Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Loomis, J. M., da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 18(4) Nov 1992, 906-921.
  • Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Philbeck, J. W., & Fukusima, S. S. (1996). Visual perception of location and distance: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 5(3) Jun 1996, 72-77.
  • Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Philbeck, J. W., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(6) Aug 1998, 966-980.
  • Loukopoulos, L. D., Engelbrecht, S. E., & Berthier, N. E. (2001). Planning of reach-and-grasp movements: Effects of validity and type of object information: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 33(3) Sep 2001, 255-264.
  • Macdonald, W. A., & Hoffmann, E. R. (1973). The recognition of road pavement messages: Journal of Applied Psychology Vol 57(3) Jun 1973, 314-319.
  • MacKay, D. B. (1989). Probabilistic multidimensional scaling: An anisotropic model for distance judgments: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 33(2) Jun 1989, 187-205.
  • MacKay, D. B. (2001). Probabilistic multidimensional scaling using a city-block metric: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 45(2) Apr 2001, 249-264.
  • Madsen, J., & Kesner, R. P. (1995). The temporal-distance effect in subjects with dementia of the Alzheimer type: Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders Vol 9(2) Sum 1995, 94-100.
  • Magne, P., & Coello, Y. (2002). Retinal and extra-retinal contribution to position coding: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 136(1) Nov 2002, 277-287.
  • Maher, B., Weisstein, N., & Sylva, K. (1964). The determinants of oscillation points in a temporal decision conflict: Psychonomic Science Vol 1(1) 1964, 13-14.
  • Makany, T., Redhead, E. S., & Dror, I. E. (2007). Spatial exploration patterns determine navigation efficiency: Trade-off between memory demands and distance travelled: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 60(12) Dec 2007, 1594-1602.
  • Maki, R. H. (1981). Categorization and distance effects with spatial linear orders: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 7(1) Jan 1981, 15-32.
  • Maples, W. C. (2005). Polaroid Projection in Near Space: Observations of Size and Distance: Optometry and Vision Development Vol 36(1) 2005, 35-40.
  • Mapp, A. P., Ono, H., & Saida, S. (1986). Extreme eye position and Hering's law of equal innervation: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 40(3) Sep 1986, 251-262.
  • Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis: Child Development Vol 70(6) Nov-Dec 1999, 1297-1313.
  • Martel, L., Grealy, M., & Coello, Y. (2006). Contribution of retinal and non-retinal sources of visual information in egocentric object localisation for action: Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition No 19(2) 2006, No Pagination Specified.
  • Martin, L. L. (1999). The use of language to restructure perceptual input in children with and without language impairment. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Mateeff, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (1996). Perception of visual motion with modulated velocity: Effects of viewing distance and aperture size: Vision Research Vol 36(18) Sep 1996, 2873-2882.
  • Mather, G., & Tunley, H. (1995). Temporal filtering enhances direction discrimination in random-dot patterns: Vision Research Vol 35(15) Aug 1995, 2105-2116.
  • Mathiak, K., Hertrich, I., Kincses, W. E., Riecker, A., Lutzenberger, W., & Ackermann, H. (2003). The right supratemporal plane hears the distance of objects: Neuromagnetic correlates of virtual reality: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 14(3) Mar 2003, 307-311.
  • Matsuda, F. (1991). Concepts about relations among time, distance and velocity in children: I. Time and velocity: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 34(1) Mar 1991, 36-46.
  • Matsuda, F. (1992). Concepts about relations among time, distance and velocity in children: II. Distance and velocity, and time and distance: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 35(4) Dec 1992, 222-231.
  • Matsuda, F. (1994). Concepts about interrelations among duration, distance, and speed in young children: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 17(3) Sep 1994, 553-576.
  • Matsuda, F. (1996). Duration, distance, a speed judgments of two moving objects by 4- to 11-year-olds: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 63(2) Nov 1996, 286-311.
  • Matsuda, F. (2001). Development of concepts of interrelationships among duration, distance, and speed: International Journal of Behavioral Development Vol 25(5) Sep 2001, 466-480.
  • Matsuda, F., Lan, W.-C., & Tanimura, R. (1999). Contribution of stimulus attributes to errors in duration and distance judgments--A developmental study: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 88(1) Feb 1999, 87-98.
  • Matsuda, F., Tanimura, R., & Woei-Chen, L. (2000). Duration and distance judgments of two moving objects by 1st to 6th graders: Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition Vol 19(5-6) Dec 2000, 575-602.
  • Matsushima, E., Chiaretti, P., Kreling, D., Lima, M., Da Silva, J. A., & Ribeiro-Filho, N. (2004). Perception-action interaction and bissection: Cadernos de Psicologia e Educacao Paideia Vol 14(27) Jan-Apr 2004, 83-88.
  • Matsushima, E. H., de Oliveira, A. P., Ribeiro-Filho, N. P., & Da Silva, J. A. (2005). Visual angle as determinant factor for relative distance perception: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 97-104.
  • Matsushima, E. H., Gomes, B. C., Oliveira, L. E., Ribeiro, N. P., & Silva, J. A. (2000). Evidence of angular elevation as an absolute distance cue in visual alley: Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia Vol 52(2) 2000, 85-92.
  • Mauerberg-deCastro, E., & Moraes, R. (2002). Distance Perception in Children while Walking: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 15(2) 2002, 373-381.
  • McConnell, D. S., Muchisky, M. M., & Bingham, G. P. (1998). The use of time and trajectory forms as visual information about spatial scale in events: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(7) Oct 1998, 1175-1187.
  • McFadden, S. A. (1990). Eye design for depth and distance perception in the pigeon: An observer orientated perspective: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 3(2) Win 1990, 101-130.
  • McIntosh, R. D., Dijkerman, H. C., Mon-Williams, M., & Milner, A. D. (2004). Grasping what is graspable: Evidence from visual form agnosia: Cortex Vol 40(4-5) Sep-Dec 2004, 695-702.
  • McIntosh, R. D., McClements, K. I., Dijkerman, H. C., & Milner, A. D. (2004). "Mind the gap": The size-distance dissociation in visual neglect is a cueing effect: Cortex Vol 40(2) Apr 2004, 339-346.
  • McKenzie, B. E., Skouteris, H., Day, R. H., Hartman, B., & et al. (1993). Effective action by infants to contact objects by reaching and leaning: Child Development Vol 64(2) Apr 1993, 415-429.
  • McKenzie, B. E., Tootell, H. E., & Day, R. H. (1980). Development of visual size constancy during the 1st year of human infancy: Developmental Psychology Vol 16(3) May 1980, 163-174.
