Psychology Wiki
No edit summary
 
No edit summary
 
(17 intermediate revisions by 5 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{SocPsy}}
 
{{SocPsy}}
  +
{{Expert}}
   
  +
'''Cognitive dissonance''' is a [[psychological]] term which describes the uncomfortable tension that comes from holding two conflicting thoughts at the same time, or from engaging in behavior that conflicts with one's beliefs. More precisely, it is the [[perception]] of incompatibility between two [[cognition]]s, where "cognition" is defined as any element of [[knowledge]], including [[attitude (psychology)|attitude]], [[emotion]], [[belief]], or [[behavior]].
'''Cognitive dissonance''' is a condition first proposed by the [[psychology|psychologist]] [[Leon Festinger]] in [[1956]], relating to his hypothesis of [[cognitive consistency]].
 
  +
The theory of cognitive dissonance states that contradicting cognitions serve as a driving force that compels the mind to acquire or invent new thoughts or beliefs, or to modify existing beliefs, so as to reduce the amount of dissonance (conflict) between cognitions. Experiments have attempted to quantify this hypothetical drive. Some of these examined how beliefs often change to match behavior when beliefs and behavior are in conflict.
   
  +
In simple terms, it can be the filtering of information that conflicts with what one already believes, in an effort to ignore that information and reinforce one's beliefs. In detailed terms, it is the [[perception]] of incompatibility between two [[cognition]]s, where "cognition" is defined as any element of [[knowledge]], including [[attitude (psychology)|attitude]], [[emotion]], [[belief]], or [[behavior]].
Cognitive dissonance is a state of opposition between [[cognition|cognitions]]. Cognitive dissonance is a perceived inconsistency between two cognitions in which the person believes one thing but then acts in a different way from what they believed. For the purpose of cognitive dissonance theory, cognitions are defined as being on any element of knowledge [[attitude (psychology)|attitude]], [[emotion]], [[belief]] or [[value]], as well as a [[goal (management)|goal]], plan, or an interest. In brief, the theory of cognitive dissonance holds that contradicting cognitions serve as a driving force that compels the human mind to acquire or invent new thoughts or beliefs, or to modify existing beliefs, so as to ''minimize'' the amount of dissonance (conflict) between cognitions. To experience the feeling of cognitive dissonance or dissonant condition may be equated with [[gut feeling|gut feel]].
 
  +
The theory of cognitive dissonance states that contradicting cognitions serve as a driving force that compels the mind to acquire or invent new thoughts or beliefs, or to modify existing beliefs, so as to reduce the amount of dissonance (conflict) between cognitions. Experiments have attempted to quantify this hypothetical drive. Some of these have examined how beliefs often change to match behavior when beliefs and behavior are in conflict.
   
  +
[[social psychology|Social psychologist]] [[Leon Festinger]] first proposed the theory in 1957 after the publication of his book ''[[When Prophecy Fails]]'', observing the counterintuitive belief persistence of members of a [[Unidentified flying object|UFO]] doomsday [[cult]] and their increased proselytization after the leader's prophecy failed. The failed message of earth's destruction, purportedly sent by aliens to a woman in 1956, became a ''[[disconfirmed expectancy]]'' that increased dissonance between cognitions, thereby causing most members of the impromptu cult to lessen the dissonance by accepting a new prophecy; that the aliens had instead spared the planet for their sake.[http://www.candleinthedark.com/festinger.html]
The main criticism of the [[cognitive consistency]] hypothesis is that it is impossible to verify or falsify by experiment. Even so, experiments have attempted to quantify this hypothetical drive. Opponents of this hypothesis cite the apparent ability of many human beings to reconcile mutually exclusive or contradictory beliefs with no apparent stress, though the original theory would suggest that such beliefs were not psychologically important.
 
   
  +
Maintaining conflicting principles (e.g. logically incompatible beliefs) or rejecting reasonable behavior to avoid conflict can be increasingly maladaptive (non-beneficial) as the gap being bridged widens, and '''popular usage''' tends to stress the maladaptive aspect. Cognitive dissonance is often associated with the tendency for people to resist information that they don't want to think about, because if they did it would create cognitive dissonance, and perhaps require them to act in ways that depart from their comfortable habits. They usually have at least partial awareness of the information, without having moved to full acceptance of it, and are thus in a state of denial about it. This "irrational inability to incorporate rational information" is perhaps the most common perception of cognitive dissonance, and this or another example of extreme maladaption would appear to be underlying many conceptions of the term in popular usage.
In economics this term is also called [[buyer's remorse]]. This post-purchase behavior is more likely to happen when the purchase is a more expensive one. The consumer may experience some regrets or questioning as to whether the purchase was a good one.
 
   
  +
Studies have not so far detected any gender or cross-cultural differences.<ref>[http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=12125926 Aronson, Elliot; "Why It's Hard to Admit to Being Wrong"; npr.com; [[July 20]], [[2007]]]</ref>
==Origins and the experiment==
 
In Festinger and [[Merrill Carlsmith|Carlsmith]]'s classic 1959 experiment, students were made to perform tedious and meaningless tasks, consisting of turning pegs quarter-turns, then removing them from a board, then putting them back in, and so forth. Subjects rated these tasks very negatively. After a long period of doing this, students were told the experiment was over and they could leave.
 
