Psychology Wiki
Register
Advertisement

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


Beat deafness is a form of congenital amusia characterized by a person's inability to feel musical rhythm or move in time to it.[1]

Characteristics[]

Generally, humans have the ability to hear musical beat and rhythm beginning in infancy. [2] Some people however, are unable to move in sync with the beat and rhythm of music, suffering from what is known as beat deafness. Beat deafness is a newly discovered form of congenital amusia, in which people lack the ability to perceive or “hear” the beat in a piece of music.[3] Unlike most hearing impairments in which an individual is unable to hear any sort of sound stimuli, those with beat deafness are generally able to hear normally, with the exception of beat and rhythm in music. Those with beat deafness are also unable to dance in step to any type of music. Even people who do not dance well can at least coordinate their movements to the song they are listening to, because they can easily keep time to the beat.[3]

Rarity[]

It is estimated that about 4% of people in Western Europe and North America are beat deaf. (cite: by whom? How?) The first reported case was of a Canadian graduate student, whom researchers have identified as “Mathieu”. Phillips-Silver et al. (2011) examined the human ability to recognize musical beat in a sample of individuals who had had no previous musical training in their lives. The researchers presented a series of songs from different genres and the participants were instructed to simply bounce up and down to the beat of the music. Results indicated that all participants except for Mathieu were able to move in sync with the beat of the music. The researchers also presented video clips which showed a person dancing to music. Mathieu could not identify when the person was or was not dancing in time to the music.[3] Other participants demonstrated no problem with this task.

The importance of being able to effectively process music has been demonstrated by past research. Music and dance have a great emotional and physical effect on humans. Historically, dance and music have been used as a method of self-expression and escape from stress and pain through changing emotional states and physical capabilities.[4] From findings such as these, it could be proposed that people who have beat deafness like Mathieu may have more stress because they cannot use music as a reliever.

Neural basis[]

When sound waves reach the ears, the energy they contain is converted into electrical signals, which are sent via the auditory nerves to the brain. Sound processing begins when these electrical signals reach the primary auditory receiving area in the core part of the temporal lobe.[5] Signals then travel to the area surrounding the core, known as the belt area, and are then transmitted to the parabelt area, which is located next to the belt. Simple sounds such as pure tones are able to activate the core area of the brain, but both the belt and parabelt areas are activated by only complex sounds, such as those found in speech and music.[5] The auditory cortex in the left hemisphere of the brain is responsible for processing beat and rhythm in music. The right auditory cortex is primarily used in distinguishing between different harmonics, which are simple pure tones that combine to create complex tones.[6]

Phillips-Silver et al. (2011) propose that beat deafness is the result of neurological problems in the areas of the brain that are used for recognizing musical beat, rhythm, and time. The main area responsible for processing musical rhythm is the left auditory cortex,[6] however other areas are most likely involved as well. According to the hypothesis of Phillips-Silver and coworkers, it should therefore be functional abnormalities in the left auditory cortex that cause beat deafness.[7]

Other areas of Mathieu’s brain appeared to be functioning normally, including the areas responsible for hearing in general and for motor control, which is used in performing the moves in dancing.[3] Mathieu’s deficiencies are therefore not caused by the inability to hear efficiently or control the movement of his body while dancing. Beat deafness has also not been shown to affect other areas of cognitive function such as language, which does not involve any sort of underlying beat or sporadic rhythm changes that are associated with music.[3] Given the normal functioning of Mathieu’s brain, the hypothesis about the beat perception deficit occurring in the brain area for rhythm processing in particular is most likely correct. Beat deafness is however, a very recent discovery and further research is necessary in gaining complete understanding of the phenomenon and its underlying brain processes.[7]

Comparison to tone deafness[]

Tone deafness is characterized by the inability to discriminate between different pitches, which are directly related to the frequencies of sound waves.[8] Tone deafness is a related, but distinct disorder from beat deafness. People with tone deafness can recognize beat and can move in time to music, but they cannot perceive pitch. People with beat deafness on the other hand, can recognize and distinguish between different tones as well as the average person and can usually sing in tune, so musical pitch is not the issue.[3] Different areas of the brain in the auditory cortex are involved in the perception of musical pitch and melody, and researchers theorize that tone deafness can potentially be from any of these sections.[8] Both beat deafness and tone deafness are derived from these same areas within the brain.

Beat perception in animals[]

File:Cacatua galerita Risdon.jpg

Sulphur-crested Cockatoos can detect the beat in music.