  • McNaughton, B. L., Knierim, J. J., & Wilson, M. A. (1995). Vector encoding and the vestibular foundations of spatial cognition: Neurophysiological and computational mechanisms. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • McNulty, J. A., & St. Claire-Smith, R. (1964). Terrain effects upon perceived distance: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 18(3) 1964, 175-182.
  • Meng, H., & Angelaki, D. E. (2006). Neural Correlates of the Dependence of Compensatory Eye Movements During Translation on Target Distance and Eccentricity: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 95(4) Apr 2006, 2530-2540.
  • Meng, J. C., & Sedgwick, H. A. (2001). Distance perception mediated through nested contact relations among surfaces: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 63(1) Jan 2001, 1-15.
  • Meng, J. C., & Sedgwick, H. A. (2002). Distance perception across spatial discontinuities: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(1) Jan 2002, 1-14.
  • Menne, D., Kaipf, I., Wagner, I., Ostwald, J., & et al. (1989). Range estimation by echolocation in the bat Eptesicus fuscus: Trading of phase versus time cues: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(6) Jun 1989, 2642-2650.
  • Mershon, D. H. (1997). Phenomenal geometry and the measurement of perceived auditory distance. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Mershon, D. H., Ballenger, W. L., Little, A. D., McMurtry, P. L., & et al. (1989). Effects of room reflectance and background noise on perceived auditory distance: Perception Vol 18(3) 1989, 403-416.
  • Mershon, D. H., & Hutson, W. E. (1991). Toward the indirect measurement of perceived auditory distance: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(2) Mar 1991, 109-112.
  • Mershon, D. H., Jones, T. A., & Taylor, M. E. (1993). Organizational factors and the perception of motion in depth: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(2) Aug 1993, 240-249.
  • Messing, R., & Durgin, F. H. (2005). Distance perception and the visual horizon in head-mounted displays: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(3) Jul 2005, 234-250.
  • Michael, D. D., Siegel, G. M., & Pick, H. L. (1995). Effects of distance on vocal intensity: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 38(5) Oct 1995, 1176-1183.
  • Michaels, C. F., Withagen, R., Jacobs, D. M., Zaal, F. T. J. M., & Bongers, R. M. (2001). Information, perception, and action: A reply to commentators: Ecological Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 227-244.
  • Michalewski, H. J., Mohamed Kamel, A.-W., & Starr, A. (1988). Brain potentials during mental distance judgments: International Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 6(3) Aug 1988, 221-229.
  • Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006). Two kinds of visual perspective taking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(2) Feb 2006, 327-337.
  • Millar, S., & Al-Attar, Z. (2001). Illusions in reading maps by touch: Reducing distance errors: British Journal of Psychology Vol 92(4) Nov 2001, 643-657.
  • Miller, K. F., & Baillargeon, R. (1990). Length and distance: Do preschoolers think that occlusion brings things together? : Developmental Psychology Vol 26(1) Jan 1990, 103-114.
  • Milner, A. D., & McIntosh, R. D. (2002). Perceptual and visuomotor processing in spatial neglect. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Montello, D. R. (1989). Route information and travel time as bases for the perception and cognition of environmental distance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Bingham, G. P. (2007). Calibrating reach distance to visual targets: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 33(3) Jun 2007, 645-656.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Tresilian, J. R. (1999). Some recent studies on the extraretinal contribution to distance perception: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 167-181.
  • Mon-Williams, M., & Tresilian, J. R. (2000). Ordinal depth information from accommodation? : Ergonomics Vol 43(3) Mar 2000, 391-404.
  • Moore, D. R., & King, A. J. (1999). Auditory perception: The near and far of sound localization: Current Biology Vol 9(10) May 1999, R361-R363.
  • Morgan, M. J., & Glennerster, A. (1991). Efficiency of locating centres of dot-clusters by human observers: Vision Research Vol 31(12) 1991, 2075-2083.
  • Morris, A., & Temme, L. A. (1989). The time required for U.S. Navy fighter pilots to shift gaze and identify near and far targets: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 60(11) Nov 1989, 1085-1089.
  • Morrongiello, B. A., Hewitt, K. L., & Gotowiec, A. (1991). Infants' discrimination of relative distance in the auditory modality: Approaching versus receding sound sources: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 14(2) Apr-Jun 1991, 187-208.
  • Mortier, F., Robin, S., Lassalvy, S., Baril, C. P., & Bar-Hen, A. (2006). Prediction of Euclidean distances with discrete and continuous outcomes: Journal of Multivariate Analysis Vol 97(8) Sep 2006, 1799-1814.
  • Mossman, C. A., & Drickamer, L. C. (1996). Odor preferences of female house mice (Mus domesticus) in seminatural enclosures: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 110(2) Jun 1996, 131-138.
  • Mowafy, M. K. (1988). Motion perception: The effects of perceived three-dimensional distance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Musseler, J., & Neumann, O. (1992). Apparent distance reduction with moving stimuli (Tandem Effect): Evidence for an attention-shifting model: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 54(4) Dec 1992, 246-266.
  • Naguib, M., Klump, G. M., Hillmann, E., Griessmann, B., & Teige, T. (2000). Assessment of auditory distance in a territorial songbird: Accurate feat or rule of thumb? : Animal Behaviour Vol 59(4) Apr 2000, 715-721.
  • Nakamizo, S., Ono, H., & Ujike, H. (1999). Subjective staircase: A multiple wallpaper illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(1) Jan 1999, 13-22.
  • Nelson, B. S. (2000). Avian dependence on sound pressure level as an auditory distance cue: Animal Behaviour Vol 59(1) Jan 2000, 57-67.
  • Nelson, B. S., & Stoddard, P. K. (1998). Accuracy of auditory distance and azimuth perception by a passerine bird in natural habitat: Animal Behaviour Vol 56(2) Aug 1998, 467-477.
  • Ni, R., Braunstein, M. L., & Andersen, G. J. (2005). Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact: Visual Cognition Vol 12(6) Aug 2005, 1235-1254.
  • Niall, K. K. (1999). The art of descrying distance: Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 511-514.
  • Niall, K. K., Reising, J. D., & Martin, E. L. (1999). Distance estimation with night vision goggles: A little feedback goes a long way: Human Factors Vol 41(3) Sep 1999, 495-506.
  • Nicol, A. U., Brown, M. W., & Horn, G. (1999). Is neuronal encoding of subject-object distance dependent on learning? : Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 10(8) Jun 1999, 1671-1675.