   
  +
==Justification==
However, the experimenter then asked the subject for a small favor. They were told that a needed [[research assistant]] was not able to make it to the experiment, and the subject was asked to fill in and try to persuade another subject (who was actually a confederate) that the dull, boring tasks the subject had just completed were actually interesting and engaging. Some subjects were paid $20 for the favor, another group was paid $1, and a control group was not requested to perform the favor.
 
  +
Justification is the result of acting against set beliefs. The relative strengths of the forces, external and internal, play a large role in whether or not belief change is experienced. This is done in order to dampen the side effects of cognitive dissonance.
   
  +
====Overjustification====
When asked to rate the peg-turning tasks, those in the $1 group showed a much greater propensity to embellish in favor of the experiment when asked to lie about the tasks. Experimenters theorized that when paid only $1, students were forced to internalize the attitude they were induced to express, because they had no other justification. Those in the $20 condition, it is argued, had an obvious external justification for their behavior, which the experimenters claim explains their lesser willingness to lie favoring the tasks in the experiment.
 
   
  +
The phenomena of not experiencing a belief change when forced to act against one's beliefs with high external justification.
The researchers further speculated that with only $1, subjects faced [[insufficient justification]] and therefore "cognitive dissonance", so when they were asked to lie about the tasks, they sought to relieve this hypothetical stress by changing their attitude in a process akin to autobrainwashing. This process allows the subject to genuinely believe that the tasks were enjoyable.
 
   
  +
====Insufficient justification====
Put simply, the experimenters concluded that human beings, when asked to lie without being given sufficient justification, will convince themselves that the lie they are asked to tell is the truth. Only when sufficient justification is given, researchers speculated, are human beings able to resist having their mind instantly reprogrammed by any request that they lie.
 
   
  +
The phenomena of experiencing a belief change when forced to act against one's beliefs with low external justification.
Festinger further tested his theory on observations of counterintuitive belief persistence of most members of a [[Unidentified flying object|UFO]] doomsday [[cult]] and their increased [[proselytization]] after the leader's prophecy failed.
 
   
==Conflicting cognitions: cognitive dissonance==
+
==Empirical research into cognitive dissonance==
  +
Several experimental methods were used as evidence for cognitive dissonance. These were:
Once two competing cognitions are held simultaneously, the individual can be said to be in a state of "cognitive dissonance". For instance: The individual purchases a given brand of washing machine, having been advised by a trusted co-worker that said machine is the best. Subsequent to this purchase, the buyer is exposed to another cognition, one which informs her that there is a higher quality, less expensive washing machine on the market. Moreover, the new source is even more trustworthy than the well-meaning co-worker, e.g. "[[Consumer Reports]]" or "[[Good Housekeeping]]". Instantly, this conflicting cognition creates an imbalance between itself and the original cognition. This conflicted state of mind will, necessarily, seek to attain psychological consonance, i.e. a balance between competing cognitions.
 
   
  +
* Induced compliance studies, where people are asked to act in ways contrary to their attitudes (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959; Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & Nelson, 1996);
==Two kinds of dissonance==
 
  +
* Postdecisional studies, where opinions of rejected alternatives after a decision are studied (Brehm, 1956; Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2002);
''Pre-decisional dissonance'' might be analogous to what [[Sigmund Freud|Freud]] called "compensation." When a test showed that subjects had latent [[sexism|sexist]] attitudes, they later awarded a female a larger reward than a male in what they were told was a different study. Researchers hypothesized that the larger reward reduced dissonance by attempting to show that they were not sexist in the later decision.
 
  +
* Studies of how people seek out information that is consonant rather than dissonant with their own views, so as to avoid cognitive dissonance (Frey, 1986);
  +
* Studies of how people respond to information that is inconsistent with their firmly-held beliefs, attitudes, or commitments (Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter, 1956; Batson, 1975; Burris, Harmon-Jones, Tarpley, 1997).
  +
* Karl, Dolby, and Enrich (2000) found that open source developers who were offered jobs by large software monopolies, subsequently had more favourable views towards closed source software.
  +
===Induced compliance studies===
  +
====Origins and one of the first experiments testing the theory====
   
 
In Festinger and [[Merrill Carlsmith|Carlsmith]]'s classic 1959 experiment, students were made to perform tedious and meaningless tasks, consisting of turning pegs quarter-turns and, another one, putting spools onto a tray, emptying the tray, refilling it with spools, and so on. Participants rated these tasks very negatively. After a long period of doing this, students were told the experiment was over and they could leave. This is an example of an induced compliance study.
The more well-known form of dissonance, however, is ''post-decisional dissonance''. Many studies have shown that people with compulsive disorders like [[gambling]] will subjectively reinforce decisions or commitments they have already made. In one simple experiment, experimenters found that bettors at a horse track believed bets were more likely to succeed immediately after being placed. According to the hypothesis, the possibility of being wrong is dissonance-arousing, so people will change their perceptions to make their decisions seem better.
 