Most people have the inborn ability to perceive the beat in music. Once we become a bit older, we can use this ability to dance and move in step to music. Recent research has shown that beat perception is not unique to humans. Even some animals such as parrots can detect beat and can dance to the beat of music.[9] A research team led by Aniruddh D. Patel of The Neurosciences Institute concluded that Sulphur-crested Cockatoos have the ability to perceive the beat in music and are able to rhythmically move to the tempo of the music as it changes. Only vocal learning species such as dolphins and parrots are hypothesized to have the ability to perceive beat. This is because beat perception and movement rely on complex vocal learning which require motor and auditory circuits in the brain. Vocal learning and beat perception do some overlapping in the parts of the brain that account auditory and motor areas. There is no significant evidence for beat perception in nonvocal learning species such as dogs and cats.[9]

See also[]

.

References[]

  1. Bower, Bruce (26 March 2011). A man lost in musical time. Science News 179 (7): 9.
  2. Stewart, L. (2011) Characterizing congenital amusia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64 (4), 625-638.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 Phillips-Silver, J., Toiviainen, P., Gosselin, N., Piche, O., Nozaradan, S., Palmer, C., & Peretz, I. (2011). Born to dance but beat deaf: a new form of congenital amusia. Neuropsychologia, 49 (5), 961-969.
  4. Sandel, S., Judge, J., Landry, N., Faria, L., Ouellete, R., Majczak, M. (2005). Dance and movement program improves quality-of-life measures among breast cancer survivors. Cancer Nursing, 28 (4), 301-309
  5. 5.0 5.1 Goldstein, E. B. (2010). Sensation and perception. California: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning
  6. 6.0 6.1 Jourdain, R. (1997). Music, the brain, and ecstasy: How music captures our imagination. New York: William Morrow and Company
  7. 7.0 7.1 Honing, H. (2011). A case of congenital beat deafness? Amsterdam: Music Matters | A blog on music cognition.
  8. 8.0 8.1 Foxton, J.M., Nandy, R.K., & Griffiths, T.D. (2006). Rhythm deficits in ‘tone deafness’. Brain and Cognition, 62 (1), 24-29.
  9. 9.0 9.1 Patel, A.D., Iversen, J.R., Bregman, M.R., & Schulz, I. (2009). Studying synchronization to a musical beat in nonhuman animals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169 (1), 459-469.

Further reading[]

Phillips-Silver, Jessica, Petri Toiviainen, Nathalie Gosselin, Olivier Piché, Sylvie Nozaradan, Caroline Palmer, Isabelle Peretz (11 February 2011). Born to dance but beat deaf: A new form of congenital amusia. Neuropsychologia 49 (in press).

includeonly>Shea, Christopher. "The Man Who Couldn’t Keep a Beat", 7 March 2011. Retrieved on 21 March 2011.

"De man zonder ritme". [[1]].


External links[]

  • CBC Radio interview (Quirks & Quarks) with Dr. Jessica Phillips-Silver



Psychology of music
Music cognition
Absolute pitch | Biomusicology | Cognitive musicology | Developmental aspects of music | Embodied music cognition | Music acoustics | Music neuroscience]] | Music-related memory | Perception of music | Pitch discrimination | Pitch perception | Psychoacoustics | Relative pitch | Sound localization |
Aspects of Music theory
Melody | Harmonic | Harmonic series | Harmony | Key | Phrasing | Rhythm | Meter | tempo  Rhythm  Tonality |
Musical behaviors
Clapping | Dancing | Everyday music listening | Eye movement in music reading | Improvisation | Musical preferences | Psychology of music composition | Psychology of music performance | sight reading | Singing | [[]] | [[]] | [[]] |
Music education
Dalcroze method | Ear training | Kodaly method]] | Orff-Approach | Suzuki method | Mnemonic major system | Mnemonic peg system | [[]] |
Social psychology of music
Culture in music cognition | Culturally linked qualities | Ethnomusicology | Role in personal identity | Sociomusicology | Systemic musicology | [[]] |[[]] |
Assessment of musical ability
Music specific disorders | Amusia | Asonia |Dysmusia | Tone deafness | Seashore Tests of Musical Ability | [[]] | [[]] |[[]] |
Prominant workers
Jamshed Bharucha | Diana Deutsch |Carol L. Krumhans |Otto Laske |H. Christopher Longuet-Higgins | Helga de la Motte-Haber | John Sloboda | [[]] |
Journals
Music Perception | Psychology of Music |Jahrbuch Musikpsychologie |Journal of Research in Music Education |Musicae Scientiae | Psychomusicology | Empirical Musicology Review | Codex Flores |
Miscellaneous
Journals |Musicology |Music therapy |Musical instruments | Musicians |Rock music | [[]] | [[]] |
Advertisement