  • Niers, P. (1987). The ego as the basis of unity: Psychoanalytic considerations concerning Piaget's genetic epistemology: Tijdschrift voor Psychotherapie Vol 13(2) Mar 1987, 92-105.
  • No authorship, i. (1894). The Perception of Distance: Psychological Review Vol 1(5) Sep 1894, 540-541.
  • No authorship, i. (1895). Experimentelle Untersuchungen uber das Gedachtniss: Comment: Psychological Review Vol 2(3) May 1895, 317-318.
  • Noordzij, M. L., & Postma, A. (2005). Categorical and metric distance information in mental representations derived from route and survey descriptions: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 69(3) Jan 2005, 221-232.
  • Norman, J. F., Crabtree, C. E., Clayton, A. M., & Norman, H. F. (2005). The perception of distances and spatial relationships in natural outdoor environments: Perception Vol 34(11) 2005, 1315-1324.
  • Norman, J. F., Lappin, J. S., & Norman, H. F. (2000). The perception of length on curved and flat surfaces: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(6) Aug 2000, 1133-1145.
  • Norman, K. L. (1974). Dynamic processes in stimulus integration theory: Effects of feedback on averaging of motor movements: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 102(3) Mar 1974, 399-408.
  • North, R. V., Sethi, B., & Owen, K. (1990). Prism adaptation and viewing distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(1) Jan 1990, 81-85.
  • Nowe, V., Wuyts, F. L., Hoppenbrouwers, M., van de Heyning, P. H., de Schepper, A. M., & Parizel, P. M. (2003). The interutricular distance determined from external landmarks: Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium & Orientation Vol 13(1) 2003, 17-23.
  • Ochaita, E., & Espinosa, M. A. (1997). Spatial knowledge and spatial structure: A study carried out in two neighbourhoods of Madrid: Revista de Psicologia Social Vol 12(1) 1997, 3-16.
  • Ohtsuka, S., Ujike, H., & Saida, S. (2002). Relative distance cues contribute to scaling depth from motion parallax: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(3) Apr 2002, 405-414.
  • Okada, H., & Wapner, S. (1991). Apparent distance between profiles of faces with dynamic properties that represent interpersonal relationships: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(2) Mar 1991, 150-152.
  • O'Kane, B. L., Biederman, I., Cooper, E. E., & Nystrom, B. (1997). An account of objects identification confusions: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Vol 3(1) Mar 1997, 21-41.
  • Ono, H., Lillakas, L., Grove, P. M., & Suzuki, M. (2003). Leonardo's constraint: Two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 132(2) Jun 2003, 253-265.
  • Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2007). A distance judgment function based on space perception mechanisms: Revisiting Gilinsky's (1951) equation: Psychological Review Vol 114(2) Apr 2007, 441-454.
  • Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon: Nature Vol 414(6860) Nov 2001, 197-200.
  • Oshima, Y., & Okaichi, H. (1990). Effect of campus experience and sex difference on distance judgment in a campus environment: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 61(3) Aug 1990, 170-176.
  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolne, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(4) Aug 1996, 879-891.
  • Pagano, C. C., & Bingham, G. P. (1998). Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 24(4) Aug 1998, 1037-1051.
  • Pagano, C. C., Grutzmacher, R. P., & Jenkins, J. C. (2001). Comparing verbal and reaching responses to visually perceived egocentric distances: Ecological Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 197-226.
  • Panerai, F., Cornilleau-Peres, V., & Droulez, J. (2002). Contribution of extraretinal signals to the scaling of object distance during self-motion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 64(5) Jul 2002, 717-731.
  • Paquet, N., Rainville, C., Lajoie, Y., & Tremblay, F. (2007). Reproducibility of distance and direction errors associated with forward, backward, and sideway walking in the context of blind navigation: Perception Vol 36(4) 2007, 525-536.
  • Parks, T. E. (2001). The mirror- (and the moon-) illusion: Perception Vol 30(7) 2001, 899.
  • Paterson, S., & Marino, F. E. (2004). Effect of Deception of Distance on Prolonged Cycling Performance: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 98(3,Pt1) Jun 2004, 1017-1026.
  • Peck, A. J., Jeffers, R. G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Haptically perceiving the length of one rod by means of another: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(3) 1996, 237-258.
  • Peh, C.-H., Panerai, F., Droulez, J., Cornilleau-Peres, V., & Cheong, L.-F. (2002). Absolute distance perception during in-depth head movement: Calibrating optic flow with extra-retinal information: Vision Research Vol 42(16) Jul 2002, 1991-2003.
  • Pei, J., Shen, J., & Gong, J. (1995). A preliminary study on the influencing factors on children's ability of motion distance prediction: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 27(4) 1995, 371-378.
  • Perrine, M. W. (1966). Formation of reference scales, discrimination and verbal reinforcement: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 13(3) 1966, 464-483.
  • Peruch, P., Borel, L., Magnan, J., & Lacour, M. (2005). Direction and distance deficits in path integration after unilateral vestibular loss depend on task complexity: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 25(3) Dec 2005, 862-872.
  • Peruch, P., Giraudo, M.-D., & Garling, T. (1989). Distance cognition by taxi drivers and the general public: Journal of Environmental Psychology Vol 9(3) Sep 1989, 233-239.
  • Philbeck, J. W. (2000). Visually directed walking to briefly glimpsed targets is not biased toward fixation location: Perception Vol 29(3) 2000, 259-272.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & Loomis, J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 72-85.
  • Philbeck, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1997). Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 59(4) May 1997, 601-612.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & Mershon, D. H. (2002). Knowledge about typical source output influences perceived auditory distance (L): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(5,Pt1) May 2002, 1980-1983.
  • Philbeck, J. W., & O'Leary, S. (2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path integration: Psicologica International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology Vol 26(1) 2005, 7-24.
  • Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., Ramsay, S. M., & Weisman, R. G. (1998). Discrimination of auditory distance cues by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) and zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 112(3) Sep 1998, 282-291.
  • Phillmore, L. S., Sturdy, C. B., & Weisman, R. G. (2003). Does reduced social contact affect discrimination of distance cues and individual vocalizations? : Animal Behaviour Vol 65(5) May 2003, 911-922.
  • Pickens, J. N. (1990). Perception of bimodal distance relations by 5-month-old human infants: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Pillsbury, W. B. (1901). Beitrage zur Analyse der Gesichtswahrnehmungen: Psychological Review Vol 8(2) Mar 1901, 212-213.
