Psychology Wiki
Advertisement


Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


In psychoacoustics, Auditory masking occurs when the auditory perception of one sound is affected by the presence of another sound.[1]

Auditory masking in the frequency domain is known as simultaneous masking, frequency masking or spectral masking. Auditory masking in the time domain is known as temporal masking or non-simultaneous masking.

Masked threshold

The unmasked threshold is the quietest level of the signal which can be perceived without a masking signal present. The masked threshold is the quietest level of the signal perceived when combined with a specific masking noise. The amount of masking is the difference between the masked and unmasked thresholds.

Gelfand provides a basic example.[1] Let us say that for a given individual, the sound of a cat scratching a post in an otherwise quiet environment is first audible at a level of 10 dB SPL. However, in the presence of a masking noise (for example, a vacuum cleaner that is running simultaneously) that same individual cannot detect the sound of the cat scratching unless the level of the scratching sound is at least 26 dB SPL. We would say that the unmasked threshold for that individual for the target sound (i.e., the cat scratching) is 10 dB SPL, while the masked threshold is 26 dB SPL. The amount of masking is simply the difference between these two thresholds: 16 dB.

The amount of masking will vary depending on the characteristics of both the target signal and the masker, and will also be specific to an individual listener. While the person in the example above was able to detect the cat scratching at 26 dB SPL, another person may not be able to hear the cat scratching while the vacuum was on until the sound level of the cat scratching was increased to 30 dB SPL (thereby making the amount of masking for the second listener 20 dB).

Simultaneous masking

Simultaneous masking occurs when a sound is made inaudible by a noise or unwanted sound of the same duration as the original sound.[2] For example, a powerful spike at 1 kHz will tend to mask out a lower-level tone at 1.1 kHz. Also, two sine tones at 440 and 450 Hz can be perceived clearly when separated. They cannot be perceived clearly when presented simultaneously.

A not masked threshold is the quietest level of the signal which can be perceived in quiet. Masked thresholds are the quietest level of the signal perceived when presented in noise. The amount of masking is the difference between the masked and not masked thresholds (Gelfand 2004). For example if the masked threshold is 35dB and the not masked threshold is 20dB the amount of masking would be 15dB. This is illustrated in figure A.

File:Masker increased threshold.svg

figure A Adapted from a diagram by Gelfand (2004)

The basic masking test involves the not masked thresholds being measured on a subject. Then the masking noise is introduced at a fixed sound level and the signal is presented at the same time. The level of the signal is varied until the new threshold is measured. This is the masked threshold (Gelfand 2004).

The phenomenon of masking is often used to investigate the auditory system’s ability to separate the components of a complex sound. For example if two sounds of two different frequencies (pitches) are played at the same time, two separate sounds can often be heard rather than a combination tone. This is otherwise known as frequency resolution or frequency selectivity. Frequency resolution is thought to occur due to filtering within the cochlea, the hearing organ in the inner ear. A complex sound is split into different frequency components and these components cause a peak in the pattern of vibration at a specific place on the basilar membrane within the cochlea. These components are then coded independently on the auditory nerve which transmits sound information to the brain. This individual coding only occurs if the frequency components are different enough in frequency, otherwise they are coded at the same place (Moore 1986).

These filters are called auditory filters or listening channels and it is thought that they line up along the basilar membrane, overlapping. Frequency resolution is said to occur on the basilar membrane due to the listener choosing a filter which is centred over the frequency they want to hear, the signal frequency. A sharply tuned filter has good frequency resolution as it allows the centre frequencies through but not other frequencies (Pickles 1982). Damage to the cochlea and the outer hair cells in the cochlea cause reduced sharpness of tuning (Moore 1986). This explains why someone with a hearing loss due to cochlea damage would have more problems than a normal hearing person with frequency selectivity. This would cause them, for example, to have difficulties distinguishing between different consonants in speech (Moore 1995).

Masking illustrates the limits of frequency selectivity even in a normal hearing person. If a signal is masked by a masker with a different frequency to the signal then the auditory system was unable to distinguish between the two frequencies. Therefore by carrying out an experiment to see the conditions which are necessary for one sound to mask a previously heard signal, the frequency selectivity of the auditory system can be investigated (Moore 1998).

Effect of frequency on masking patterns

How effective the masker is at raising the threshold of the signal depends on the frequency of the signal and the frequency of the masker. The graphs in figure B are a series of masking patterns, otherwise known as masking audiograms adapted from findings by Ehmer (Gelfand 2004). Each graph shows the amount of masking produced at each masker frequency shown at the top corner, 250, 500, 1000 and 2000Hz. For example, in the first graph the masker is presented at a frequency of 250Hz at the same time as the signal. The amount the masker increases the threshold of the signal is plotted and this is repeated for different signal frequencies, shown on the X axis. The frequency of the masker is kept constant. The masking effect is shown in each graph at various masker sound levels.

File:Maskingpatterns sp11.jpg

figure B Adapted from Ehmer, Illustrating changes in masking patterns for different masker frequencies and intensities

Figure B shows along the Y axis the amount of masking- so how much the not masked threshold in quiet is raised to get the masked threshold in noise. The X axis shows the frequency of the signal. You can see that the greatest masking is at the centre frequency, when the masker and the signal are the same frequency, and this decreases as the signal moves further away from the masker frequency (Gelfand 2004). This phenomenon is called on-frequency masking and occurs because the masker and signal are within the same auditory filter (figure C). This means that the auditory system can not distinguish between them and so the signal is masked (Gelfand 2004).

File:Auditoryfiltermaskersignal1.svg

figure C Showing on frequency masking where the frequency of the signal is within the frequency band of the masker

File:Off frequency mask diff freq1.svg

figure D- showing off frequency masking where the frequency of the signal is outside the frequency band of the masker

Off-frequency masking is when the signal and masker are at different frequencies (figure D.)

The amount the masker raises the threshold of the signal is much less in off frequency masking. You can see from figure E however, that it does have some masking effect because some of the masker overlaps into the auditory filter of the signal (Moore 1998).

File:Maskersameauditoryfilter1.svg

figure E Showing the amount of masker which shares the same auditory filter/listening channel as the signal

Off frequency masking requires the level of the masker to be greater in order to have a masking effect; this is shown in figure F.

File:Onandofffreqlistening1.svg

figure F Showing the relationship between the level of the masker and the masking threshold for both on and off frequency masking

This is because only a certain amount of the masker overlaps into the auditory filter (see Auditory filters) of the signal, therefore more masker is needed to mask the signal (Moore 1998).

You can also see from figure B that the masking pattern changes depending on the frequency of the masker and the intensity. You can observe from the 1000Hz graph that for low levels e.g. 20-40 dB the curve is relatively symmetrical. As the masker intensity increases the curves become wider with greater masking particularly to signals at a frequency higher than the masker (Gelfand 2004). This shows that there is a spread of the masking effect upward in frequency as the intensity of the masker is increased. The curve is much shallower in the high frequencies than in the low frequencies and this is termed upward spread of masking. This means that a sound (masker) masks high frequency signals much better than low frequency signals (Gelfand 2004).

You can also observe from figure B that as the masker frequency increases, the masking patterns become increasingly compressed. This demonstrates that high frequency maskers are only effective over a narrow range of frequencies, close to the masker frequency. Low frequency maskers on the other hand are effective over a wide frequency range (Gelfand 2004). This is due to particular patterns of activity on the basilar membrane.

As mentioned before, masking experiments reveal information about the frequency selectivity of the ear and the listening channels/auditory filters which are used to distinguish between one frequency and another. Fletcher carried out an experiment to discover how much of a band of noise contributes to the masking of a tone. He carried out a masking experiment whereby a fixed tone signal had various bandwidths of noise centred on it. The masked threshold was recorded for each bandwidth. His research showed that there is a critical bandwidth of noise which causes maximum masking effect and energy outside this critical band does not have an influence on the masking effect. This can be explained by the auditory system having an auditory filter which is centred over the frequency of the tone. The bandwidth of the masker which is within this auditory filter effectively masks the tone but the rest of the masker which is outside the filter has no effect (figure G.)

File:Maskercriticalbandwidth1.svg

figure G Showing the amount of masker which contributes to the masking of the tone signal - known as critical bandwidth. Energy outside the auditory filter does not contribute to the masking of the tone signal. Adapted from a diagram by Gelfand (2004)

This is used in Mp3 files to reduce the size of audio files. Parts of the signals which are outside the critical bandwidth are cut out leaving only the parts of the signals which are perceived by the listener (Sellars 2000). Another application of auditory masking in everyday situations is the cocktail party effect.


Critical bandwidth

If two sounds of two different frequencies are played at the same time, two separate sounds can often be heard rather than a combination tone. The ability to hear frequencies separately is known as frequency resolution or frequency selectivity. When signals are as a combination tone, they are said to reside in the same critical bandwidth. This effect is thought to occur due to filtering within the cochlea, the hearing organ in the inner ear. A complex sound is split into different frequency components and these components cause a peak in the pattern of vibration at a specific place on the cilia inside the basilar membrane within the cochlea. These components are then coded independently on the auditory nerve which transmits sound information to the brain. This individual coding only occurs if the frequency components are different enough in frequency, otherwise they are in the same critical band and are coded at the same place and are perceived as one sound instead of two.[3]

The filters that distinguish one sound from another are called auditory filters, listening channels or critical bandwidths. Frequency resolution occurs on the basilar membrane due to the listener choosing a filter which is centered over the frequency they expect to hear, the signal frequency. A sharply tuned filter has good frequency resolution as it allows the center frequencies through but not other frequencies (Pickles 1982). Damage to the cochlea and the outer hair cells in the cochlea can impair the ability to tell sounds apart (Moore 1986). This explains why someone with a hearing loss due to cochlea damage would have more difficulty than a normal hearing person in distinguishing between different consonants in speech.[4]

Masking illustrates the limits of frequency selectivity. If a signal is masked by a masker with a different frequency to the signal then the auditory system was unable to distinguish between the two frequencies. By experimenting with conditions where one sound can mask a previously heard signal, the frequency selectivity of the auditory system can be tested.[5]

Similar frequencies

File:Maskingpatterns sp11.jpg

Figure B - Adapted from Ehmer

How effective the masker is at raising the threshold of the signal depends on the frequency of the signal and the frequency of the masker. The graphs in Figure B are a series of masking patterns, also known as masking audiograms. Each graph shows the amount of masking produced at each masker frequency shown at the top corner, 250, 500, 1000 and 2000 Hz. For example, in the first graph the masker is presented at a frequency of 250 Hz at the same time as the signal. The amount the masker increases the threshold of the signal is plotted and this is repeated for different signal frequencies, shown on the X axis. The frequency of the masker is kept constant. The masking effect is shown in each graph at various masker sound levels.

