Psychology Wiki
No edit summary
 
 
(13 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{ExpPsy}}
 
{{ExpPsy}}
'''Categorization''' is the process in which ideas and objects are recognised, [[difference|differentiated]] and understood. Categorization implies that objects are grouped into [[Wiktionary:categories|categories]], usually for some specific purpose. Ideally, a category illuminates a [[relation (mathematics)|relationship]] between the [[subject (philosophy)|subject]]s and [[object (philosophy)|object]]s of [[knowledge]]. Categorization is fundamental in [[decision making]] and in all kinds of interaction with the environment. There are, however, different ways of approaching categorization.
 
   
  +
This article is on the [[cognitive process]] for more general discussion and philosophy see [[Categorization]]
==The Classical View==
 
{{main|Categories (Aristotle)}}
 
The classical [[Aristotelian]] view claims that categories are discrete [[entity|entities]] characterized by a set of [[property (disambiguation)|properties]] which are shared by their members. These are assumed to establish the conditions which are both [[necessary and sufficient]] to capture [[meaning]].
 
   
 
'''Classification''' or is the [[cognitive process]] in which ideas and objects are recognised, differentiated and understood. Classification implies that objects are grouped into categories, usually for some specific purpose. Ideally, a category illuminates a [[relation (mathematics)|relationship]] between the [[subject (philosophy)|subject]]s and [[object (philosophy)|object]]s of [[knowledge]]. Categorization is fundamental in [[decision making]] and in all kinds of interaction with the environment. There are, however, different ways of approaching categorization.
According to the classical view, categories should be clearly defined, mutually exclusive and collectively exhaustive. This way, any entity of the given classification universe belongs unequivocally to one, and only one, of the proposed categories.
 
   
==Cognitive science: Prototype Theory==
+
==Cognitive science view==
  +
In recent years categorization has moved from an area of philosophical speculation towards an area of cognitive scientific study. A number of testable theories have been generated and experimental progress has been made.
  +
  +
== Prototype theory==
 
{{main|Prototype Theory}}
 
{{main|Prototype Theory}}
 
Since the research by [[Eleanor Rosch]] and [[George Lakoff]] in the 1970s, categorization can also be viewed as the process of grouping things based on prototypes - the idea of necessary and sufficient conditions is almost never met in categories of naturally occurring things. It has also been suggested that categorisation based on prototypes is the basis for human development, and that this [[learning theory (education)|learning]] relies on learning about the world via [[embodiment]].
 
Since the research by [[Eleanor Rosch]] and [[George Lakoff]] in the 1970s, categorization can also be viewed as the process of grouping things based on prototypes - the idea of necessary and sufficient conditions is almost never met in categories of naturally occurring things. It has also been suggested that categorisation based on prototypes is the basis for human development, and that this [[learning theory (education)|learning]] relies on learning about the world via [[embodiment]].
  +
  +
==Defining-attribute Theory==
  +
  +
==Exemplar based theory==
   
 
A [[cognitive]] approach accepts that natural categories are graded (they tend to be fuzzy at their boundaries) and inconsistent in the status of their constituent members.
 
A [[cognitive]] approach accepts that natural categories are graded (they tend to be fuzzy at their boundaries) and inconsistent in the status of their constituent members.
Line 16: Line 20:
 
Systems of categories are not objectively "out there" in the world but are rooted in people's experience. Conceptual categories are not identical for different cultures, or indeed, for every individual in the same culture.
 
Systems of categories are not objectively "out there" in the world but are rooted in people's experience. Conceptual categories are not identical for different cultures, or indeed, for every individual in the same culture.
   
Categories form part of a hierarchical structure when applied to such subjects as [[taxonomy]] in [[biological classification]]: higher level: life-form level, middle level: generic or [[genus]] level, and lower level: the [[species]] level. These can be distinguished by certain traits that put an item in its distinctive category. But even these can be arbitrary and are subject to revision.
+
Categories form part of a hierarchical structure when applied to such subjects as [[taxonomy]] in biological classification: higher level: life-form level, middle level: generic or genus level, and lower level: the [[species]] level. These can be distinguished by certain traits that put an item in its distinctive category. But even these can be arbitrary and are subject to revision.
   
 
Categories at the middle level are perceptually and conceptually the more salient. The generic level of a category tends to elicit the most responses and richest images and seems to be the psychologically basic level. Typical taxonomies in zoology for example exhibit categorisation at the [[embodiment|embodied]] level, with similarities leading to formulation of "higher" categories, and differences leading to differentiation within categories.
 
Categories at the middle level are perceptually and conceptually the more salient. The generic level of a category tends to elicit the most responses and richest images and seems to be the psychologically basic level. Typical taxonomies in zoology for example exhibit categorisation at the [[embodiment|embodied]] level, with similarities leading to formulation of "higher" categories, and differences leading to differentiation within categories.
   