  • Pippert, E. (1959). Muscularly performed absolute distance judgments: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 6 1959, 372-393.
  • Pizzamiglio, L., Cappa, S., Vallar, G., Zoccolotti, P., & et al. (1989). Visual neglect for far and near extra-personal space in humans: Cortex Vol 25(3) Sep 1989, 471-477.
  • Plug, C. (1989). The registered distance of the celestial sphere: Some historical cross-cultural data: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 68(1) Feb 1989, 211-217.
  • Plumert, J. M., Kearney, J. K., Cremer, J. F., & Recker, K. (2005). Distance perception in real and virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(3) Jul 2005, 216-233.
  • Polich, J. (1993). Hemispheric differences for feature migrations: Acta Psychologica Vol 83(3) Aug 1993, 179-201.
  • Poucet, B., & Buhot, M. C. (1989). Scopolamine impairs response-to-change based on distal cues in the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 46(3) Sep 1989, 355-359.
  • Pouget, A., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1994). A neural model of the cortical representation of egocentric distance: Cerebral Cortex Vol 4(3) May-Jun 1994, 314-329.
  • Prazdny, K. (1981). A note on "optical motions as information for unsigned depth." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 7(2) Apr 1981, 286-289.
  • Predebon, J. (1990). Relative distance judgments of familiar and unfamiliar objects viewed under representatively natural conditions: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 47(4) Apr 1990, 342-348.
  • Predebon, J. (1993). The familiar-size cue to distance and stereoscopic depth perception: Perception Vol 22(8) 1993, 985-995.
  • Predebon, J. (1994). Convergence responses to monocularly viewed objects: Implications for distance perception: Perception Vol 23(3) 1994, 303-319.
  • Predebon, J. (2002). Viewing distance and estimates of duration: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 95(1) Aug 2002, 326-328.
  • Prentice, W. C. H., Krimsky, J., & Barker, S. (1951). The roles of pattern and apparent distance in determining the color of areas seen through transparencies: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 42(3) Sep 1951, 201-206.
  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Distance Perception: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 15(3) Jun 2006, 131-135.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance: Psychological Science Vol 14(2) Mar 2003, 106-112.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2006). A Final Reply to Hutchison and Loomis: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 346-348.
  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2006). Reply to Hutchison and Loomis: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 9(2) Nov 2006, 340-342.
  • Provines, W. F., Rahe, A. J., Block, M. G., Pena, T., & et al. (1992). Yellow lens effects upon visual acquisition performance: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 63(7) Jul 1992, 561-564.
  • Pufall, P. B., & Dunbar, C. (1992). Perceiving whether or not the world affords stepping onto and over: A developmental study: Ecological Psychology Vol 4(1) 1992, 17-38.
  • Purdy, J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Distance judgment by the method of fractionation: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 50(6) Dec 1955, 374-380.
  • Radvansky, G. A., Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Irwin, D. E. (1995). Uncertainty in estimating distances from memory: Memory & Cognition Vol 23(5) Sep 1995, 596-606.
  • Rafter, J. P. (1994). Prediction of time-to-arrival in relation to dual motion and distraction. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Raghubir, P., & Krishna, A. (1996). As the crow flies: Bias in consumers' map-based distance judgments: Journal of Consumer Research Vol 23(1) Jun 1996, 26-39.
  • Redlick, F. P., Jenkin, M., & Harris, L. R. (2001). Humans can use optic flow to estimate distance of travel: Vision Research Vol 41(2) Jan 2001, 213-219.
  • Reed, C. F. (1985). More things in heaven and earth: A reply to Loftus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 114(1) Mar 1985, 122-124.
  • Reed, C. F. (1996). The immediacy of the moon illusion: Perception Vol 25(11) 1996, 1295-1300.
  • Reed, C. F., & Krupinski, E. A. (1992). The target in the celestial (moon) illusion: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 18(1) Feb 1992, 247-256.
  • Reese, H. W. (1962). The distance effect in transposition in the intermediate size problem: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 55(4) Aug 1962, 528-531.
  • Reynolds, R. I. (1989). Auditory and visual distance estimation after active or passive visual deprivation: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 27(3) May 1989, 237-238.
  • Richardson, A. R., & Waller, D. (2007). Interaction with an immersive virtual environment corrects users' distance estimates: Human Factors Vol 49(3) Jun 2007, 507-517.
  • Rieser, J. J., Ashmead, D. H., Talor, C. R., & Youngquist, G. A. (1990). Visual perception and the guidance of locomotion without vision to previously seen targets: Perception Vol 19(5) 1990, 675-689.
  • Rinck, M., & Bower, G. H. (2000). Temporal and spatial distance in situation models: Memory & Cognition Vol 28(8) Dec 2000, 1310-1320.
  • Rinck, M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G. H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The metrics of spatial situation models: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 23(3) May 1997, 622-637.
  • Rival, C., Olivier, I., Ceyte, H., & Bard, C. (2004). Age-related differences in the visual processes implied in perception and action: Distance and location parameters: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 87(2) Feb 2004, 107-124.
  • Roazzi, A., & de Castro Filho, J. A. (2001). The development of the concept of time as integration of distance and speed: Psicologia: Reflexao e Critica Vol 14(3) 2001, 497-503.
  • Roazzi, A., Santos, A. R. R., & de Castro Filho, J. A. (1994). Time as integration of distance and velocity: Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa Vol 10 (1) Jan-Apr 1994, 49-58.
  • Rochat, P. (1995). Perceived reachability for self and for others by 3- to 5-year-old children and adults: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 59(2) Apr 1995, 317-333.
  • Rochat, P., Goubet, N., & Senders, S. J. (1999). To reach or not to be reach? Perception of body effectivities by young infants: Infant and Child Development Vol 8(3) Sep 1999, 129-148.
  • Rochat, P., & Wraga, M. (1997). An account of the systematic error in judging what is reachable: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 23(1) Feb 1997, 199-212.
  • Rock, I., & Ebenholtz, S. (1959). The relational determination of perceived size: Psychological Review Vol 66(6) Nov 1959, 387-401.
  • Rock, I., & McDermott, W. (1964). The perception of visual angle: Acta Psychologica, Amsterdam 22(2) 1964, 119-134.
  • Roggeman, C., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities: Cognition Vol 105(2) 2007, 380-394.
  • Roscoe, S. N. (1997). Size-distance variance and eye accommodation: The ways of an investigator: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 8(1) 1997, 75-81.