File:Auditoryfiltermaskersignal1.svg

Figure C - Adapted from Gelfand 2004[1]

File:Off frequency mask diff freq1.svg

Figure D- Adapted from Gelfand 2004[1]

Figure B shows along the Y axis the amount of masking. The greatest masking is when the masker and the signal are the same frequency and this decreases as the signal frequency moves further away from the masker frequency.[1] This phenomenon is called on-frequency masking and occurs because the masker and signal are within the same auditory filter (Figure C). This means that the listener cannot distinguish between them and they are perceived as one sound with the quieter sound masked by the louder one (Figure D).

File:Maskersameauditoryfilter1.svg

Figure E - adapted from Moore 1998[5]

The amount the masker raises the threshold of the signal is much less in off-frequency masking, but it does have some masking effect because some of the masker overlaps into the auditory filter of the signal (Figure E)[5]

File:Onandofffreqlistening1.svg

Figure F - adapted from Moore 1998[5]

Off-frequency masking requires the level of the masker to be greater in order to have a masking effect; this is shown in Figure F. This is because only a certain amount of the masker overlaps into the auditory filter of the signal and more masker is needed to cover the signal.[5]

Lower frequencies

The masking pattern changes depending on the frequency of the masker and the intensity (Figure B). For low levels on the 1000 Hz graph, such as the 20-40 dB range, the curve is relatively parallel. As the masker intensity increases the curves separate, especially for signals at a frequency higher than the masker. This shows that there is a spread of the masking effect upward in frequency as the intensity of the masker is increased. The curve is much shallower in the high frequencies than in the low frequencies. This flattening is called upward spread of masking and is why an interfering sound masks high frequency signals much better than low frequency signals.[1]

Figure B also shows that as the masker frequency increases, the masking patterns become increasingly compressed. This demonstrates that high frequency maskers are only effective over a narrow range of frequencies, close to the masker frequency. Low frequency maskers on the other hand are effective over a wide frequency range.[1]

File:Maskercriticalbandwidth1.svg

Figure G - adapted from a diagram by Gelfand[1]

Harvey Fletcher carried out an experiment to discover how much of a band of noise contributes to the masking of a tone. In the experiment, a fixed tone signal had various bandwidths of noise centered on it. The masked threshold was recorded for each bandwidth. His research showed that there is a critical bandwidth of noise which causes the maximum masking effect and energy outside that band does not affect the masking. This can be explained by the auditory system having an auditory filter which is centered over the frequency of the tone. The bandwidth of the masker that is within this auditory filter effectively masks the tone but the masker outside of the filter has no effect (Figure G.)

This is used in MP3 files to reduce the size of audio files. Parts of the signals which are outside the critical bandwidth are represented with reduced precision. The parts of the signals which are perceived by the listener are reproduced with higher fidelity.[6]

Effects of intensity

File:OutputlevelMoore.svg

Figure H - adapted from Moore 1998[5]

Varying intensity levels can also have an effect on masking. The lower end of the filter becomes flatter with increasing decibel level, whereas the higher end becomes slightly steeper. Changes in slope of the high frequency side of the filter with intensity are less consistent than they are at low frequencies. At the medium frequencies (1–4 kHz) the slope increases as intensity increases, but at the low frequencies there is no clear inclination with level and the filters at high center frequencies show a small decrease in slope with increasing level. The sharpness of the filter depends on the input level and not the output level to the filter. The lower side of the auditory filter also broadens with increasing level.[5] These observations are illustrated in Figure H.

Temporal masking

Temporal masking or non-simultaneous masking occurs when a sudden stimulus sound makes inaudible other sounds which are present immediately preceding or following the stimulus. Masking which obscures a sound immediately preceding the masker is called backward masking or pre-masking and masking which obscures a sound immediately following the masker is called forward masking or post-masking.[5] Temporal masking's effectiveness attenuates exponentially from the onset and offset of the masker, with the onset attenuation lasting approximately 20 ms and the offset attenuation lasting approximately 100 ms.

Similar to simultaneous masking, temporal masking reveals the frequency analysis performed by the auditory system; forward masking thresholds for complex harmonic tones (e.g., a sawtooth probe with a fundamental frequency of 500 Hz) exhibit threshold peaks (i.e., high masking levels) for frequency bands centered on the first several harmonics. In fact, auditory bandwidths measured from forward masking thresholds are narrower and more accurate than those measured using simultaneous masking.

Temporal masking should not be confused with the ear's acoustic reflex, an involuntary response in the middle ear that is activated to protect the ear's delicate structures from loud sounds.

Other masking conditions

File:Ipsisimmasking.png

figure I - ipsilateral simultaneous masking

Ipsilateral ("same side") masking is not the only condition where masking takes place. Another situation where masking occurs is called contralateral ("other side") simultaneous masking. In this case, the instance where the signal might be audible in one ear but is deliberately taken away by applying a masker to the other ear.

The last situation where masking occurs is called central masking. This refers to the case where a masker causes a threshold elevation. This can be in the absence of, or in addition to, another effect and is due to interactions within the central nervous system between the separate neural inputs obtained from the masker and the signal.[1]

Effects of different stimulus types

Experiments have been carried out to see the different masking effects when using a masker which is either in the form of a narrow band noise or a sinusoidal tone.

When a sinusoidal signal and a sinusoidal masker (tone) are presented simultaneously the envelope of the combined stimulus fluctuates in a regular pattern described as beats. The fluctuations occur at a rated defined by the difference between the frequencies of the two sounds. If the frequency difference is small then the sound is perceived as a periodic change in the loudness of a single tone. If the beats are fast then this can be described as a sensation of roughness. When there is a large frequency separation, the two components are heard as separate tones without roughness or beats. Beats can be a cue to the presence of a signal even when the signal itself is not audible. The influence of beats can be reduced by using a narrowband noise rather than a sinusoidal tone for either signal or masker.[3]

Ipsilateral, contralateral and central masking

Masking can be carried out in several different conditions. One of these being Ipsilateral, simultaneous masking which refers to the instance where masker and maskee are both delivered to the test ear at the same time. This can be both on-frequency or off-frequency (Gelfand 2004).

File:Ipsisimmasking.png

figure I Demonstrating ipsilateral simultaneous masking

Another condition of masking is contralateral simultaneous masking. This condition of masking refers to the instance where the signal might be audible in the non-test ear (through transcranial conduction) but is deliberately obliterated by applying a masker to the non-test ear. The last condition of masking is central masking. This refers to the case where a masker causes a threshold elevation (makes a previously heard signal inaudible) in the absence of, or additional to, any ipsilateral, contralateral or cross-masking effect. It is due to interactions within the central nervous system between the separate neural inputs derived from the masker and the signal (Gelfand 2004).

Mechanisms of masking

There are many different mechanisms of masking, one being suppression. This is when there is a reduction of a response to a signal due to the presence of another. This happens because the original neural activity caused by the first signal is reduced by the neural activity of the other sound.[7]

Combination tones are products of a signal and a masker. This happens when the two sounds interact causing new sound, which can be more audible than the original signal. This is caused by the non linear distortion that happens in the ear. For example, the combination tone of two maskers can be a better masker than the two original maskers alone.[5]

The sounds interact in many ways depending on the difference in frequency between the two sounds. The most important two are cubic difference tones and quadratic difference tones.[5]

Cubic difference tones are calculated by the sum

F1 – F2

(F1 being the first frequency, F2 the second) These are audible most of the time and especially when the level of the original tone is low. Hence they have a greater effect on psychoacoustic tuning curves than quadratic difference tones.

Quadratic difference tones are the result of

F2 – F1

This happens at relatively high levels hence have a lesser effect on psychoacoustic tuning curves.[5]

Combination tones can interact with primary tones resulting in secondary combination tones due to being like their original primary tones in nature, stimulus like. An example of this is

3F1 – 2F2

Secondary combination tones are again similar to the combination tones of the primary tone.[5]




Off frequency listening

Off frequency listening is when a listener chooses a filter just lower than the signal frequency to improve their auditory performance. This “off frequency” filter reduces the level of the masker more than the signal at the output level of the filter, which means they can hear the signal more clearly hence causing an improvement of auditory performance (Moore 2004).

Non-simultaneous masking

Main article: Temporal masking

Non simultaneous masking is when the signal and masker are not presented at the same time. This can be split into forward masking and backward masking. Forward masking is when the masker is presented first and the signal follows it. Backward masking is when the signal precedes the masker (Moore 1998).

Sound masking Systems

The effect of auditory masking is used in Sound masking systems. These are audio systems that broadcast White noise for the purpose of hiding an unwanted sound. The unwanted noise may be intermittent sounds from machinery, people or other sources. Usually, this sound is filtered to provide the best effect of hiding the unwanted noise.