 
== See also ==
 
== See also ==
  +
* [[Categorical perception]]
 
  +
* [[Choice behavior]]
  +
* [[Conceptualization]]
  +
* [[List of psychological tests requiring sorting]]
  +
* [[Neural processing for individual categories of objects]]
 
* [[Semantics]]
 
* [[Semantics]]
 
* [[Symbol grounding]]
 
* [[Symbol grounding]]
  +
  +
  +
[[Category:Classification]]
  +
  +
==References & Bibliography==
  +
==Key texts==
  +
===Books===
  +
Rosch, E., & Lloyd, B. B. (Eds.) (1978), [[Cognition and categorization]]. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  +
  +
===Papers===
  +
Categories and Induction ]
  +
*Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 32).
  +
  +
*Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), [[Cognition and categorization]]. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Reprinted in: Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (Eds.) (1999). Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  +
  +
*Rosch, E. (1977). Human categorization. In N. Warren (Ed.), Advances in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 1). London: Academic Press.
  +
  +
Mervis, C. & Rosch E. (1981). Categorization of Natural Objects. [[Annual Review of Psychology]], 32: 89-113. Annual Reviews, Inc.
  +
  +
*Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. [[Cognitive Psychology]], 8, 382-439
  +
  +
*Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. [[Cognitive Psychology]], 7, 573-605.
  +
  +
*Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. [[Journal of Experimental Psychology: General]], 104, 192-233.
  +
  +
  +
  +
==Additional material==
  +
===Books===
  +
  +
===Papers===
  +
*[http://scholar.google.com/scholar?sourceid=mozclient&num=50&scoring=d&ie=utf-8&oe=utf-8&q=Depression+women Google Scholar]
  +
  +
  +
   
 
==External links==
 
==External links==
Line 29: Line 72:
 
* [http://cogprints.org/3027/ Cognition is Categorization]
 
* [http://cogprints.org/3027/ Cognition is Categorization]
 
* [http://www-staff.lboro.ac.uk/~ssde/Edwards%20-%20Categories%20are%20for%20Talking%20T&P%201991.pdf Paper on the Discursive creation of categorisation]
 
* [http://www-staff.lboro.ac.uk/~ssde/Edwards%20-%20Categories%20are%20for%20Talking%20T&P%201991.pdf Paper on the Discursive creation of categorisation]
  +
* [http://www.javedabsar.com/public/pmwiki.php?n=Main.CognitivePsychology
   
  +
[[Category:Categorization]]
  +
[[Category:Classification (cognitive process)]]
  +
[[Category:Cognitive psychology]]
 
[[Category:Knowledge representation]]
 
[[Category:Knowledge representation]]
[[Category:Philosophical arguments]]
 
[[Category:Philosophical terminology]]
 
 
[[Category:Semantics]]
 
[[Category:Semantics]]
   
  +
<!--
 
[[cs:Kategorizace]]
 
[[cs:Kategorizace]]
 
[[de:Kategorisierung]]
 
[[de:Kategorisierung]]
 
[[gl:Categorización]]
 
[[gl:Categorización]]
  +
-->
 
{{enWP|Categorization}}
 
{{enWP|Categorization}}

Latest revision as of 12:37, 9 October 2013

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


This article is on the cognitive process for more general discussion and philosophy see Categorization

Classification or is the cognitive process in which ideas and objects are recognised, differentiated and understood. Classification implies that objects are grouped into categories, usually for some specific purpose. Ideally, a category illuminates a relationship between the subjects and objects of knowledge. Categorization is fundamental in decision making and in all kinds of interaction with the environment. There are, however, different ways of approaching categorization.

Cognitive science view

In recent years categorization has moved from an area of philosophical speculation towards an area of cognitive scientific study. A number of testable theories have been generated and experimental progress has been made.

Prototype theory

Main article: Prototype Theory

Since the research by Eleanor Rosch and George Lakoff in the 1970s, categorization can also be viewed as the process of grouping things based on prototypes - the idea of necessary and sufficient conditions is almost never met in categories of naturally occurring things. It has also been suggested that categorisation based on prototypes is the basis for human development, and that this learning relies on learning about the world via embodiment.

Defining-attribute Theory

Exemplar based theory

A cognitive approach accepts that natural categories are graded (they tend to be fuzzy at their boundaries) and inconsistent in the status of their constituent members.

Systems of categories are not objectively "out there" in the world but are rooted in people's experience. Conceptual categories are not identical for different cultures, or indeed, for every individual in the same culture.

Categories form part of a hierarchical structure when applied to such subjects as taxonomy in biological classification: higher level: life-form level, middle level: generic or genus level, and lower level: the species level. These can be distinguished by certain traits that put an item in its distinctive category. But even these can be arbitrary and are subject to revision.

Categories at the middle level are perceptually and conceptually the more salient. The generic level of a category tends to elicit the most responses and richest images and seems to be the psychologically basic level. Typical taxonomies in zoology for example exhibit categorisation at the embodied level, with similarities leading to formulation of "higher" categories, and differences leading to differentiation within categories.

See also

References & Bibliography

Key texts

Books

Rosch, E., & Lloyd, B. B. (Eds.) (1978), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Papers

Categories and Induction ]

  • Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 32).
  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Reprinted in: Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. (Eds.) (1999). Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Rosch, E. (1977). Human categorization. In N. Warren (Ed.), Advances in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 1). London: Academic Press.

Mervis, C. & Rosch E. (1981). Categorization of Natural Objects. Annual Review of Psychology, 32: 89-113. Annual Reviews, Inc.

  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382-439
  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605.


Additional material

Books

Papers



External links

This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).