  • Roscoe, S. N., Hasler, S. G., & Dougherty, D. J. (1966). Flight by periscope: Making takeoffs and landings; the influence of image magnification, practice, and various conditions of flight: Human Factors 8(1) 1966, 13-40.
  • Rosenblum, L. D., Wuestefeld, A. P., & Anderson, K. L. (1996). Auditory reachability: An affordance approach to the perception of sound source distance: Ecological Psychology Vol 8(1) 1996, 1-24.
  • Rosenfield, M., & Ciuffreda, K. J. (1990). Proximal and cognitively-induced accommodation: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 10(3) Jul 1990, 252-256.
  • Ross, J., & Cliff, N. (1964). A generalization of the interpoint distance model: Psychometrika 29(2) 1964, 167-176.
  • Rossano, M. J., & Moak, J. (1998). Spatial representations acquired from computer models: Cognitive load, orientation specificity and the acquisition of survey knowledge: British Journal of Psychology Vol 89(3) Aug 1998, 481-497.
  • Roumes, C., Meehan, J. W., Plantier, J., & Menu, J.-P. (2001). Distance estimation in a 3-D imaging display: International Journal of Aviation Psychology Vol 11(4) Oct 2001, 381-396.
  • Rovamo, J., Franssila, R., & Nasanen, R. (1992). Contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency, viewing distance and eccentricity with and without spatial noise: Vision Research Vol 32(4) Apr 1992, 631-637.
  • Royden, C. S. (1994). In defense of "Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or absolute size." Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 733-735.
  • Rozhkova, G. I., Podugolnikova, T. A., & Vasiljeva, N. N. (2005). Visual acuity in 5-7-year-old children: Individual variability and dependence on observation distance: Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Vol 25(1) Jan 2005, 66-80.
  • Ruiguang, L. (2005). The perspective theory of space perception: Psychological Science (China) Vol 28(2) Mar 2005, 509-511.
  • Rushton, S. K., & Duke, P. A. (2007). The use of direction and distance information in the perception of approach trajectory: Vision Research Vol 47(7) Mar 2007, 899-912.
  • Russell, M. K. (1997). Perceiving distances of nearby sound sources: An investigation of Marler's canonical features of locatable sounds: Ecological Psychology Vol 9(4) 1997, 299-322.
  • Saberi, K., Perrott, D. R., & Sadralodabai, T. (1991). Judgments of lateral distance using transients presented with interaural differences of time: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(1) Jan 1991, 59-61.
  • Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, W. J., & Staplin, L. J. (1980). Reference points in spatial cognition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 6(5) Sep 1980, 516-528.
  • Sahm, C. S., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Thompson, W. B., & Willemsen, P. (2005). Throwing versus walking as indicators of distance perception in similar real and virtual environments: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(1) Jan 2005, 35-45.
  • Sakamoto, T., & Okaichi, H. (1997). The effects of a fimbria-fornix lesion on distance discrimination in rats: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 68(1) Apr 1997, 38-42.
  • Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 6(4) Apr 1998, 351-360.
  • Santini, F., & Rucci, M. (2007). Active estimation of distance in a robotic system that replicates human eye movement: Robotics and Autonomous Systems Vol 55(2) Feb 2007, 107-121.
  • Sasaki, H., & Nagasaki, H. (1989). The mental distance: Its differences in educational circumstances: Journal of Human Development Vol 25 Jan 1989, 1-10.
  • Saunders, J. A. (1999). Perception of slant and distance from texture. (vision). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Sawyerr, E. S. (1992). Conservation of distance in African children from rural and urban schools: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 74(3, Pt 2), Spec Issue Jun 1992, 1019-1025.
  • Scarf, P., & Grehan, P. (2005). An empirical basis for route choice in cycling: Journal of Sports Sciences Vol 23(9) Sep 2005, 919-925.
  • Schmidt, H.-E. (1964). Relation of the narrowing of the visual field with an increase in distance to manifest anxiety: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 68(4) Oct 1964, 334-336.
  • Schmitt, M., Postma, A., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2001). Cross-modal exogenous attention and distance effects in vision and hearing: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 13(3) Jul 2001, 343-368.
  • Schuster, S., Strauss, R., & Gotz, K. G. (2002). Virtual reality techniques resolve the visual cues used by fruit flies to evaluate object distances: Current Biology Vol 12(18) Sep 2002, 1591-1594.
  • Schutte, A. R., Spencer, J. P., & Schoner, G. (2003). Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development: Child Development Vol 74(5) Sep-Oct 2003, 1393-1417.
  • Schwartz, M. (1999). Haptic perception of the distance walked when blindfolded: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 25(3) Jun 1999, 852-865.
  • Segui-Gomez, M., Levy, J., & Graham, J. D. (1998). Airbag safety and the distance of the driver from the steering wheel: New England Journal of Medicine Vol 339(2) Jul 1998, 132-133.
  • Segui-Gomez, M., Levy, J., Roman, H., Thompson, K. M., McCabe, K., & Graham, J. D. (1999). Driver distance from the steering wheel: Perception and objective measurement: American Journal of Public Health Vol 89(7) Jul 1999, 1109-1111.
  • Seizova-Cajic, T. (2003). The role of perceived relative position in pointing to objects apparently shifted by depth-contrast: Spatial Vision Vol 16(3-4) 2003, 325-346.
  • Servos, P. R. (1995). Binocular vision and the kinematics of human prehension. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Shebilske, W. L., Proffitt, D. R., & Fisher, S. K. (1984). Efferent factors in natural event perception can be rationalized and verified: A reply to Turvey and Solomon: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 10(3) Jun 1984, 455-460.
  • Shechter, S., Hochstein, S., & Hillman, P. (1988). Shape similarity and distance disparity as apparent motion correspondence cues: Vision Research Vol 28(9) 1988, 1013-1021.
  • Shieh, K.-K., & Lee, D.-S. (2007). Preferred viewing distance and screen angle of electronic paper displays: Applied Ergonomics Vol 38(5) Sep 2007, 601-608.
  • Shimono, K., Nakamizo, S., & Higashiyama, A. (2000). Distance perception and vergence: Japanese Psychological Review Vol 43(3) 2000, 335-348.
  • Shipley, W. C., Coffin, J. I., & Hadsell, K. C. (1945). Affective distance and other factors determining reaction time in judgments of color preference: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 35(3) Jun 1945, 206-215.
  • Shoben, E. J., Cech, C. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1983). Choosing model of comparative judgment involving numerical reference points: A rejoinder to Holyoak: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 9(3) Jun 1983, 491-495.