See also

References & Bibliography

Key texts

Books

  • Gelfand, S.A. (2004) Hearing- An Introduction to Psychological and Physiological Acoustics 4th Ed. New York, Marcel Dekker
  • Moore, B.C.J. (2004) An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th Ed. London, Elsevier Academic Press
  • Moore, B.C.J. (1986) Frequency Selectivity in Hearing, London, Academic Press
  • Moore, B.C.J. (1995) Perceptual Consequences of Cochlear Damage, Oxford, Oxford University Press
  • Moore, B.C.J. (1998) Cochlear Hearing Loss, London, Whurr Publishers Ltd
  • Pickles, J.O. (1982) An Introduction to the Physiology of Hearing, London, Academic Press
  • Sellars, P. (2000) Behind the Mask, Cambridge, Sound on Sound, Available from: http://www.soundonsound.com/sos/may00/articles/mp3.htm [Accessed on 26th February 2007]


Papers

  • Abdala, C., & Folsom, R. C. (1995). The development of frequency resolution in humans as revealed by the auditory brain-stem response recorded with notched-noise masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(2, Pt 1) Aug 1995, 921-930.
  • Adams, M. R., & Hutchinson, J. (1974). The effects of three levels of auditory masking on selected vocal characteristics and the frequency of disfluency of adult stutterers: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 17(4) Dec 1974, 682-688.
  • Adams, M. R., & Moore, W. H. (1972). The effects of auditory masking on the anxiety level, frequency of dysfluency, and selected vocal characteristics of stutterers: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 15(3) Sep 1972, 572-578.
  • Adams, S. G., & Lang, A. E. (1992). Can the Lombard effect be used to improve low voice intensity in Parkinson's disease? : European Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 27(2) 1992, 121-127.
  • Aikawa, K., Singer, H., Kawahara, H., & Tohkura, Y. i. (1996). Cepstral representation of speech motivated by time-frequency masking: An application to speech recognition: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(1) Jul 1996, 603-614.
  • Aithal, V., Yonovitz, A., & Aithal, S. (2006). Tonal Masking Level Differences in Aboriginal Children: Implications for Binaural Interaction, Auditory Processing Disorders and Education: Australian and New Zealand Journal of Audiology Vol 28(1) May 2006, 31-40.
  • Aithal, V., Yonovitz, A., Aithal, S., & Dold, N. (2006). Tonal Masking Level Difference in Children: Australian and New Zealand Journal of Audiology Vol 28(1) May 2006, 11-17.
  • Albeck, Y., Nebenzahl, I., & Lewis, A. (1992). Temporal structure model of binaural masking level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(3) Sep 1992, 1389-1393.
  • Alfonso, P. J. (1978). Contextual influences on phonetic feature recognition in dichotic backward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Allan, L. G., & Rousseau, R. (1977). Backward masking in judgments of duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 21(5) May 1977, 482-486.
  • Allan, L. M., Williams, J. H., Wellman, N. A., Tonin, J., & et al. (1995). Effects of tobacco smoking, schizotypy and number of pre-exposures on latent inhibition in healthy subjects: Personality and Individual Differences Vol 19(6) Dec 1995, 893-902.
  • Allard, J. B., & Williamson, D. G. (1974). Relative distractibility of wide-band and narrow-band noise in audiometric masking: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 14(3) Jul 1974, 220-222.
  • Allen, J., & Haggard, M. (1978). Backward masking and asymmetry of processing for vowels differing in acoustical similarity: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 30(1) Feb 1978, 43-55.
  • Allen, P., & Wightman, F. (1995). Effects of signal and masker uncertainty on children's detection: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 38(2) Apr 1995, 503-511.
  • Allen, S. K. (1982). Masking level differences in otitis prone learning disabled children: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Al'tman, Y. A., & Nikitin, N. I. (2000). Properties of derived cochlear action potentials in forward tonal masking in guinea pigs: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 30(5) Sep-Oct 2000, 587-598.
  • Al'tman, Y. A., & Ogorodnikova, E. A. (1991). Binaural release from masking with non-simultaneous presentation of masker and signal: Sensory Systems Vol 5(2) Apr-Jun 1991, 127-130.
  • Al'tman, Y. A., & Vaitulevich, S. F. (1999). Manifestation of the phenomenon of successive backward masking in human long-latency auditory evoked potentials: Human Physiology Vol 25(1) Jan-Feb 1999, 27-33.
  • Al'tman, Y. A., Vaitulevich, S. F., Mal'tseva, N. V., Pak, S. P., & et al. (1993). Binaural mechanisms of auditory system of noise tolerance as reflected in long latency auditory evoked potentials in humans: Sensory Systems Vol 7(2) Apr-Jun 1993, 93-96.
  • Altrows, I. F., & Bryden, M. P. (1977). Temporal factors in the effects of masking noise on fluency of stutterers: Journal of Communication Disorders Vol 10(4) Jun 1977, 315-329.
  • Alwan, A. A. (1992). Modeling speech perception in noise: The stop consonants as a case study: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Ananthanarayan, A. K. (1983). Alterations in the auditory brainstem evoked responses in a forward-masking paradigm: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Ananthanarayan, A. K., & Gerken, G. M. (1983). Post-stimulatory effects on the auditory brain stem response: Partial-masking and enhancement: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 55(2) Feb 1983, 223-226.
  • Ananthanarayan, A. K., & Gerken, G. M. (1987). Response enhancement and reduction of the auditory brain-stem response in a forward-masking paradigm: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 66(4) Apr 1987, 427-439.
  • Andreeva, I. G., & Vartanyan, I. A. (2000). The reaction time during perception of acoustic models of approach and retreat of sound in backward masking conditions: Human Physiology Vol 26(4) Jul-Aug 2000, 494-496.
  • Androulidakis, A. G., & Jones, S. J. (2006). Detection of signals in modulated and unmodulated noise observed using auditory evoked potentials: Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 117(8) Aug 2006, 1783-1793.
  • Anourova, I., Rama, P., Alho, K., Koivusalo, S., Kahnari, J., & Carlson, S. (1999). Selective interference reveals dissociation between auditory memory for location and pitch: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 10(17) Nov 1999, 3543-3547.
  • Arbogast, T. L., & Kidd, G., Jr. (2000). Evidence for spatial tuning in informational masking using the probe-signal method: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(4) Oct 2000, 1803-1810.
  • Arbogast, T. L., Mason, C. R., & Kidd, G., Jr. (2002). The effect of spatial separation on informational and energetic masking of speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(5,Pt1) Nov 2002, 2086-2098.
  • Arehole, S., Salvi, R. J., Saunders, S. S., & Gratton, M. A. (1989). Evoked-response forward-masking functions in chinchillas with noise-induced permanent hearing loss: Audiology Vol 28(2) Mar-Apr 1989, 92-110.
  • Arrabito, G. R. (2006). Three-Dimensional Auditory Display for Enhancing Detection of Passive Sonar Signals: Human Factors Vol 48(3) Fal 2006, 465-473.
  • Attias, J., Shemesh, Z., Sohmer, H., Gold, S., & et al. (1993). Comparison between self-hypnosis, masking and attentiveness for alleviation of chronic tinnitus: Audiology Vol 32(3) May-Jun 1993, 205-212.
  • Ayres, T. J. (1981). A dichotic probe investigation of octave phase effects: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bachrach, D. L. (1964). Sex differences in reactions to delayed auditory feedback: Perceptual and Motor Skills 19(1) 1964, 81-82.
  • Bacon, S. P. (1985). Temporal factors in simultaneous pure-tone masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bacon, S. P. (1990). Effect of masker level on overshoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(2) Aug 1990, 698-702.
  • Bacon, S. P. (1996). "Manipulations of the duration and relative onsets of two-tone forward maskers": Comment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(5) May 1996, 3246-3248.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Blake, P. E. (1990). Temporal effects in simultaneous masking by vowel and consonant-vowel maskers: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 33(1) Mar 1990, 38-44.
  • Bacon, S. P., Boden, L. N., Lee, J., & Repovsch, J. L. (1999). Growth of simultaneous masking for f-sub(m)<f-sub(s): Effects of overall frequency and level: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(1) Jul 1999, 341-350.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Brandt, J. F. (1982). Auditory processing of vowels by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(3) Sep 1982, 339-347.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Grantham, D. W. (1989). Modulation masking: Effects of modulation frequency, depth, and phase: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(6) Jun 1989, 2575-2580.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Grantham, D. W. (1992). Fringe effects in modulation masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(6) Jun 1992, 3451-3455.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Healy, E. W. (2000). Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral precursors on the temporal effect in simultaneous masking with pure tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(3) Mar 2000, 1589-1597.
  • Bacon, S. P., Hedrick, M. S., & Grantham, D. W. (1988). Temporal effects in simultaneous pure-tone masking in subjects with high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss: Audiology Vol 27(6) Nov-Dec 1988, 313-323.
  • Bacon, S. P., Hicks, M. L., & Johnson, K. L. (2000). Temporal integration in the presence of off-frequency maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(2) Feb 2000, 922-932.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Jesteadt, W. (1987). Effects of pure-tone forward masker duration on psychophysical measures of frequency selectivity: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(6) Dec 1987, 1925-1932.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Konrad, D. L. (1993). Modulation detection interference under conditions favoring within- or across-channel processing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(2) Feb 1993, 1012-1022.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Lee, J. (1997). The modulated-unmodulated difference: Effects of signal frequency and masker modulation depth: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(6) Jun 1997, 3617-3624.
  • Bacon, S. P., Lee, J., Peterson, D. N., & Rainey, D. (1997). Masking by modulated and unmodulated noise: Effects of bandwidth, modulation rate, signal frequency, and masker level: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(3) Mar 1997, 1600-1610.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Liu, L. (2000). Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral precursors on overshoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(4) Oct 2000, 1811-1818.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Liu, L. (2000). On the decline in overshoot at high masker levels: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(4) Apr 2000, 2295-2297.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Moore, B. C. (1993). Modulation detection interference: Some spectral effects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(6) Jun 1993, 3442-3453.
  • Bacon, S. P., Moore, B. C. J., Shailer, M. J., & Jorasz, U. (1995). Effects of combining maskers in modulation detection interference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(3) Mar 1995, 1847-1853.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Opie, J. M. (1994). Monotic and dichotic modulation detection interference in practiced and unpracticed subjects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(5, Pt 1) May 1994, 2637-2641.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Opie, J. M. (2002). Modulation detection interference in listeners with normal and impaired hearing: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 45(2) Apr 2002, 392-402.
  • Bacon, S. P., Opie, J. M., & Montoya, D. Y. (1998). The effects of hearing loss and noise masking on the masking release for speech in temporally complex backgrounds: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 41(3) Jun 1998, 549-563.