   
 
However, the experimenter then asked the subject for a small favor. They were told that a needed [[research assistant]] was not able to make it to the experiment, and the participant was asked to fill in and try to persuade another subject (who was actually a confederate) that the dull, boring tasks the subject had just completed were actually interesting and engaging. Some participants were paid $20 for the favor, another group was paid $1, and a control group was not requested to perform the favor.
Post-decisional dissonance may be increased by the importance of the issue, the length of time the subject takes to make or avoid the decision, and the extent to which the decision could be reversed.
 
   
  +
When asked to rate the peg-turning tasks later, those in the $1 group rated them more positively than those in the $20 group and control group. This was explained by Festinger and Carlsmith as evidence for cognitive dissonance. Experimenters theorized that people experienced dissonance between the conflicting cognitions "I told someone that the task was interesting", and "I actually found it boring". When paid only $1, students were forced to internalize the attitude they were induced to express, because they had no other justification. Those in the $20 condition, it is argued, had an obvious external justification for their behavior. Behavior internalization is only one way to explain the subject's ratings of the task. The research has been extended in later years. It is now believed that there is a conflict between the belief that "I am not a liar", and the recognition that "I lied". Therefore, the truth is brought closer to the lie, so to speak, and the rating of the task goes up.
==Further propositions by Festinger==
 
Festinger proposed that cognitive dissonance is a "negative drive state", a similar psychological tension to [[hunger]] and thirst and that people will seek to resolve this tension.
 
   
 
The researchers further speculated that with only $1, subjects faced insufficient justification and therefore "cognitive dissonance", so when they were asked to lie about the tasks, they sought to relieve this hypothetical stress by changing their attitude. This process allows the subject to genuinely believe that the tasks were enjoyable.
Reduction of cognitive dissonance may be good because one feels better, and because one can come closer to consonance by eliminating contradictions. On the other hand some of the ways of reduction of cognitive dissonance involve a distortion of the truth, which may cause wrong decisions. The harder way of changing favorable cognitions may in the longer run be better.
 
   
  +
Put simply, the experimenters concluded that many human beings, when persuaded to lie without being given sufficient justification, will carry out the task by convincing themselves of the falsehood, rather than telling a bald lie.
When confronted with two [[belief]] cognitions that contradict each other, Festinger suggests the dissonance can be resolved by finding and adding a third piece of information relevant to the two beliefs. For example, if Sam believes that elected officials are trustworthy, but also believes that elected officials have broken his trust, then the cognitive dissonance can be resolved by discovering that all elected officials lie. This enables Sam to (it is to be hoped) to still hold that elected officials are still largely trustworthy, but that they also all lie.
 
   
  +
This study has been criticized, on the grounds that being paid twenty dollars may have aroused the suspicion of some participants. In subsequent experiments, two common alternative methods of "inducing dissonance were used". In one, experimenters used counter-attitudinal essay-writing, in which people were paid varying amounts of money (e.g., one or ten dollars) for writing essays expressing opinions contrary to their own. The other method was to ask subjects to rate a number of different objects according to their desirability. The subject is then offered a choice between two objects s/he had rated equally, with the knowledge that choosing any one of the two would mean "missing out" on the possible positive features of the unchosen object, thus inducing dissonance.
==References==
 
  +
* Festinger, Leon; co-authors Henry W. Riecken and Stanley Schachter ''When Prophecy fails a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World'' (1956)
 
  +
====Forbidden Toy Study====
* Festinger, L. (1957). ''A theory of cognitive dissonance''. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
 
  +
* Festinger, L. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). "Cognitive consequences of forced compliance". ''Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology'', 58, 203-211. [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Festinger/index.htm Full text].
 
  +
In a later experiment Aronson and Carlsmith (1963) viewed cognitive justification to forced compliance in children.
*Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (1999). ''Cognitive Dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology''. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
 
  +
* Sherman, S. J., & Gorkin, R. B. (1980). "Attitude bolstering when behavior is inconsistent with central attitudes". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology'', 16, 388-403.
 
  +
The experimenter would question the child on a set of toys to gauge which toys the children liked the most and which they found the least tempting. The experimenter then chose a toy that the child really liked, put them in a room with said toy and left the room. Upon leaving the room the experimenter told half the children that there would be a severe punishment if they played with the toy and told the other half that there would be a moderate punishment.
* Knox, R. E., & Inkster, J. A. (1968). "Postdecision dissonance at post time". ''Journal of Personality and Social Psychology'', 8, 319-323.
 