  • Shontz, F. C., & McNish, R. D. (1972). The human body as stimulus object: Estimates of distances between body landmarks: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 95(1) Sep 1972, 20-24.
  • Siegel, A. W., Allen, G. L., & Kirasic, K. C. (1979). Children's ability to make bidirectional distance comparisons: The advantage of thinking ahead: Developmental Psychology Vol 15(6) Nov 1979, 656-657.
  • Simpson, P. J., & Nation, K. J. (1997). Head orientation and binaural depth perception. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Co.
  • Singh, S. J. (1987). Role of visual experience in short term recall of movements: Studies on congenitally blind subjects: Indian Journal of Current Psychological Research Vol 2(1) Apr 1987, 37-46.
  • Sipes, D. E. (1997). Hyperstereopsis as an attenuator for perceptual depth compression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Sirkar, A. K. (1963). Depth perception: Practice-effect and effect of rod-width on distance discrimination: Indian Journal of Psychology 38(3) 1963, 101-105.
  • Smeets, J. B. J., Brenner, E., Trebuchet, S., & Mestre, D. R. (1996). Is judging time-to-contact based on 'tau'? : Perception Vol 25(5) 1996, 583-590.
  • Smith, O. W. (1958). Distance constancy: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 55(4) Apr 1958, 388-389.
  • Smith, W. M. (1952). Gilinsky's theory of visual size and distance: Psychological Review Vol 59(3) May 1952, 239-243.
  • Smyth, M. M., Katamba, J., & Peacock, K. A. (2004). Development of prehension between 5 and 10 years of age: Distance scaling, grip aperture, and sight of the hand: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 36(1) Mar 2004, 91-103.
  • Sobel, E. C. (1990). Distance perception by motion parallax in the locust: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 14(3) Aug 1988, 404-427.
  • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Gravitational and muscular variables in perceiving rod extent by wielding: Ecological Psychology Vol 1(3) 1989, 265-300.
  • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: Haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 15(1) Feb 1989, 58-68.
  • Solomon, R. L. (1949). The role of effort in the performance of a distance discrimination: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 39(1) Feb 1949, 73-83.
  • Spence, K. W., & Grice, G. R. (1942). The role of final and sub-goals in distance discrimination by the white rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 34(2) Oct 1942, 179-184.
  • Spencer, J. P. (1998). The development dynamics of reaching errors. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Spetch, M. L., Rust, T. B., Kamil, A. C., & Jones, J. E. (2003). Searching by rules: Pigeons' (Columba livia) landmark-based search according to constant bearing or constant distance: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 117(2) Jun 2003, 123-132.
  • Srinivasan, M. B., Zhang, S., Altwein, M., & Tautz, J. (2000). Honeybee navigation: Nature and calibration of the "odometer." Science Vol 287(5454) Feb 2000, 851-853.
  • Srinivasan, M. V. (1992). Distance perception in insects: Current Directions in Psychological Science Vol 1(1) Feb 1992, 22-26.
  • Srinivasan, M. V., Lehrer, M., Kirchner, W. H., & Zhang, S. W. (1991). Range perception through apparent image speed in freely flying honeybees: Visual Neuroscience Vol 6(5) May 1991, 519-535.
  • Srinivasan, M. V., Zhang, S. W., & Lehrer, M. (1998). Honeybee navigation: Odometry with monocular input: Animal Behaviour Vol 56(5) Nov 1998, 1245-1259.
  • Stamps, A. E., III, & Krishnan, V. V. (2004). Perceived Enclosure of Space, Angle Above Observer, and Distance to Boundary: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 99(3,Part2) Dec 2004, 1187-1192.
  • Stanley, G. (1966). The apparent size of entoptic after-images: Psychonomic Science Vol 4(8) 1966, 289-290.
  • Stapleton, L., Ward, N. J., Parkes, A. M., Kiefer, R. J., Wolffsohn, J. S., Edgar, G. K., et al. (1998). Head up displays. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science Ltd.
  • Starch, D., & Crawford, A. L. (1909). Minor studies from the psychological laboratory of the Wellesley College: The perception of the distance of sound: Psychological Review Vol 16(6) Nov 1909, 427-430.
  • Steenhuis, R. E. (1988). Target-directed locomotion: A critique of the short-term memory for spatial locations proposed by Thomson: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1988). The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: Does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? : Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 20(4) Dec 1988, 399-415.
  • Steenhuis, R. E., & Goodale, M. A. (1989). "The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: Does an accurate memory for spatial location exist?": Errata: Journal of Motor Behavior Vol 21(2) Jun 1989, 177.
  • Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2005). Distances appear different on hills: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 67(6) Aug 2005, 1052-1060.
  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2003). Affordances Are Enough: Reply to Chemero et al. (2003): Ecological Psychology Vol 15(1) Jan 2003, 29-36.
  • Swanston, M. T., Wade, N. J., Ono, H., & Shibuta, K. (1992). The interaction of perceived distance with the perceived direction of visual motion during movements of the eyes and the head: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 52(6) Dec 1992, 705-713.
  • Szuster-Zbrojewicz, A. (1989). Asymmetry in assessment of we others distances and exocentric engagement in others' problems: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 41-54.
  • Szuster-Zbrojewicz, A. (1989). Cognitive isolation of self and the asymmetry phenomenon in we others distance assessment: Studia Psychologiczne Vol 27(2) 1989, 55-71.
  • Taieb-Maimon, M., & Shinar, D. (2001). Minimum and comfortable driving headways: Reality versus perception: Human Factors Vol 43(1) Spr 2001, 159-172.
  • Taroyan, N. A., Buckley, D., Porrill, J., & Frisby, J. P. (2000). Exploring sequential stereopsis for co-planarity tasks: Vision Research Vol 40(24) 2000, 3373-3390.
  • Tausch, R. (1953). Binocular space perception: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie 1 1953, 394-421.
  • Taylor, J. G. (1965). The behavioral basis of perceived size and distance: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 19(1) 1965, 1-14.
  • Taylor, M. M. (1962). The distance paradox of the figural after-effect in aduitory localization: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 16(4) 1962, 278-282.
  • Taylor, M. M. (1965). Non-additivity of perceived distance with the Mueller-Lyer figure: Perceptual and Motor Skills 20(3, Pt 2) 1965, 1964.
  • Taylor, P. E., Scott, G. B., & Rose, P. (2003). The ability of domestic hens to jump between horizontal perches: Effects of light intensity and perch colour: Applied Animal Behaviour Science Vol 83(2) Sep 2003, 99-108.