  • Bacon, S. P., Repovsch-Duffey, J. L., & Liu, L. (2002). Effects of signal delay on auditory filter shapes derived from psychophysical tuning curves and notched-noise data obtained in simultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(1) Jul 2002, 227-237.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Smith, M. A. (1991). Spectral, intensive, and temporal factors influencing overshoot: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 43A(3) Aug 1991, 373-399.
  • Bacon, S. P., & Takahashi, G. A. (1992). Overshoot in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(5) May 1992, 2865-2871.
  • Baddeley, A., & Salame, P. (1986). The unattended speech effect: Perception or memory? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 12(4) Oct 1986, 525-529.
  • Baer, T., & Moore, B. C. J. (1994). Effects of spectral smearing on the intelligibility of sentences in the presence of interfering speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(4) Apr 1994, 2277-2280.
  • Bailey, W. J., & Morris, G. K. (1986). Confusion of phonotaxis by masking sounds in the bushcricket Conocephalus brevipennis (Tettigoniidae: Concephalinae): Ethology formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie Vol 73(1) Sep 1986, 19-28.
  • Balakrishnan, U. (1994). Effect of onset cues on lateralization and binaural masking. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Balakrishnan, U., & Freyman, R. L. (2002). Lateralization and detection of pulse trains with alternating interaural time delays: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(4) Oct 2002, 1605-1616.
  • Balthazor, R. J. (1976). A psychoacoustic technique for determining tuning characteristics: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Banbury, S., & Berry, D. C. (1997). Habituation and dishabituation to speech and office noise: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied Vol 3(3) Sep 1997, 181-195.
  • Bartlett, E. L., & Wang, X. (2005). Long-Lasting Modulation by Stimulus Context in Primate Auditory Cortex: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 94(1) Jul 2005, 83-104.
  • Bashford, J. A. (1984). The illusory continuity and enhanced intelligibility produced by adding noise to periodic silent gaps in speech: A study of multiple phonemic restoration: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bashford, J. A., & Warren, R. M. (1987). Multiple phonemic restorations follow the rules for auditory induction: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 42(2) Aug 1987, 114-121.
  • Bauer, J. W. (1978). Tuning curves and masking functions of auditory-nerve fibers in cat: Sensory Processes Vol 2(2) Jun 1978, 156-172.
  • Bazhin, E. F., Meerson, Y. A., & Tonkonogii, I. M. (1973). On distinguishing a visual signal from noise by patients with visual agnosia and visual hallucinations: Neuropsychologia Vol 11(3) Jul 1973, 319-324.
  • Beattie, R. C., Pappas, N. J., & Smiarowski, R. A. (1974). Forward masking: Interaction of probe duration and mask duration: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 14(4) Oct 1974, 263-272.
  • Beattie, R. C., & Smiarowski, R. (1974). Forward masking: Effects of practice and psychophysical technique: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 14(4) Oct 1974, 273-278.
  • Bee, M. A. (2007). Sound source segregation in grey treefrogs: Spatial release from masking by the sound of a chorus: Animal Behaviour Vol 74(3) Sep 2007, 549-558.
  • Bee, M. A., & Swanson, E. M. (2007). Auditory masking of anuran advertisement calls by road traffic noise: Animal Behaviour Vol 74(6) Dec 2007, 1765-1776.
  • Bekhterev, N. N., Vaitulevich, S. F., Nikitin, N. I., & Shestopalova, L. B. (2002). Comparison of binaural release from forward masking in animals and humans. Electrophysiological studies: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 32(1) Jan-Feb 2002, 71-79.
  • Bench, J., & Wilson, I. (1975). A comparison of live and videorecord viewing of infant behavior under sound stimulation: II. Six-week-old infants: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 8(4) Jul 1975, 347-353.
  • Bentin, S., & Mann, V. A. (1990). Masking and stimulus intensity effects on duplex perception: A confirmation of the dissociation between speech and nonspeech modes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(1) Jul 1990, 64-74.
  • Benton, S. L., & Sheeley, E. C. (1987). Effects of three contralateral maskers on pure-tone thresholds using manual audiometry: Audiology Vol 26(4) Jul-Aug 1987, 227-234.
  • Berg, B. G. (1996). On the relation between comodulation masking release and temporal modulation transfer functions: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(2, Pt 1) Aug 1996, 1013-1023.
  • Berg, K. M. (1985). Temporal masking level differences for transients: Further evidence for a short-term integrator: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 37(5) May 1985, 397-406.
  • Berg, K. M. (1991). Auditory temporal summation in infants and adults: Effects of stimulus bandwidth and masking noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 50(4) Oct 1991, 314-320.
  • Berg, K. M., & Boswell, A. E. (1999). Effect of masker level on infants' detection of tones in noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 61(1) Jan 1999, 80-86.
  • Bernstein, L. R. (1991). Spectral interference in a binaural detection task: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(3) Mar 1991, 1306-1313.
  • Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (1992). Discrimination of interaural envelope correlation and its relation to binaural unmasking at high frequencies: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 306-316.
  • Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (1993). Spectral interference in a binaural detection task: Effects of masker bandwidth and temporal fringe: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(2, Pt 1) Aug 1993, 735-742.
  • Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (1994). The effect of nonsimultaneous on-frequency and off-frequency cues on the detection of a tonal signal masked by narrow-band noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(2) Feb 1994, 920-930.
  • Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (2002). Enhancing sensitivity to interaural delays at high frequencies by using "transposed stimuli." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(3,Pt 1) Sep 2002, 1026-1036.
  • Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (2003). Enhancing interaural-delay-based extents of laterality at high frequencies by using "transposed stimuli": Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(6) Jun 2003, 3335-3347.
  • Bernstein, L. R., Trahiotis, C., & Hyde, E. L. (1998). Inter-individual differences in binaural detection of low-frequency or high-frequency tonal signals masked by narrow-band or broadband noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2069-2078.
  • Bernstein, R. S., & Raab, D. H. (1990). The effects of bandwidth on the detectability of narrow- and wideband signals: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(5) Nov 1990, 2115-2125.
  • Berry, R. C., & Nerbonne, G. P. (1971). A comparison of the masking functions of speech modulated and white noise: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 11(3) Jul 1971, 248-250.
  • Berstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (1995). Binaural interference effects measured with masking-level differences and with ITD- and IID-discrimination paradigms: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(1) Jul 1995, 155-163.
  • Blakeslee, E. A. (1974). Comparison of detectability and discriminability of tones of different durations under homophasic and antiphasic conditions: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Blegvad, B. (1972). Contralateral masking effect after transsection of the efferent fibers to the cochlea: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 1(3) Sep 1972, 115-118.
  • Bocca, E., & Antonelli, A. R. (1976). Masking level difference: Another tool for the evaluation of peripheral and cortical defects: Audiology Vol 15(6) Nov-Dec 1976, 480-487.
  • Boezeman, E. H., Kapteyn, T. S., Visser, S. L., & Snel, A. M. (1983). Effect of contralateral and ipsilateral masking of acoustic stimulation on the latencies of auditory evoked potentials from cochlea and brain stem: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 55(6) Jun 1983, 710-713.
  • Bond, R. N., Feldstein, S., & Simpson, S. (1988). Relative and absolute judgments of speech rate from masked and content-standard stimuli: The influence of vocal frequency and intensity: Human Communication Research Vol 14(4) Sum 1988, 548-568.
  • Borg, E., & Zakrisson, J. E. (1974). Stapedius reflex and monaural masking: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 78(3-4) Sep-Oct 1974, 155-161.
  • Borgeat, F., Elie, R., Chaloult, L., & Chabot, R. (1985). Psychophysiological responses to masked auditory stimuli: The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry / La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie Vol 30(1) Feb 1985, 22-27.
  • Borrill, S. J., & Moore, B. C. J. (2002). Evidence that comodulation detection differences depend on within-channel mechanisms: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(1,Pt1) Jan 2002, 309-319.
  • Botte, M.-C. (1995). Auditory attentional bandwidth: Effect of level and frequency range: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(5, Pt 1) Nov 1995, 2475-2485.
  • Botte, M. C., & Chocolle, R. (1979). Auditory fatigue: Recovery at threshold and above threshold: Audiology Vol 18(2) 1979, 125-132.
  • Botte, M.-C., & Chocholle, R. (1979). Auditory fatigue: Results of a few new methods of measurement in man: Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique Vol 76(1) Jan-Mar 1979, 5-21.
  • Boyer, R. W. (1984). Conscious processes in automatic visual and auditory search: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bray, V. H. (1992). Upward spread of masking for pure tones and speech in normal-hearing and cochlear-impaired listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Breebaart, J., van de Par, S., & Kohlrausch, A. (1998). Biaural signal detection with phase-shifted and time-delayed noise maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2079-2083.
  • Bregman, A. S., Ahad, P., Kim, J., & Melnerich, L. (1994). Resetting the pitch-analysis system: I. Effects of rise times of tones in noise backgrounds or of harmonics in a complex tone: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 56(2) Aug 1994, 155-162.
  • Bremond, J.-C. (1978). Acoustic competition between the song of the wren (Troglodytes troglodytes) and the songs of other species: Behaviour Vol 65(1-2) 1978, 89-98.
  • Breznitz, Z. (1997). Enhancing the reading of dyslexic children by reading acceleration and auditory masking: Journal of Educational Psychology Vol 89(1) Mar 1997, 103-113.
  • Broadbent, D. E. (1978). The current state of noise research: Reply to Poulton: Psychological Bulletin Vol 85(5) Sep 1978, 1052-1067.
  • Bronkhorst, A. W., & Plomp, R. (1992). Effect of multiple speechlike maskers on binaural speech recognition in normal and impaired hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(6) Dec 1992, 3132-3139.
  • Brosch, M., Schulz, A., & Scheich, H. (1998). Neuronal mechanisms of auditory backward recognition masking in macaque auditory cortex: Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience Vol 9(11) Aug 1998, 2551-2555.
  • Brown, C. H., & Sinnott, J. M. (1990). The perception of complex acoustic patterns in noise by blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis) and human listeners: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 4(1) 1990, 79-90.
  • Brown, W. S., & Brandt, J. F. (1972). The effect of masking on vocal intensity during vocal and whispered speech: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 12(2) Apr 1972, 157-161.
  • Brungart, D. S. (2001). Evaluation of speech intelligibility with the coordinate response measure: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2276-2279.
  • Brungart, D. S. (2001). Informational and energetic masking effects in the perception of two simultaneous talkers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(3) Mar 2001, 1101-1109.
  • Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, B. D. (2002). The effects of spatial separation in distance on the informational and energetic masking of a nearby speech signal: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(2) Aug 2002, 664-676.
  • Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, B. D. (2002). Within-ear and across-ear interference in a cocktail-party listening task: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(6) Dec 2002, 2985-2995.
  • Brungart, D. S., Simpson, B. D., Ericson, M. A., & Scott, K. R. (2001). Informational and energetic masking effects in the perception of multiple simultaneous talkers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(5,Pt1) Nov 2001, 2527-2538.
  • Brysbaert, M., & Praet, C. (1992). Reading isolated words: No evidence for automatic incorporation of the phonetic code: Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung Vol 54(2) Jun 1992, 91-102.
  • Budohoska, W., & Szymanski, L. (1974). Backward and forward masking between two speech-like sounds: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 34(6) 1974, 723-736.
  • Bull, D., Schneider, B. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1981). The masking of octave-band noise by broad-spectrum noise: A comparison of infant and adult thresholds: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 30(2) Aug 1981, 101-106.
  • Bulygin, I. A., & Kachuro, I. I. (1970). Primary cortical responses as functional characteristics of ascending systems of internal and external analysers: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 20(1) Jan 1970, 115-122.
  • Burkard, R., Trautwein, P., & Salvi, R. (1997). The effects of click level, click rate, and level of background masking noise on the inferior colliculus potential (ICP) in the normal and carboplatin-treated chinchilla: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(6) Dec 1997, 3620-3627.
  • Burkard, R. F. (1984). Far-field electrophysiologic manifestations of auditory masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Burns, E. M. (1984). A comparison of variability among measurements of subjective tinnitus and objective stimuli: Audiology Vol 23(4) Jul-Aug 1984, 426-440.
  • Buschow, K. L. (1973). Signal frequency and interaural phase in binaural detection: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Buss, E. (1997). Three studies of comodulation masking release. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Buss, E., Hall, J. W., III, & Grose, J. H. (1998). Change in envelope beats as a possible cue in comodulation masking release (CMR): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(3) Mar 1998, 1592-1597.
  • Buss, E., Hall, J. W., III, Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (1999). Development of adult-like performance in backward, simultaneous, and forward masking: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 42(4) Aug 1999, 844-849.
  • Buss, E., & Richards, V. M. (1996). CMR and factors influencing grouping: Masker bands overlapping in frequency: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(3) Sep 1996, 1897-1900.
  • Buss, E., & Richards, V. M. (1996). The effects on comodulation masking release of systematic variations in on- and off-frequency masker modulation patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(5) May 1996, 3109-3118.
  • Buss, E., Wall, J. W., III, & Grose, J. H. (2003). Effect of amplitude modulation coherence for masked speech signals filtered into narrow bands: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(1) Jan 2003, 462-467.
  • Butts, F. M. (1986). Backward and forward masking in the elderly: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Buus, S. (1999). Temporal integration and multiple looks, revisited: Weights as a function of time: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(4) Apr 1999, 2466-2475.
  • Buus, S., Musch, H., & Florentine, M. (1998). On loudness at threshold: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(1) Jul 1998, 399-410.
  • Buus, S., Zhang, L., & Florentine, M. (1996). Stimulus-driven, time-varying weights for comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(4, Pt 1) Apr 1996, 2288-2297.
  • Campbell, E. M. (1974). Backward auditory masking and sensory storage in normal and retarded subjects: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Campbell, E. M., & Meyer, P. A. (1981). Evaluation of auditory sensory memory of mentally retarded and nonretarded persons: American Journal of Mental Deficiency Vol 86(1) Jul 1981, 50-59.
  • Campbell, R. A. (1964). Masker level and noise-signal detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 36(3) 1964, 570-575.
  • Carhart, R., Tillman, T. W., & Johnson, K. R. (1966). Binaural masking of speech by periodically modulated noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 39(6) 1966, 1037-1050.
  • Carlile, S., & Wardman, D. (1996). Masking produced by broadband noise presented in virtual auditory space: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(6) Dec 1996, 3761-3768.
  • Carlyon, R. P. (1996). Encoding the fundamental frequency of a complex tone in the presence of a spectrally overlapping masker: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(1) Jan 1996, 517-524.
  • Carlyon, R. P. (1996). Masker asynchrony impairs the fundamental-frequency discrimination of unresolved harmonics: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(1) Jan 1996, 525-533.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & Beveridge, H. A. (1993). Effects of forward masking on intensity discrimination, frequency discrimination, and the detection of tones in noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(5) May 1993, 2886-2895.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & Datta, A. J. (1997). Excitation produced by Schroeder-phase complexes: Evidence for fast-acting compression in the auditory system: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(6) Jun 1997, 3636-3647.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & Datta, A. J. (1997). Masking period patterns of Schroeder-phase complexes: Effects of level, number of components, and phase of flanking components: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(6) Jun 1997, 3648-3657.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & Deeks, J. M. (2002). Limitations on rate discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(3,Pt 1) Sep 2002, 1009-1025.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & Sloan, E. P. (1987). The "overshoot" effect and sensory hearing impairment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(3) Sep 1987, 1078-1081.
  • Carlyon, R. P., & White, L. J. (1992). Effect of signal frequency and masker level on the frequency regions responsible for the overshoot effect: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1034-1041.
  • Carney, A. E. (1978). An analysis of psychophysical tuning curves in normal and pathological ears: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Carylon, R. P. (1996). Spread of excitation produced by maskers with damped and ramped envelopes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(6) Jun 1996, 3647-3655.
  • Casali, J. G., Robinson, G. S., Dabney, E. C., & Gauger, D. (2004). Effect of Electronic ANR and Conventional Hearing Protectors on Vehicle Backup Alarm Detection in Noise: Human Factors Vol 46(1) Spr 2004, 1-10.
  • Cefaratti, L. K., & Zwislocki, J. J. (1994). Relationships between the variability of magnitude matching and the slope of magnitude level functions: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(1) Jul 1994, 126-133.
  • Cegalis, J. A., & Murdza, S. (1976). Changes of auditory word discrimination induced by inversion of the visual field: Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol 85(3) Jun 1976, 318-323.
  • Cervellera, G., Quaranta, A., & Amoroso, C. (1980). Clinical experience with remote masking: Audiology Vol 19(5) Sep-Oct 1980, 404-410.
  • Cervellera, G., Quaranta, A., & Cassano, P. (1978). Remote masking as a test of cochlear conductive hearing losses: Audiology Vol 17(4) Jul-Aug 1978, 317-323.
  • Chafin, P., & Peipher, R. A. (1979). Simulated hearing loss: An aid to in-service education: Mental Retardation Vol 17(5) Oct 1979, 255-256.
  • Chait, M., Eden, G., Poeppel, D., Simon, J. Z., Hill, D. F., & Flowers, D. L. (2007). Delayed detection of tonal targets in background noise in dyslexia: Brain and Language Vol 102(1) Jul 2007, 80-90.
  • Champlin, C. A., & Wright, B. A. (1993). Manipulations of the duration and relative onsets of two-tone forward maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(3, Pt 1) Sep 1993, 1269-1274.
  • Chatterjee, M. (1999). Temporal mechanisms underlying recovery from forward masking in multielectrode-implant listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(3) Mar 1999, 1853-1863.
  • Chatterjee, M. (2003). Modulation masking in cochlear implant listeners: Envelope versus tonotopic components: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(4,Pt 1) Apr 2003, 2042-2053.
  • Chatterjee, M., & Shannon, R. V. (1998). Forward masked excitation patterns in multielectrode electrical stimulation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(5, Pt 1) May 1998, 2565-2572.
  • Chen, X. (1995). Monaural echo suppression factors influencing detection of a delayed signal. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences.
  • Chermak, G. D. (1975). Intelligibility of monosyllabic words masked by linguistic strings: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 41(2) Oct 1975, 587-593.
  • Chermak, G. D. (1975). Simultaneous monotic masking as a function of linguistic violations: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Chermak, G. D. (1977). Simultaneous monotic masking as a function of linguistic violations: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 44(1) Feb 1977, 195-202.
  • Chermak, G. D., Dengerink, J. E., & Dengerink, H. A. (1984). Threshold of octave masking as a predictor of temporary threshold shift following repeated noise exposure: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders Vol 49(3) Aug 1984, 303-308.
  • Chocholle, R. (1972). New approaches in the understanding of the effect of homolateral auditory masking: Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique Apr 1972, 133-154.
  • Chocholle, R. (1975). The effects of masking, both total and partial, on sounds of short duration by homolateral sounds of long duration: Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique Vol 72(1) Jan-Mar 1975, 5-22.
  • Chocholle, R., & et al. (1975). Psychoacoustic study of a case of "cortical deafness." Neuropsychologia Vol 13(2) Apr 1975, 163-172.
  • Chocholle, R., & Saulnier, C. (1965). Action of an effect of unilateral masking on the binaural threshold: Psychologie Francaise 10(4) 1965, 336-343.
  • Chung, D. Y. (1977). Masking by combination tones and bands: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 22(3) Sep 1977, 282-286.
  • Chung, D. Y. (1981). Masking, temporal integration, and sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(4) Dec 1981, 514-520.
  • Chung, D. Y. (1981). Tone-on-tone masking in subjects with normal hearing and with sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(4) Dec 1981, 506-513.
  • Coats, A. C., & Dickey, J. R. (1972). Postmasking recovery of human click action potentials and click loudness: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 52(6, Pt 2) Dec 1972, 1607-1612.
  • Cobb, F. E. (1985). Backward masking: An analysis of age, frequency, interstimulus interval and target threshold: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Cobb, F. E. (1995). "Age-associated degeneration of backward masking task performance: Evidence of declining temporal resolution abilities in normal listeners": Commentary: Audiology Vol 34(3) May-Jun 1995, 160.
  • Cobb, F. E., Jacobson, G. P., Newman, C. W., Kretschmer, L. W., & et al. (1993). Age-associated degeneration of backward masking task performance: Evidence of declining temporal resolution abilities in normal listeners: Audiology Vol 32(4) Jul-Aug 1993, 260-271.
  • Code, C., & Muller, D. (1980). Comments on paper: The long-term use of an automatically triggered masking device in the treatment of stammering: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 15(2) Sep 1980, 141-142.