  +
  +
Later, when the punishment, whether severe or moderate, was removed, the children in the moderate punishment condition were less likely to play with the toy, even though now it had no repercussion.
  +
  +
When questioned, the children in the moderate condition expressed more of a disinterest in the toy than would be expected towards a toy that they had initially ranked high in interest. Alternatively, the desirability of the toy went up for the children in the severe punishment condition.
  +
  +
This study laid out the effect of overjustification and insufficient justification on cognition.
  +
  +
So in overjustification the personal beliefs and attitudes of the person do not change because they have a good external reason for their actions. So the children with the severe punishment had a good external reasoning for not playing with the toy because they knew that they would be badly punished for it. However they still wanted the toy. Conversely, the children who would get the moderate punishment displayed insufficient justification because they had to justify to themselves why they did not want to play with the toy since the external motivator, the degree of punishment, was not strong enough by itself. As a result they convinced themselves that the toy was not worth playing with which is why even when the punishment was removed they still did not play with the toy.
  +
  +
===Postdecisional dissonance studies===
  +
Jack Brehm's famous experiment looked at how housewives, after making a decision, favoured the alternatives which they had selected more strongly (Brehm, 1956). This can be explained in dissonance terms — to go on wishing for rejected alternatives would arouse dissonance between the cognitions "I chose something else" and "I preferred that option".
  +
  +
==Basic theory==
  +
  +
Cognitions which contradict each other are said to be "dissonant," while cognitions which agree with each other are said to be "consonant." Cognitions which neither agree nor disagree with each other are said to be "irrelevant." (Festinger, 1957).
  +
  +
The introduction of a new cognition that is dissonant with a currently held cognition creates a state of "dissonance," the magnitude of which relates to the relative importance of the involved cognitions. Dissonance can be reduced either by eliminating dissonant cognitions, or by adding new consonant cognitions. The maximum possible dissonance is equal to the resistance to change of the less [[resistant cognition]]; therefore, once dissonance reaches a level that overcomes the resistance of one of the cognitions involved, that cognition will be changed or eliminated, and dissonance will be reduced.{{Facts|date=May 2007}}
  +
  +
This leads some peoples who feel dissonance to seek information that will reduce dissonance and avoid information that will increase dissonance. People who are involuntarily exposed to information that increases dissonance are likely to discount that information, either by ignoring it, misinterpreting it, or denying it.{{Facts|date=May 2007}}
  +
  +
==Challenges and qualifications==
  +
[[Elliot Aronson]] (1969) challenged the basic theory by linking it to the [[self-concept]]. He said that cognitive dissonance did not arise because people experience dissonance between conflicting cognitions; rather, it surfaced when people saw their actions as conflicting with their self-concept. Thus, in the Festinger and Carlsmith study, Aronson would interpret the dissonance as between "I am an honest person" and "I lied to some one about finding a task interesting". Thus, according to Aronson, people would not experience dissonance in this situation if their self-concept involved perception of the self as a liar.
  +
  +
It should be noted however, that Festinger did acknowledge the powerful impact of central, self-relevant cognitions. He did imply that in spite of the strong drive to seek consistency between cognitions and behavior, there may be situations where the original cognitions are so central to the person's self-concept that they may be resistant to change towards greater consistency. Indeed, several scientists in the literature have shown how individuals who are provided with performance feedback that are discrepant from original beliefs about the self, will tend to strengthen their original beliefs and attitudes further through other behaviors when given the opportunity to do so (BDG, 2007).
  +
  +
More recently, Tedeschi has argued that maintaining cognitive consistency is a way to protect public self-image (Tedeschi, Schlenker & Bonoma, 1971). From 1965, Daryl Bem (1965; 1967) has proposed self-perception theory as an alternative to cognitive dissonance theory. This states that people do not have inner access to their own attitudes - let alone whether they are in conflict. Bem interpreted people in the Festinger and Carlsmith study as inferring their attitudes from their behaviour. Thus, when asked "Did you find that task interesting?" they would judge that, as they told some one they did, they must have done. This self-perception theory was based largely on the behaviourism of [[Burrhus Frederic Skinner|B.F. Skinner]]. Bem interprets those paid twenty dollars in the Festinger and Carlsmith study as being able to interpret their vocal behaviour as an example of what behaviourists such as [[B.F. Skinner]] call "mands" - that is, elements of speech that are commands and demands rather than mere statements. Consequently, these people would have not seen their vocal behaviour as an utterance describing their behaviour.
  +
  +
In many experimental situations, Bem's theory and Festinger's theory make similar predictions, and so it has been very difficult for experimental social psychologists to design a conclusive experiment that will provide more evidence for one rather than the other of these two theories. However, advocates of dissonance theory sometimes argue that of these two theories, only Festinger's theory predicts that certain processes in social cognition will increase [[arousal]], although there is some dispute about how much Festinger's original theory really did imply that cognitive dissonance increased arousal. Therefore, from 1970 onwards, some psychologists have investigated whether being faced with situations where one's cognitions are likely to conflict, arousal is likely to increase, and have found experimental evidence that this is the case.
   
 
==See also==
 
==See also==
  +
* [[Buyer's remorse]]
* [[Great Disappointment]] for an example of how cognitive dissonance works in relation to religion
 
 
* [[Cultural dissonance]]
* [[Self-perception|Self-perception theory]], a competing theory of attitude change
 
 
* The [[Great Disappointment]] of 1844 as an example of cognitive dissonance in a religious context
  +
* [[Groupthink]], lacking in cognitive process
  +
* [[Choice-supportive bias]], memory distortion that makes past choices seem better than they actually were.
 