  • Thompson, P. (2002). Eyes wide apart: Overestimating interpupillary distance: Perception Vol 31(6) 2002, 651-656.
  • Thompson, S. G., McConnell, D. S., Slocum, J. S., & Bohan, M. (2007). Kinematic analysis of multiple constraints on a pointing task: Human Movement Science Vol 26(1) Feb 2007, 11-26.
  • Thompson, W. B., Dilda, V., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2007). Absolute distance perception to locations off the ground plane: Perception Vol 36(11) 2007, 1559-1571.
  • Tibau, S., Willems, B., Van Den Bergh, E., & Wagemans, J. (2001). The role of the centre of projection in the estimation of slant from texture of planar surfaces: Perception Vol 30(2) 2001, 185-193.
  • Todd, J. T., & Norman, J. F. (2003). The visual perception of 3-D shape from multiple cues: Are observers capable of perceiving metric structure: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 65(1) Jan 2003, 31-47.
  • Todorovic, D. (1989). Orientation anisotropy of spatial discrimination on the skin: Revija za Psihologiju Vol 19(1-2) Dec 1989, 9-16.
  • Torrey, H. B. (1917). General reviews and summaries: Tropism and instinctive activities: Psychological Bulletin Vol 14(8) Aug 1917, 265-276.
  • Tozawa, J. (1992). Perceived size and perceived distance of a growing pattern on the frontal plane: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 10(2) Mar 1992, 99-107.
  • Tozawa, J., & Oyama, T. (2006). Effects of motion parallax and perspective cues on perceived size and distance: Perception Vol 35(8) 2006, 1007-1023.
  • Tresilian, J. R. (1994). A note on "Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or size." Perception Vol 23(6) 1994, 727-731.
  • Tresilian, J. R., & Mon-Williams, M. (1999). A curious illusion suggests complex cue interactions in distance perception: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 25(3) Jun 1999, 677-687.
  • Trotter, Y. (1995). Cortical representation of visual three-dimensional space: Perception Vol 24(3) 1995, 287-298.
  • Trueman, M. T., & Wilson, A. E. (1989). Examination of apparent extent as an explanation of the Poggendorff effect: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(6) Jun 1989, 494-500.
  • Tsuji, K., Hayashibe, K., Hara, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Visuo-motor development which causes detection of visual depth from motion and density cues: Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift fur Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie Vol 59(2) Jun 2000, 102-107.
  • Turatto, M., Galfano, G., Gardini, S., & Mascetti, G. G. (2004). Stimulus-driven attentional capture: An empirical comparison of display-size and distance methods: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 57A(2) 2004, 297-324.
  • Turvey, M. T., & Solomon, J. (1984). Visually perceiving distance: A comment on Shebilske, Karmiohl, and Proffitt (1983): Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 10(3) Jun 1984, 449-454.
  • Ungar, S., Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1997). Teaching visually impaired children to make distance judgments from a tactile map: Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness Vol 91(2) Mar-Apr 1997, 163-174.
  • Ungs, T. J., & Sangal, S. P. (1990). Perception of near earth altitudes by pilots: Ascending vs. descending over both a land and water surface: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 61(12) Dec 1990, 1098-1101.
  • van de Grind, W. A., Verstraten, F. A. J., & van der Smagt, M. J. (2003). Influence of viewing distance on aftereffects of moving random pixel arrays: Vision Research Vol 43(23) Oct 2003, 2413-2426.
  • Van Dillen, L. R. (1995). Memory for spatial information used for aimed limb movements. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • van Ee, R., Banks, M. S., & Backus, B. T. (1999). Perceived visual direction near an occluder: Vision Research Vol 39(24) Dec 1999, 4085-4097.
  • van Ee, R., & Erkelens, C. J. (2000). Is there an interaction between perceived direction and perceived aspect ratio in stereoscopic vision? : Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(5) Jul 2000, 910-926.
  • Van Erp, J. B. F., Van Veen, H. A. H. C., Jansen, C., & Dobbins, T. (2005). Waypoint navigation with a vibrotactile waist belt: ACM Transactions on Applied Perception Vol 2(2) Apr 2005, 106-117.
  • Varnava, A., McCarthy, M., & Beaumont, J. G. (2002). Line bisection in normal adults: Direction of attentional bias for near and far space: Neuropsychologia Vol 40(8) 2002, 1372-1378.
  • Vartanyan, I. A., & Andreeva, I. G. (2007). A psychophysiological study of auditory illusions of approach and withdrawal in the context of the perceptual environment: The Spanish Journal of Psychology Vol 10(2) Nov 2007, 266-276.
  • Vianin, P., Baumberger, B., & Fluckiger, M. (2004). Distance estimation in a dynamic simulated environment: A visual field dependence problem? : Perception Vol 33(5) 2004, 561-568.
  • Viguier, A., Clement, G., & Trotter, Y. (2001). Distance perception within near visual space: Perception Vol 30(1) 2001, 115-124.
  • Virsu, V., & Hari, R. (1996). Cortical magnification, scale invariance and visual ecology: Vision Research Vol 36(18) Sep 1996, 2971-2977.
  • von der Emde, G., Schwarz, S., Gomez, L., Budelli, R., & Grant, K. (1998). Electric fish measure distance in the dark: Nature Vol 395(6705) Oct 1998, 890-894.
  • von Hofsten, C., & Lee, D. N. (1994). Measuring with the optic sphere. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Voss, J., Keary, N., & Bischof, H.-J. (2007). The use of the geomagnetic field for short distance orientation in zebra finches: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 18(10) Jul 2007, 1053-1057.
  • Vujnovich, D. J. (1988). Perception of size and distance: Progressive cue reduction and the size-distance invariance hypothesis: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Wagemans, J., & Tibau, S. (1999). Visual measurement of relative distances between three collinear dots rotating in a slanted plane: Perception Vol 28(2) 1999, 267-282.
  • Walker, J. T., Rupich, R. C., & Powell, J. L. (1989). The vista paradox: A natural visual illusion: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(1) Jan 1989, 43-48.
  • Wallach, H. (1985). Learned stimulation in space and motion perception: American Psychologist Vol 40(4) Apr 1985, 399-404.
  • Wallach, H., & Norris, C. M. (1963). Accommodation as a distance cue: American Journal of Psychology 76(4) 1963, 659-664.
  • Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., & Beall, A. C. (2000). Place learning in humans: The role of distance and direction information: Spatial Cognition and Computation Vol 2(4) 2000, 333-354.