  • Cohen, L. T., Lenarz, T., Battmer, R.-D., von Saebelkampf, C. B., Busby, P. A., & Cowan, R. S. C. (2005). A psychophysical forward masking comparison of longitudinal spread of neural excitation in the ContourTM and straight NucleusReg. electrode arrays: International Journal of Audiology Vol 44(10) Oct 2005, 559-566.
  • Cohen, M. F. (1975). The functional significance of the binaural masking-level difference: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Cohen, M. F. (1991). Comodulation masking release over a three octave range: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(3) Sep 1991, 1381-1384.
  • Cohen, M. F., & Schubert, E. D. (1991). Comodulation masking release and the masking-level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(6) Jun 1991, 3007-3008.
  • Cohen, R. L., & Keith, R. W. (1976). Use of low-pass noise in word-recognition testing: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 19(1) Mar 1976, 48-54.
  • Cokely, C. G., & Humes, L. E. (1993). Two experiments on the temporal boundaries for the nonlinear additivity of masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(5) Nov 1993, 2553-2559.
  • Cokely, C. L. G. (1994). The additivity of auditory masking. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Colcord, R. D., Loren, C. A., & Rastatter, M. P. (1986). Effects of auditory masking by white noise on primary stressing during spontaneous speech: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 63(3) Dec 1986, 1217-1218.
  • Colle, H. A., & Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory: Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior Vol 15(1) Feb 1976, 17-31.
  • Conijn, E. A., Brocaar, M. P., & Van Zanten, G. A. (1990). Monaural versus binaural auditory brainstem response threshold to clicks masked by high-pass noise in normal-hearing subjects: Audiology Vol 29(1) Jan-Feb 1990, 29-35.
  • Connington, M. C. (1999). Masking overshoot: Effects of ipsilateral, bilateral and contralateral priming. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Connolly, J. F. (1993). The influence of stimulus intensity, contralateral masking and handedness on the temporal N1 and the T complex components of the auditory N1 wave: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 86(1) Jan 1993, 58-68.
  • Connolly, J. F. (1994). "The influence of stimulus intensity, contralateral masking and handedness on the temporal N1 and the T complex components of the auditory N1 wave, John F. Connolly": Reply: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 91(1) Jul 1994, 74-76.
  • Conraux, C., Dauman, R., & Feblot, P. (1981). Early "derived" auditory-evoked responses: Audiology Vol 20(5) 1981, 382-393.
  • Conture, E. G., & Wingate, M. E. (1973). Comment on "Effect on stuttering of changes in audition." Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 16(4) Dec 1973, 753-754.
  • Cook, J. A., Bacon, S. P., & Sammeth, C. A. (1997). Effect of low-frequency gain reduction on speech recognition and its relation to upward spread of masking: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 40(2) Apr 1997, 410-422.
  • Coombs, S., & Fay, R. R. (1989). The temporal evolution of masking and frequency selectivity in the goldfish (Carasius auratus): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(3) Sep 1989, 925-933.
  • Cornelius, P. T. (1977). The effects of meatal noise on the interaural correlation of noise maskers: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Corthals, P., Vinck, B., De Vel, E., & Van Cauwenberge, P. (1998). Masking effects and tinnitus as explanatory variables in hearing disability: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 27(1) 1998, 31-36.
  • Costermans, J. (1964). Speech intelligibility in different bands of white noise: Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique No 2 1964, 157-172.
  • Cowan, N., Suomi, K., & Morse, P. A. (1982). Echoic storage in infant perception: Child Development Vol 53(4) Aug 1982, 984-990.
  • Cranford, J. L. (1975). Auditory masking-level differences in the cat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 89(3) May 1975, 219-223.
  • Cranford, J. L. (1975). Role of neocortex in binaural hearing in the cat: I. Contralateral masking: Brain Research Vol 100(2) 1975, 395-406.
  • Cranford, J. L., Stramler, J., & Igarashi, M. (1978). Role of neocortex in binaural hearing in the cat: III. Binaural masking-level differences: Brain Research Vol 151(2) Aug 1978, 381-385.
  • Crawley, E. J. (2000). Demonstrating "overshoot" in the gap detection paradigm: Breaking down a paradigmatic barrier. (auditory perception). Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Crawley, E. J., Kallman, H. J., & Neely, J. H. (1994). A test of an interruption/temporal-uncertainty theory of auditory backward recognition masking of target duration: Acta Psychologica Vol 87(1) Oct 1994, 1-18.
  • Crowder, R. G. (1978). Mechanisms of auditory backward masking in the stimulus suffix effect: Psychological Review Vol 85(6) Nov 1978, 502-524.
  • Crowder, R. G. (1982). Disinhibition of masking in auditory sensory memory: Memory & Cognition Vol 10(5) Sep 1982, 424-433.
  • Cudahy, E. A. (1975). Residue pitch for monaural and dichotic two-tone complexes: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Culling, J. F. (2000). Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking: III. The existence region of the Fourcin pitch: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(4) Apr 2000, 2201-2208.
  • Cunningham, M., & Posey, T. B. (1974). Shock tolerance in rats as a function of white noise: Psychological Reports Vol 34(3, Pt 1) Jun 1974, 711-713.
  • Dai, H., & Buus, S. (1991). Effect of gating the masker on frequency-selective listening: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(4, Pt 1) Apr 1991, 1816-1818.
  • Dai, H., Scharf, B., & Buus, S. (1991). Effective attenuation of signals in noise under focused attention: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(6) Jun 1991, 2837-2842.
  • Danaher, E. M., & Pickett, J. M. (1975). Some masking effects produced by low-frequency vowel formants in persons with sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 18(2) Jun 1975, 261-271.
  • Danaher, E. M., Wilson, M. P., & Pickett, J. M. (1978). Backward and forward masking in listeners with severe sensorineural hearing loss: Audiology Vol 17(4) Jul-Aug 1978, 324-338.
  • Dau, T., Puschel, D., & Kohlrausch, A. (1996). A quantitative model of the "effective" signal processing in the auditory system: II. Simulations and measurements: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(6) Jun 1996, 3623-3631.
  • Davidson, S. A. (2007). Detection of tones in reproducible noise: Psychophysical and computational studies of stimulus features and processing mechanisms. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Davidson, S. A., & Melnick, W. (1988). A clinically feasible method for determining frequency resolution: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 31(2) Jun 1988, 299-303.
  • Day, H. M. (1985). The use of earplugs to reduce shrieking in a profoundly retarded child: Education & Treatment of Children Vol 8(2) Spr 1985, 107-114.
  • de Boer, E., & Bouwmeester, J. (1974). Critical bands and sensorineural hearing loss: Audiology Vol 13(3) May 1974, 236-259.
  • De Gennaro, S. V., & Braida, L. D. (1997). Effects of linear amplification and amplitude compression on sentence reception by listeners with hearing loss simulated by masking noise. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • de Laat, J. A. P. M., & Plomp, R. (1986). Masking of short noise probes by frequency sweeps for hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • De Pascalis, V., Varriale, V., & Matteoli, A. (2008). Intelligence and P3 components of the event-related potential elicited during an auditory discrimination task with masking: Intelligence Vol 36(1) Jan 2008, 35-47.
  • Dean, M. R., & McDermott, H. J. (2000). Preferred listening levels: The effect of background noise for moderate-to-profoundly hearing impaired aid users: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 29(3) 2000, 139-149.
  • Delahaye, R., Fantini, D. A., & Meddis, R. (2001). Effects of flanking component spectral position and modulation pattern on thresholds for signals presented in the peaks of a modulated tonal masker: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(1) Jan 2001, 426-429.
  • Delgutte, B. (1990). Physiological mechanisms of psychophysical masking: Observations from auditory-nerve fibers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(2) Feb 1990, 791-809.
  • Demany, L., & Semal, C. (1990). The effect of vibrato on the recognition of masked vowels: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 48(5) Nov 1990, 436-444.
  • DeMarco, S. (1981). An investigation into the phonetic auditory processing time of children with histories of chronic otitis media: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Dempsey, J. J. (1983). Factors affecting identification and discrimination of synthetic speech stimuli by normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Dent, M. L., Larsen, O. N., & Dooling, R. J. (1997). Free-field binaural unmasking in budgerigars ( Melopsittacus undulatus ): Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 111(3) Jun 1997, 590-598.
  • Derleth, R. P., & Dau, T. (2000). On the role of envelope fluctuation processing in spectral masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(1) Jul 2000, 285-296.
  • Deutsch, C. P., & Zawel, D. (1966). Comparison of visual and auditory perceptual functions of brain-injured and normal children: Perceptual and Motor Skills 22(1) 1966, 303-309.
  • Dewar, A., Dewar, A. D., & Anthony, J. F. (1976). The effect of auditory feedback masking on concomitant movements of stammering: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 11(2) Oct 1976, 95-102.
  • Dewar, A., Dewar, A. D., & Barnes, H. E. (1976). Automatic triggering of auditory feedback masking in stammering and cluttering: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 11(1) Apr 1976, 19-26.
  • Dillon, H. A. (1981). The perception of musical transients: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Dirks, D., & Malmquist, C. (1964). Changes in bone-conduction thresholds produced by masking in the non-test ear: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 7(3) 1964, 271-278.
  • Dirks, D. D., & Malmquist, C. (1965). Shifts in air-conduction thresholds produced by pulsed and continuous contralateral masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 37(4) 1965, 631-637.
  • Dirks, D. D., & Norris, J. C. (1966). Shifts in auditory thresholds produced by ipsilateral and contralateral maskers at low-intensity levels: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 40(1) 1966, 12-19.
  • Dmitrieva, E. S., Gel'man, V. Y., & Zaitseva, K. A. (2000). Perception of the emotional component of speech by stuttering children against the background of noise: I. Analysis of the efficiency of the identification of various emotions: Human Physiology Vol 26(3) May-Jun 2000, 258-264.
  • Dobie, R. A., & Wilson, M. J. (1985). Binaural interaction in auditory brain-stem responses: Effects of masking: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials Vol 62(1) Jan 1985, 56-64.
  • Docherty, E. M. (1973). The effects of reducing and masking the auditory cues accompanying performance of select gross motor tasks on the performance of those tasks: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Dodd, F. (1991). A neuroethological study of sound communication in a reptile (Gekko gecko): Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Doherty, K. A., & Lutfi, R. A. (1999). Level discrimination of single tones in a multitone complex by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(3) Mar 1999, 1831-1840.
  • Doll, T. J., & Hanna, T. E. (1995). Spatial and spectral release from masking in three-dimensional auditory displays: Human Factors Vol 37(2) Jun 1995, 341-355.
  • Doll, T. J., Hanna, T. E., & Russotti, J. S. (1992). Masking in three-dimensional auditory displays: Human Factors Vol 34(3) Jun 1992, 255-265.
  • Donovan, G. E. (1971). A new device for the treatment of stammering: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 6(1) Apr 1971, 86-88.