* [[Supernaturalization]] for a description of another explanation of causal belief
 
* [[Supernaturalization]] for a description of another explanation of causal belief
 
* [[The Fox and the Grapes]] for an example in fiction
 
* [[The Fox and the Grapes]] for an example in fiction
  +
* [[Dialectics]], is an exchange of propositions resulting in a synthesis of the opposing assertions
* [[Weapons of Mass Destruction]] or the lack of evidence prior to [[Iraq War]] as an example
 
  +
* [[Double bind]], is a communicative situation where a person receives different or contradictory messages.
* [[noise music#Luigi Russolo|Luigi Russolo]]'s ideas about noise music in an industrialized society
 
  +
* [[Doublethink]], is the act of holding two contradictory beliefs simultaneously and fervently believing both.
* [[Does not compute]], a phrase common in popular [[science fiction]] to indicate the [[theme (literature)|theme]] of cognitive dissonance in an [[artificial intelligence]]
 
  +
* [[Denial]] as a related defense mechanism.
  +
* [[False dilemma]], involves a situation in which two alternative statements are held to be the only options...
  +
* [[Hostility]] (in the psychological sense) as a psychological response to cognitive dissonance
  +
* [[Javert]]
  +
* [[Paradox]], paradox is an apparently true statement or group of statements that leads to a contradiction
  +
* [[Selective exposure theory]]
 
* [[Self-perception|Self-perception theory]], a competing theory of attitude change
  +
* [[True believer syndrome]] as an example of immunity to cognitive dissonance.
  +
 
==References==
  +
*Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). ''[[Advances in Experimental Social Psychology]], 4,'' pp1-34. Ne Jork: Academic Press.
  +
*Aronson, E. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1963) Effects of severity of threat in the devaluation of forbidden behavior, ''[[Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology]], 66,'' 584-588
  +
*Bem, D.J. (1965). An experimental analysis of self-persuasion. ''[[Journal of Experimental Social Psychology]], 1,'' 199-218
  +
*Bem, D.J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. ''[[Psychological Review]], 74,'' 183-200
  +
*Brehm, J. (1956). Post-decision changes in desirability of alternatives. ''Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52,'' 384-389
  +
*Burris, C. T., Harmon-Jones, E., Tarpley, W. R. (1997). “By faith alone”: Religious agitation and cognitive dissonance. ''[[Basic and Applied Social Psychology]], 19,'' 17-31.
 
* Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). ''When Prophecy fails a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World''
 
* Festinger, L. (1957). ''A theory of cognitive dissonance''. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
 
* Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). "Cognitive consequences of forced compliance". ''Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58,'' 203-211. [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Festinger/index.htm Full text].
  +
*Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). ''When Prophecy fails a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World'' Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  +
*Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nelson, D. E. (1996). Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance. ''[[Journal of Personality and Social Psychology]], 70,'' 5-16.
 
*Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (1999). ''Cognitive Dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology''. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
 
* Sherman, S. J., & Gorkin, R. B. (1980). "Attitude bolstering when behavior is inconsistent with central attitudes". ''Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16,'' 388-403.
 
* Knox, R. E., & Inkster, J. A. (1968). "Postdecision dissonance at post time". ''Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8,'' 319-323.
  +
*Tedeschi, J.T., Schlenker, B.R. & Bonoma, T.V. (1971). Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle? ''[[American Psychologist]], 26,'' 685-695.
  +
   
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
Line 61: Line 125:
 
* [http://www.yetiarts.com/aaron/science/cogdiss.shtml Paper: Cognitive dissonance in decision making]
 
* [http://www.yetiarts.com/aaron/science/cogdiss.shtml Paper: Cognitive dissonance in decision making]
 
* [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Festinger/index.htm Festinger and Carlsmith's original paper]
 
* [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Festinger/index.htm Festinger and Carlsmith's original paper]
  +
* [http://hirr.hartsem.edu/ency/Jehovah's.htm ''Encyclopedia of Religion and Society'' William H. Swatos, Jr. Editor: Refers to Jehovah's Witnesses and the problem of the "millennial delay"]
[http://www.freeminds.org/psych/propfail.htm When Prophecies Fail A Sociological Perspective on Failed Expectation in the Watchtower Society] by Randall Waters from the Bethel Ministries Newsletter May/June 1990 (now the Free Minds Journal)
 
  +
   
[[Category:Social psychology]]
 
 
[[Category:Attitude change]]
 
[[Category:Attitude change]]
[[Category:Communication of falsehoods]]
+
[[Category:Cognitive dissonance]]
 
[[Category:Cognitive processes]]
  +
  +
<!--
  +
{{Link FA|pl}}
  +
[[az:Koqnitiv dissonans nəzəriyyəsi]]
  +
[[cs:Kognitivní disonance]]
 
[[de:Kognitive Dissonanz]]
 
[[de:Kognitive Dissonanz]]
 
[[es:Disonancia cognitiva]]
 