  • Wang, S., & Zhang, M. (1990). Tactile discrimination of distance between two end-points of a curved line: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 22(2) 1990, 135-140.
  • Wang, S., & Zhang, M. (1995). Tactile discrimination of distance between 2 points under the effect of a nearby extra point: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 27(2) 1995, 113-120.
  • Wann, J. P. (1996). Anticipating arrival: Is the tau margin a specious theory? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 22(4) Aug 1996, 1031-1048.
  • Watson, J. S., Banks, M. S., von Hofsten, C., & Royden, C. S. (1992). Gravity as a monocular cue for perception of absolute distance and/or absolute size: Perception Vol 21(1) 1992, 69-76.
  • Weaver, J. A. (2007). Dorsal stream visual regions use monocular depth cues to compute object distance. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Wei, M., & Angelaki, D. E. (2004). Viewing distance dependence of the vestibulo-ocular reflex during translation: Extra-otolith influences: Vision Research Vol 44(9) Apr 2004, 933-942.
  • Whisenand, T. G., & Emurian, H. H. (1996). Effects of angle of approach on cursor movement with a mouse: Consideration of Fitts' Law: Computers in Human Behavior Vol 12(3) Fal 1996, 481-495.
  • Wickelgren, E. A., McConnell, D. S., & Bingham, G. P. (2000). Reaching measures of monocular distance perception: Forward versus side-to-side head movements and haptic feedback: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 62(5) Jul 2000, 1051-1059.
  • Wiener, J. M., & Mallot, H. A. (2003). 'Fine-to-Coarse' Route Planning and Navigation in Regionalized Environments: Spatial Cognition and Computation Vol 3(4) 2003, 331-358.
  • Wiest, W. M., & Bell, B. (1985). Stevens's exponent for psychophysical scaling of perceived, remembered, and inferred distance: Psychological Bulletin Vol 98(3) Nov 1985, 457-470.
  • Wilberg, R. B., & Girouard, Y. (1976). Information on distances, effect of stimuli length and short-term motor memory: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 30(2) Jun 1976, 63-71.
  • Wilcox, T., Rosser, R., & Nadel, L. (1994). Representation of object location in 6.5-month-old infants: Cognitive Development Vol 9(2) Apr-Jun 1994, 193-209.
  • Wilkening, F., & Martin, C. (2004). How to speed up to be in time: Action-judgment dissociations in children and adults: Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift fur Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie Vol 63(1) Mar 2004, 17-29.
  • Witmer, B. G., & Sadowski, W. J., Jr. (1998). Nonvisually guided locomotion to a previously viewed target in real and virtual environments: Human Factors Vol 40(3) Sep 1998, 478-488.
  • Witt, J. K. (2007). Reachability influences perception via motor simulation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent: Perception Vol 33(5) 2004, 577-590.
  • Wohlgemuth, S., Ronacher, B., & Wehner, R. (2001). Ant odometry in the third dimension: Nature Vol 411(6839) Jun 2001, 795-798.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1964). Changes in distance judgments as a function of corrected and noncorrected practice: Perceptual and Motor Skills 19(2) 1964, 403-413.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1965). Texture of the stimulus field and age as variables in the perception of relative distance in photographic slides: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2(2) 1965, 163-177.
  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1966). Smith and Smith's developmental studies of spatial judgments: A note: Perceptual and Motor Skills 23(1) 1966, 137-138.
  • Wrinch, F. S. (1907). Review of Ueber die Beziehungen von Zeitschatzung und Bewegungsemp findung: Psychological Bulletin Vol 4(4) Apr 1907, 119-120.
  • Wrinch, F. S. (1909). The Effect of Suggestion upon the Reproduction of Triangles and Point Distances: Psychological Bulletin Vol 6(9) Sep 1909, 319-320.
  • Wu, B. (2004). The visual perception of distance in action space. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Wu, B., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2007). Inaccurate representation of the ground surface beyond a texture boundary: Perception Vol 36(5) 2007, 703-721.
  • Wu, B., He, Z. J., & Ooi, T. L. (2007). The linear perspective information in ground surface representation and distance judgment: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 69(5) Jul 2007, 654-672.
  • Wu, B., Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (2004). Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information: Nature Vol 428(6978) Mar 2004, 73-77.
  • Wu, J. (2007). Construction of the ground surface representation for distance perception in the intermediate distance range. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Yanase, R. (2001). The effects of spatial information for cognitive distance in an architectural space: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 72(1) Apr 2001, 44-50.
  • Yang, Q., & Kapoula, Z. (2003). Binocular coordination of saccades at far and at near in children and in adults: Journal of Vision Vol 3(8) 2003, 554-561.
  • Yela, M. (1996). The perception of causation at a distance: Psicothema Vol 8(Suppl) 1996, 53-71.
  • Yonas, A., Granrud, C. E., & Pettersen, L. (1985). Infants' sensitivity to relative size information for distance: Developmental Psychology Vol 21(1) Jan 1985, 161-167.
  • Yonas, A., & Hartman, B. (1993). Perceiving the affordance of contact in four- and five-month-old infants: Child Development Vol 64(1) Feb 1993, 298-308.
  • Young, P. T. (1931). The role of head movements in auditory localization: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 14(2) Apr 1931, 95-124.
  • Yun-Chiu, F. (1964). Convergence, interocular distance, and perception of distance: Acta Psychologica Sinica No 3 1964, 203-210.
  • Zahorik, P. (2002). Assessing auditory distance perception using virtual acoustics: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(4) Apr 2002, 1832-1846.
  • Zahorik, P. A. (1999). Experiments in auditory distance perception. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Zakay, D., Hayduk, L. A., & Tsal, Y. (1992). Personal space and distance misperception: Implications of a novel observation: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 30(1) Jan 1992, 33-35.
  • Zhou, Z. (1996). The development of American and Chinese children's understanding of distance, time, and speed interrelations. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Ziskind, D. (1987). Linear perspective is not linear: Compensation for visual field spansion during movement: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Zollner, P. A., & Lima, S. L. (1999). Illumination and the perception of remote habitat patches by white-footed mice: Animal Behaviour Vol 58(3) Sep 1999, 489-500.

Ad blocker interference detected!


Wikia is a free-to-use site that makes money from advertising. We have a modified experience for viewers using ad blockers

Wikia is not accessible if you’ve made further modifications. Remove the custom ad blocker rule(s) and the page will load as expected.

Also on Fandom

Random Wiki