  • Dooling, R. J., Okanoya, K., Downing, J., & Hulse, S. (1986). Hearing in the starling (Sturnus vulgaris): Absolute thresholds and critical ratios: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 24(6) Nov 1986, 462-464.
  • Dooling, R. J., & Searcy, M. H. (1985). Nonsimultaneous auditory masking in the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 99(2) Jun 1985, 226-230.
  • Dorman, M. F., Kewley-Port, D., Brady, S., & Turvey, M. T. (1977). Vowel recognition: Inferences from studies of forward and backward masking: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 29(3) Aug 1977, 483-497.
  • Dorman, M. F., Lindholm, J. M., & Hannley, M. T. (1985). Influence of the first formant on the recognition of voiced stop consonants by hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 28(3) Sep 1985, 377-380.
  • Dreschler, W. A., & Festen, J. M. (1986). The effect of hearing impairment on auditory filter shapes in simultaneous and forward masking. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Dreschler, W. A., & Leeuw, A. R. (1990). Speech reception in reverberation related to temporal resolution: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 33(1) Mar 1990, 181-187.
  • Drullman, R. (1995). Speech intelligibility in noise: Relative contribution of speech elements above and below the noise level: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(3) Sep 1995, 1796-1798.
  • Duan, M. L., & Canlon, B. (1996). Differences in forward masking after a temporary and a permanent noise-induced hearing loss: Audiology & Neurotology Vol 1(6) Nov-Dec 1996, 328-338.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (1995). Growth of low-pass masking of pure tones and speech for hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(6) Dec 1995, 3113-3124.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (1995). Masked thresholds and consonant recognition in low-pass maskers for hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(4) Apr 1995, 2430-2441.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (1997). Additivity of multiple maskers of speech. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (2001). Forward- and simultaneous-masked thresholds in bandlimited maskers in subjects with normal hearing and cochlear hearing loss: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(2) Aug 2001, 1049-1057.
  • Dubno, J. R., Ahlstrom, J. B., & Horwitz, A. R. (2002). Spectral contributions to the benefit from spatial separation of speech and noise: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 45(6) Dec 2002, 1297-1310.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Dorman, M. F. (1987). Effects of spectral flattening on vowel identification: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1503-1511.
  • Dubno, J. R., Horwitz, A. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (2003). Recovery from prior stimulation: Masking of speech by interrupted noise for younger and older adults with normal hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(4,Pt 1) Apr 2003, 2084-2094.
  • Dubno, J. R., & Schaefer, A. B. (1995). Frequency selectivity and consonant recognition for hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners with equivalent masked thresholds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(2) Feb 1995, 1165-1174.
  • Duifhuis, H. (1973). Consequences of peripheral frequency selectivity for nonsimultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(6) Dec 1973, 1471-1488.
  • Dunn, B. E., Posey, M. H., & Drummond, L. (1980). Central masking with interaural asymmetry: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 20(2) Apr 1980, 81-88.
  • Durlach, N. I. (1964). Note on binaural masking-level differences at high frequencies: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 36(3) 1964, 576-581.
  • Durlach, N. I., Mason, C. R., Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., Arbogast, T. L., Colburn, H. S., & Kidd, G., Jr. (2003). Informational masking: Counteracting the effects of stimulus uncertainty by decreasing target-masker similarity: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 368-379.
  • Durrant, J. D., Nozza, R. J., Hyre, R. J., & Sabo, D. L. (1989). Masking level difference at relatively high masker levels: Preliminary report: Audiology Vol 28(4) Jul-Aug 1989, 221-229.
  • Eddins, D. A. (2001). Measurement of auditory temporal processing using modified masking period patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(4) Apr 2001, 1550-1558.
  • Eddins, D. A. (2001). Monaural masking release in random-phase and low-noise noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(4) Apr 2001, 1538-1549.
  • Eddins, D. A., & Barber, L. E. (1998). The influence of stimulus envelope and fine structure on the binaural masking level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(5, Pt 1) May 1998, 2578-2589.
  • Eddins, D. A., Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1992). The detection of temporal gaps as a function of frequency region and absolute noise bandwidth: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1069-1077.
  • Eddins, D. A., & Wright, B. A. (1994). Comodulation masking release for single and multiple rates of envelope fluctuation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(6) Dec 1994, 3432-3442.
  • Egan, J. P. (1965). Masking-level differences as a function of interaural disparities in intensity of signal and of noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38(6) 1965, 1043-1049.
  • Egan, J. P. (1974). Auditory masking and signal detection theory. Oxford, England: D Reidel.
  • Eggermont, J. J. (1974). The temperature dependency of cochlear adaptation and masking in the guinea pig: Audiology Vol 13(2) Mar 1974, 147-161.
  • Eggermont, J. J., & Spoor, A. (1973). Masking of action potentials in the guinea pig cochlea: Its relation to adaptation: Audiology Vol 12(4) Jul 1973, 221-241.
  • Egorova, M., Vartanyan, I., & Ehret, G. (2006). Frequency response areas of mouse inferior colliculus neurons: II. Critical bands: Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience Vol 17(17) Nov 2006, 1783-1786.
  • Ehret, G. (1979). Correlations between cochlear hair cell loss and shifts of masked and absolute behavioral auditory thresholds in the house mouse: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 87(1-2) Jan-Feb 1979, 28-38.
  • Eisenberg, L. S., Dirks, D. D., & Bell, T. S. (1995). Speech recognition in amplitude-modulated noise of listeners with normal and listeners with impaired hearing: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 38(1) Feb 1995, 222-233.
  • Ellermeier, W., & Hellbruck, J. (1998). Is level irrelevant in "irrelevant speech"? Effects of loudness, signal-to-noise ratio, and binaural unmasking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 24(5) Oct 1998, 1406-1414.
  • Elliott, E. M., & Cowan, N. (2001). Habituation to auditory distractors in a cross-modal, color-word interference task: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 27(3) May 2001, 654-667.
  • Elliott, L. L. (1965). Changes in the simultaneous masked threshold of brief tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38(5) 1965, 738-746.
  • Elliott, L. L. (1975). Temporal and masking phenomena in persons with sensorineural hearing loss: Audiology Vol 14(4) 1975, 336-353.
  • Elliott, T. F., & Wilbanks, W. A. (1980). Lateralization of tonal signals in noise: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 15(1) Jan 1980, 37-40.
  • Emmerich, D. S., Fantini, D. A., & Ellermeier, W. (1989). An investigation of the facilitation of simple auditory reaction time by predictable background stimuli: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 45(1) Jan 1989, 66-70.
  • Emmerich, D. S., Pitchford, L. J., & Becker, C. A. (1976). Reaction time to tones in tonal backgrounds and a comparison of reaction time to signal onset and offset: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 20(3) Sep 1976, 210-214.
  • Erbe, C. (2000). Detection of whale calls in noise: Performance comparison between a beluga whale, human listeners, and a neural network: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(1) Jul 2000, 297-303.
  • Erdreich, J. (1977). Subjective tones: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Ericson, M. A., & McKinley, R. L. (1997). The intelligibility of multiple talkers separated spatially in noise. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Ermey, H. L. (1974). Duration effects in homophasic and antiphasic tonal detection: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Evans, E. F., & Elberling, C. (1982). Location-specific components of the gross cochlear action potential: An assessment of the validity of the high-pass masking technique by cochlear nerve fibre recording in the cat: Audiology Vol 21(3) May-Jun 1982, 204-227.
  • Evilsizer, M. E., Gilkey, R. H., Mason, C. R., Colburn, H. S., & Carney, L. H. (2002). Binaural detection with narrowband and wideband reproducible noise maskers: I. Results for human: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(1,Pt1) Jan 2002, 336-345.
  • Ewert, S. D., Verhey, J. L., & Dau, T. (2002). Spectro-temporal processing in the envelope-frequency domain: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(6) Dec 2002, 2921-2931.
  • Fabry, D. A., & Van Tasell, D. J. (1986). Masked and filtered simulation of hearing loss: Effects on consonant recognition: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 29(2) Jun 1986, 170-178.
  • Fallon, M., Trehub, S. E., & Schneider, B. A. (2002). Children's use of semantic cues in degraded listening environments: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(5,Pt1) May 2002, 2242-2249.
  • Fankhauser, C. E. (1974). An analysis of the perceptual confusions of normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners of speech masked by white noise: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fantini, D. A. (1986). Edge effects on frequency discrimination of tones in noise backgrounds: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fantini, D. A. (1989). Frequency discrimination near the spectral edge of simultaneous and forward maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(4) Apr 1989, 1691-1698.
  • Fantini, D. A. (1991). The processing of envelope information in comodulation masking release (CMR) and envelope discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(4, Pt 1) Oct 1991, 1876-1888.
  • Fantini, D. A., & Emmerich, D. S. (1987). Edge effects on frequency discrimination of tones presented in low- and high-pass noise backgrounds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1593-1597.
  • Fantini, D. A., Moore, B. C., & Schooneveldt, G. P. (1993). Comodulation masking release as a function of type of signal, gated or continuous masking, monaural or dichotic presentation of flanking bands, and center frequency: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(4, Pt 1) Apr 1993, 2106-2115.
  • Fantini, D. A., & Moore, B. C. J. (1994). A comparison of the effectiveness of across-channel cues available in comodulation masking release and profile analysis tasks: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(6) Dec 1994, 3451-3462.
  • Farmer, A., & Green, W. B. (1978). Differential effect of auditory masking on cerebral palsied speakers: Journal of Phonetics Vol 6(2) Apr 1978, 127-132.
  • Fastl, H. (1975). Pulsation patterns of sinusoids vs. critical band noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 18(2) Aug 1975, 95-97.
  • Fastl, H. (1976). Influence of test tone duration on auditory masking patterns: Audiology Vol 15(1) 1976, 63-71.
  • Fastl, H. (1977). Simulation of a hearing loss at long versus short test tones: Audiology Vol 16(2) Mar-Apr 1977, 102-109.
  • Fastl, H., & Schorer, E. (1986). Critical bandwidth at low frequencies reconsidered. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Faure, P. A., Fremouw, T., Casseday, J. H., & Covey, E. (2003). Temporal masking reveals properties of sound-evoked inhibition in duration-tuned neurons of the inferior colliculus: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 23(7) Apr 2003, 3052-3065.
  • Fay, R. R. (1974). Masking of tones by noise for the goldfish (Carassius auratus): Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(4) Oct 1974, 708-716.
  • Ferrand, C. T. (2006). Relationship Between Masking Levels and Phonatory Stability in Normal-Speaking Women: Journal of Voice Vol 20(2) Jun 2006, 223-228.