[[es:Disonancia cognitiva]]
Line 72: Line 142:
 
[[hu:Kognitív disszonancia]]
 
[[hu:Kognitív disszonancia]]
 
[[nl:Cognitieve dissonantie]]
 
[[nl:Cognitieve dissonantie]]
  +
[[ja:認知的不協和]]
 
[[pl:Dysonans poznawczy]]
 
[[pl:Dysonans poznawczy]]
  +
[[pt:Dissonância Cognitiva]]
  +
[[ru:Когнитивный диссонанс]]
  +
[[sr:Когнитивна дисонанца]]
  +
[[fi:Kognitiivinen dissonanssi]]
 
[[sv:Kognitiv dissonans]]
 
[[sv:Kognitiv dissonans]]
  +
[[uk:Когнітивний дисонанс]]
  +
[[zh:認知失調]]
  +
-->
 
{{enWP|Cognitive dissonance}}
 
{{enWP|Cognitive dissonance}}

Latest revision as of 22:39, 7 August 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Social psychology: Altruism · Attribution · Attitudes · Conformity · Discrimination · Groups · Interpersonal relations · Obedience · Prejudice · Norms · Perception · Index · Outline


This article is in need of attention from a psychologist/academic expert on the subject.
Please help recruit one, or improve this page yourself if you are qualified.
This banner appears on articles that are weak and whose contents should be approached with academic caution.

Cognitive dissonance is a psychological term which describes the uncomfortable tension that comes from holding two conflicting thoughts at the same time, or from engaging in behavior that conflicts with one's beliefs. More precisely, it is the perception of incompatibility between two cognitions, where "cognition" is defined as any element of knowledge, including attitude, emotion, belief, or behavior. The theory of cognitive dissonance states that contradicting cognitions serve as a driving force that compels the mind to acquire or invent new thoughts or beliefs, or to modify existing beliefs, so as to reduce the amount of dissonance (conflict) between cognitions. Experiments have attempted to quantify this hypothetical drive. Some of these examined how beliefs often change to match behavior when beliefs and behavior are in conflict.

In simple terms, it can be the filtering of information that conflicts with what one already believes, in an effort to ignore that information and reinforce one's beliefs. In detailed terms, it is the perception of incompatibility between two cognitions, where "cognition" is defined as any element of knowledge, including attitude, emotion, belief, or behavior. The theory of cognitive dissonance states that contradicting cognitions serve as a driving force that compels the mind to acquire or invent new thoughts or beliefs, or to modify existing beliefs, so as to reduce the amount of dissonance (conflict) between cognitions. Experiments have attempted to quantify this hypothetical drive. Some of these have examined how beliefs often change to match behavior when beliefs and behavior are in conflict.

Social psychologist Leon Festinger first proposed the theory in 1957 after the publication of his book When Prophecy Fails, observing the counterintuitive belief persistence of members of a UFO doomsday cult and their increased proselytization after the leader's prophecy failed. The failed message of earth's destruction, purportedly sent by aliens to a woman in 1956, became a disconfirmed expectancy that increased dissonance between cognitions, thereby causing most members of the impromptu cult to lessen the dissonance by accepting a new prophecy; that the aliens had instead spared the planet for their sake.[1]

Maintaining conflicting principles (e.g. logically incompatible beliefs) or rejecting reasonable behavior to avoid conflict can be increasingly maladaptive (non-beneficial) as the gap being bridged widens, and popular usage tends to stress the maladaptive aspect. Cognitive dissonance is often associated with the tendency for people to resist information that they don't want to think about, because if they did it would create cognitive dissonance, and perhaps require them to act in ways that depart from their comfortable habits. They usually have at least partial awareness of the information, without having moved to full acceptance of it, and are thus in a state of denial about it. This "irrational inability to incorporate rational information" is perhaps the most common perception of cognitive dissonance, and this or another example of extreme maladaption would appear to be underlying many conceptions of the term in popular usage.

Studies have not so far detected any gender or cross-cultural differences.[1]

Justification

Justification is the result of acting against set beliefs. The relative strengths of the forces, external and internal, play a large role in whether or not belief change is experienced. This is done in order to dampen the side effects of cognitive dissonance.

Overjustification

The phenomena of not experiencing a belief change when forced to act against one's beliefs with high external justification.

Insufficient justification

The phenomena of experiencing a belief change when forced to act against one's beliefs with low external justification.

Empirical research into cognitive dissonance

Several experimental methods were used as evidence for cognitive dissonance. These were:

  • Induced compliance studies, where people are asked to act in ways contrary to their attitudes (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959; Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & Nelson, 1996);
  • Postdecisional studies, where opinions of rejected alternatives after a decision are studied (Brehm, 1956; Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2002);
  • Studies of how people seek out information that is consonant rather than dissonant with their own views, so as to avoid cognitive dissonance (Frey, 1986);
  • Studies of how people respond to information that is inconsistent with their firmly-held beliefs, attitudes, or commitments (Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter, 1956; Batson, 1975; Burris, Harmon-Jones, Tarpley, 1997).
  • Karl, Dolby, and Enrich (2000) found that open source developers who were offered jobs by large software monopolies, subsequently had more favourable views towards closed source software.

Induced compliance studies

Origins and one of the first experiments testing the theory

In Festinger and Carlsmith's classic 1959 experiment, students were made to perform tedious and meaningless tasks, consisting of turning pegs quarter-turns and, another one, putting spools onto a tray, emptying the tray, refilling it with spools, and so on. Participants rated these tasks very negatively. After a long period of doing this, students were told the experiment was over and they could leave. This is an example of an induced compliance study.

However, the experimenter then asked the subject for a small favor. They were told that a needed research assistant was not able to make it to the experiment, and the participant was asked to fill in and try to persuade another subject (who was actually a confederate) that the dull, boring tasks the subject had just completed were actually interesting and engaging. Some participants were paid $20 for the favor, another group was paid $1, and a control group was not requested to perform the favor.

When asked to rate the peg-turning tasks later, those in the $1 group rated them more positively than those in the $20 group and control group. This was explained by Festinger and Carlsmith as evidence for cognitive dissonance. Experimenters theorized that people experienced dissonance between the conflicting cognitions "I told someone that the task was interesting", and "I actually found it boring". When paid only $1, students were forced to internalize the attitude they were induced to express, because they had no other justification. Those in the $20 condition, it is argued, had an obvious external justification for their behavior. Behavior internalization is only one way to explain the subject's ratings of the task. The research has been extended in later years. It is now believed that there is a conflict between the belief that "I am not a liar", and the recognition that "I lied". Therefore, the truth is brought closer to the lie, so to speak, and the rating of the task goes up.

The researchers further speculated that with only $1, subjects faced insufficient justification and therefore "cognitive dissonance", so when they were asked to lie about the tasks, they sought to relieve this hypothetical stress by changing their attitude. This process allows the subject to genuinely believe that the tasks were enjoyable.

Put simply, the experimenters concluded that many human beings, when persuaded to lie without being given sufficient justification, will carry out the task by convincing themselves of the falsehood, rather than telling a bald lie.

This study has been criticized, on the grounds that being paid twenty dollars may have aroused the suspicion of some participants. In subsequent experiments, two common alternative methods of "inducing dissonance were used". In one, experimenters used counter-attitudinal essay-writing, in which people were paid varying amounts of money (e.g., one or ten dollars) for writing essays expressing opinions contrary to their own. The other method was to ask subjects to rate a number of different objects according to their desirability. The subject is then offered a choice between two objects s/he had rated equally, with the knowledge that choosing any one of the two would mean "missing out" on the possible positive features of the unchosen object, thus inducing dissonance.

Forbidden Toy Study

In a later experiment Aronson and Carlsmith (1963) viewed cognitive justification to forced compliance in children.

The experimenter would question the child on a set of toys to gauge which toys the children liked the most and which they found the least tempting. The experimenter then chose a toy that the child really liked, put them in a room with said toy and left the room. Upon leaving the room the experimenter told half the children that there would be a severe punishment if they played with the toy and told the other half that there would be a moderate punishment.

Later, when the punishment, whether severe or moderate, was removed, the children in the moderate punishment condition were less likely to play with the toy, even though now it had no repercussion.

When questioned, the children in the moderate condition expressed more of a disinterest in the toy than would be expected towards a toy that they had initially ranked high in interest. Alternatively, the desirability of the toy went up for the children in the severe punishment condition.

This study laid out the effect of overjustification and insufficient justification on cognition.

So in overjustification the personal beliefs and attitudes of the person do not change because they have a good external reason for their actions. So the children with the severe punishment had a good external reasoning for not playing with the toy because they knew that they would be badly punished for it. However they still wanted the toy. Conversely, the children who would get the moderate punishment displayed insufficient justification because they had to justify to themselves why they did not want to play with the toy since the external motivator, the degree of punishment, was not strong enough by itself. As a result they convinced themselves that the toy was not worth playing with which is why even when the punishment was removed they still did not play with the toy.

Postdecisional dissonance studies

Jack Brehm's famous experiment looked at how housewives, after making a decision, favoured the alternatives which they had selected more strongly (Brehm, 1956). This can be explained in dissonance terms — to go on wishing for rejected alternatives would arouse dissonance between the cognitions "I chose something else" and "I preferred that option".

Basic theory

Cognitions which contradict each other are said to be "dissonant," while cognitions which agree with each other are said to be "consonant." Cognitions which neither agree nor disagree with each other are said to be "irrelevant." (Festinger, 1957).

The introduction of a new cognition that is dissonant with a currently held cognition creates a state of "dissonance," the magnitude of which relates to the relative importance of the involved cognitions. Dissonance can be reduced either by eliminating dissonant cognitions, or by adding new consonant cognitions. The maximum possible dissonance is equal to the resistance to change of the less resistant cognition; therefore, once dissonance reaches a level that overcomes the resistance of one of the cognitions involved, that cognition will be changed or eliminated, and dissonance will be reduced.[citations needed]


This leads some peoples who feel dissonance to seek information that will reduce dissonance and avoid information that will increase dissonance. People who are involuntarily exposed to information that increases dissonance are likely to discount that information, either by ignoring it, misinterpreting it, or denying it.[citations needed]


Challenges and qualifications

Elliot Aronson (1969) challenged the basic theory by linking it to the self-concept. He said that cognitive dissonance did not arise because people experience dissonance between conflicting cognitions; rather, it surfaced when people saw their actions as conflicting with their self-concept. Thus, in the Festinger and Carlsmith study, Aronson would interpret the dissonance as between "I am an honest person" and "I lied to some one about finding a task interesting". Thus, according to Aronson, people would not experience dissonance in this situation if their self-concept involved perception of the self as a liar.

It should be noted however, that Festinger did acknowledge the powerful impact of central, self-relevant cognitions. He did imply that in spite of the strong drive to seek consistency between cognitions and behavior, there may be situations where the original cognitions are so central to the person's self-concept that they may be resistant to change towards greater consistency. Indeed, several scientists in the literature have shown how individuals who are provided with performance feedback that are discrepant from original beliefs about the self, will tend to strengthen their original beliefs and attitudes further through other behaviors when given the opportunity to do so (BDG, 2007).

More recently, Tedeschi has argued that maintaining cognitive consistency is a way to protect public self-image (Tedeschi, Schlenker & Bonoma, 1971). From 1965, Daryl Bem (1965; 1967) has proposed self-perception theory as an alternative to cognitive dissonance theory. This states that people do not have inner access to their own attitudes - let alone whether they are in conflict. Bem interpreted people in the Festinger and Carlsmith study as inferring their attitudes from their behaviour. Thus, when asked "Did you find that task interesting?" they would judge that, as they told some one they did, they must have done. This self-perception theory was based largely on the behaviourism of B.F. Skinner. Bem interprets those paid twenty dollars in the Festinger and Carlsmith study as being able to interpret their vocal behaviour as an example of what behaviourists such as B.F. Skinner call "mands" - that is, elements of speech that are commands and demands rather than mere statements. Consequently, these people would have not seen their vocal behaviour as an utterance describing their behaviour.

In many experimental situations, Bem's theory and Festinger's theory make similar predictions, and so it has been very difficult for experimental social psychologists to design a conclusive experiment that will provide more evidence for one rather than the other of these two theories. However, advocates of dissonance theory sometimes argue that of these two theories, only Festinger's theory predicts that certain processes in social cognition will increase arousal, although there is some dispute about how much Festinger's original theory really did imply that cognitive dissonance increased arousal. Therefore, from 1970 onwards, some psychologists have investigated whether being faced with situations where one's cognitions are likely to conflict, arousal is likely to increase, and have found experimental evidence that this is the case.

See also

  • Buyer's remorse
  • Cultural dissonance
  • The Great Disappointment of 1844 as an example of cognitive dissonance in a religious context
  • Groupthink, lacking in cognitive process
  • Choice-supportive bias, memory distortion that makes past choices seem better than they actually were.
  • Supernaturalization for a description of another explanation of causal belief
  • The Fox and the Grapes for an example in fiction
  • Dialectics, is an exchange of propositions resulting in a synthesis of the opposing assertions
  • Double bind, is a communicative situation where a person receives different or contradictory messages.
  • Doublethink, is the act of holding two contradictory beliefs simultaneously and fervently believing both.
  • Denial as a related defense mechanism.
  • False dilemma, involves a situation in which two alternative statements are held to be the only options...
  • Hostility (in the psychological sense) as a psychological response to cognitive dissonance
  • Javert
  • Paradox, paradox is an apparently true statement or group of statements that leads to a contradiction
  • Selective exposure theory
  • Self-perception theory, a competing theory of attitude change
  • True believer syndrome as an example of immunity to cognitive dissonance.

References

  • Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 4, pp1-34. Ne Jork: Academic Press.
  • Aronson, E. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1963) Effects of severity of threat in the devaluation of forbidden behavior, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 584-588
  • Bem, D.J. (1965). An experimental analysis of self-persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 199-218
  • Bem, D.J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200
  • Brehm, J. (1956). Post-decision changes in desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384-389
  • Burris, C. T., Harmon-Jones, E., Tarpley, W. R. (1997). “By faith alone”: Religious agitation and cognitive dissonance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 17-31.
  • Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When Prophecy fails a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World
  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
  • Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). "Cognitive consequences of forced compliance". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203-211. Full text.
  • Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When Prophecy fails a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  • Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., & Nelson, D. E. (1996). Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 5-16.
  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (1999). Cognitive Dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Sherman, S. J., & Gorkin, R. B. (1980). "Attitude bolstering when behavior is inconsistent with central attitudes". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 388-403.
  • Knox, R. E., & Inkster, J. A. (1968). "Postdecision dissonance at post time". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 319-323.
  • Tedeschi, J.T., Schlenker, B.R. & Bonoma, T.V. (1971). Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle? American Psychologist, 26, 685-695.


External links

This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).