  • Festen, J. M. (1993). Contributions of comodulation masking release and temporal resolution to the speech-reception threshold masked by an interfering voice: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(3, Pt 1) Sep 1993, 1295-1300.
  • Feth, L. L., & O'Malley, H. (1977). Influence of temporal masking on click-pair discriminability: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 22(5) Nov 1977, 497-505.
  • Finck, A. (1966). Physiological correlate of tonal masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 39(6) 1966, 1056-1062.
  • Findlay, R. C., & Schuchman, G. I. (1976). Masking level difference for speech: Effects of ear dominance and age: Audiology Vol 15(3) 1976, 232-241.
  • Finneran, J. J., Schlundt, C. E., Carder, D. A., & Ridgway, S. H. (2002). Auditory filter shapes for the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the white whale (Delphinapterus leucas) derived with notched noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(1) Jul 2002, 322-328.
  • Fisiloglu, A. G. (1992). The effects of contralateral noise on the perception of puretones and speech: A study of children with auditory processing difficulties and normal controls: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fitch, J. L. (1974). Effects of auditory masking on oral sensory perception: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 39(2) Oct 1974, 999-1001.
  • Fleet, G. J. (1987). Evidence of temporal coding in a forward masking task: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Florentine, M. (1992). Effects of cochlear impairment and equivalent-threshold masking on psychoacoustic tuning curves: Audiology Vol 31(5) Sep-Oct 1992, 241-253.
  • Florentine, M., & Buus, S. (1995). Masked level discrimination: Deterring profile listening: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(4) Apr 1995, 2460-2467.
  • Florentine, M., Buus, S., Scharf, B., & Zwicker, E. (1980). Frequency selectivity in normally-hearing and hearing-impaired observers: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 23(3) Sep 1980, 646-669.
  • Flynn, M. C., & Dowell, R. C. (2003). Effects of Background Noise and Reverberation on the Aided Speech Perception in Adults with a Severe or Severe-to-profound Hearing Impairment: Australian and New Zealand Journal of Audiology Vol 25(2) Nov 2003, 63-73.
  • Formby, C., & Gjerdingen, D. B. (1980). Pure-tone masking of tinnitus: Audiology Vol 19(6) Nov-Dec 1980, 519-535.
  • Formby, C., Heinz, M. G., Luna, C. E., & Shaheen, M. K. (1994). Masked detection thresholds and temporal integration for noise band signals: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(1) Jul 1994, 102-114.
  • Formby, C., Sherlock, L. P., & Ferguson, S. H. (2000). Enhancement of the edges of temporal masking functions by complex patterns of overshoot and undershoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(4) Apr 2000, 2169-2187.
  • Forrest, K., Abbas, P. J., & Zimmermann, G. N. (1986). Effects of white noise masking and low pass filtering on speech kinematics: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 29(4) Dec 1986, 549-562.
  • Forrest, K. M. (1984). The effects of white noise masking and low pass filtering on speech kinematics: A cinefluorographic analysis: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fortune, T. W. (1990). Envelope effects in simultaneous masking: The influence of masker temporal fluctuations on the shapes of high level psychophysical tuning curves: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fowler, C. G., & Leonards, J. S. (1985). Frequency dependence of the binaural interaction component of the auditory brainstem response: Audiology Vol 24(6) Nov-Dec 1985, 420-429.
  • Fowler, C. G., & Mikami, C. M. (1992). Effects of noise bandwidth on the late-potential masking level difference: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials Vol 84(2) Mar-Apr 1992, 157-163.
  • Foyle, D. C., & Watson, C. S. (1984). Stimulus-based versus performance-based measurement of auditory backward recognition masking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 36(6) Dec 1984, 515-522.
  • Frank, A. S. (1980). Detection of diotic and dichotic tones masked by repeatable samples of noise: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Frank, T., & Karlovich, R. S. (1975). Effect of contralateral noise on speech detection and speech reception thresholds: Audiology Vol 14(1) Jan-Feb 1975, 34-43.
  • Frankish, C., & Turner, J. (1984). Delayed suffix effect at very short delays: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 10(4) Oct 1984, 767-777.
  • Freda, J. S. (1979). Temporal integration and forward masking in auditory information processing: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Frederickson, C. J., & Gerken, G. M. (1977). Masking of electrical by acoustic stimuli: Behavioral evidence for tonotopic organization: Science Vol 198(4323) Dec 1977, 1276-1278.
  • Frederickson, C. J., & Gerken, G. M. (1978). Functional characteristics of cochlear nucleus in behaving cat examined by acoustic masking of electrical stimuli: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 41(6) Nov 1978, 1535-1545.
  • Freigang, B., von Specht, H., & Oeken, F. W. (1974). The use of masking in evoked response audiometry: Audiology Vol 13(4) Jul 1974, 349-355.
  • Freyman, R. L., Balakrishnan, U., & Helfer, K. S. (2001). Spatial release from informational masking in speech recognition: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2112-2122.
  • Freyman, R. L., Helfer, K. S., McCall, D. D., & Clifton, R. K. (1999). The role of perceived spatial separation in the unmasking of speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(6) Dec 1999, 3578-3588.
  • Freyman, R. L., McCall, D. D., & Clifton, R. K. (1998). Intensity discrimination for precedence effect stimuli: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2031-2041.
  • Friend, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1994). A comparison of content-masking procedures for obtaining judgments of discrete affective states: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(3) Sep 1994, 1283-1290.
  • Fucci, D., Petrosino, L., & Robey, R. R. (1982). Auditory masking effects on lingual vibrotactile thresholds as a function of age: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 54(3, Pt 1) Jun 1982, 943-950.
  • Fucci, D., Petrosino, L., & Sloane, N. (1982). Auditory masking effects on lingual vibrotactile thresholds: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 54(1) Feb 1982, 251-256.
  • Fuente, A., & McPherson, B. (2006). Auditory processing tests for Spanish-speaking adults: An initial study: International Journal of Audiology Vol 45(11) Nov 2006, 645-659.
  • Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Glover, G. H. (2007). Assessing the influence of scanner background noise on auditory processing: II. An fMRI study comparing auditory processing in the absence and presence of recorded scanner noise using a sparse design: Human Brain Mapping Vol 28(8) Aug 2007, 721-732.
  • Gabriel, K. J., Koehnke, J., & Colburn, H. S. (1992). Frequency dependence of binaural performance in listeners with impaired binaural hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 336-347.
  • Gagne, J.-P. (1988). Excess masking among listeners with a sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(6) Jun 1988, 2311-2321.
  • Galambos, R., & Makeig, S. (1992). Physiological studies of central masking in man: I. The effects of noise on the 40-Hz steady-state response: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(5) Nov 1992, 2683-2690.
  • Galambos, R., & Makeig, S. (1992). Physiological studies of central masking in man: II. Tonepip SSRs and the masking level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(5) Nov 1992, 2691-2697.
  • Gallun, F. J. (2004). The role of stimulus envelope in the detection of brief increments in the intensity of a tone. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Garner, W. R., & Miller, G. A. (1947). The masked threshold of pure tones as a function of duration: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 37(4) Aug 1947, 293-303.
  • Gehr, S. E., & Sommers, M. S. (1999). Age differences in backward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(5) Nov 1999, 2793-2799.
  • Gengel, R. W. (1972). Auditory temporal integration at relatively high masked-threshold levels: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1849-1851.
  • Gerber, S. E. (1972). Interaural phase and the release from masking: Audiology Vol 11(5-6) Sep 1972, 271-276.
  • Gerrard, R. L. (1991). Auditory masking in the rat: Frequency specificity, auditory filter shape, and effects of noise-induced hearing impairment: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Gerritsen, T. A., Racansky, D. O., & Kunov, H. (1996). A source impedance compensated artificial voice for mask speech intelligibility testing: Thevenin model: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(5, Pt 1) Nov 1996, 3408-3416.
  • Gerull, G., Mrowinski, D., & Nubel, K. (1991). Low-frequency masking of brainstem potentials: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 20(4) 1991, 227-234.
  • Gescheider, G. A., Bolanowski, S. J., Zwislocki, J. J., Hal, K. L., & et al. (1994). The effects of masking on the growth of vibrotactile sensation magnitude and on the amplitude difference limen: A test of the equal sensation magnitude-equal difference limen hypotesis: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(3) Sep 1994, 1479-1488.
  • Gescheider, G. A., Verrillo, R. T., & Pelli, D. G. (1992). Effects of noise on detection of amplitude increments of sinusoidal vibration of the skin: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 348-353.
  • Gifford, R. H., & Bacon, S. P. (2000). Contributions of suppression and excitation to simultaneous masking: Effects of signal frequency and masker-signal frequency relation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(4) Apr 2000, 2188-2200.
  • Gifford, R. H., Bacon, S. P., & Williams, E. J. (2007). An examination of speech recognition in a modulated background and of forward masking in younger and older listeners: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 50(4) Aug 2007, 857-864.
  • Gilkey, R. H., & Good, M. D. (1995). Effects of frequency on free-field masking: Human Factors Vol 37(4) Dec 1995, 835-843.
  • Gilkey, R. H., Simpson, B. D., & Weisenberger, J. M. (1990). Masker fringe and binaural detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(3) Sep 1990, 1323-1332.
  • Glasberg, B. R., & Moore, B. C. J. (1994). Growth-of-masking functions for several types of maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(1) Jul 1994, 134-144.
  • Gobsch, H., Kevanishvili, Z., Gamgebeli, Z., & Gvelesiani, T. (1992). Behaviour of delayed evoked otoacoustic emission under forward masking paradigm: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 21(3) 1992, 143-148.
  • Gockel, H., Moore, B. C. J., & Patterson, R. D. (2003). Asymmetry of masking between complex tones and noise: Partial loudness: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 349-360.
  • Gockel, H., Moore, B. C. J., Patterson, R. D., & Meddis, R. (2003). Louder sounds can produce less forward masking: Effects of component phase in complex tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(2) Aug 2003, 978-990.
  • Goff, D. P. (1974). Cross-field visual masking by tachistoscopic presentation of target and noise patterns to opposite cerebral hemispheres: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Goldstein, D. P., & Stephens, S. D. (1975). Masking level difference: A measure of auditory processing capability: Audiology Vol 14(4) 1975, 354-367.
  • Good, M. D., Gilkey, R. H., & Ball, J. M. (1997). The relation between detection in noise and localization in noise in the free field. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Gordon, P. C. (1997). Coherence masking in brief noise complexes: Effects of temporal patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(4) Oct 1997, 2276-2283.
  • Gordon, P. C. (1997). Coherence masking protection in speech sounds: The role of formant synchrony: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 59(2) Feb 1997, 232-242.
  • Gordon, P. C. (2000). Masking protection in the perception of auditory objects: Speech Communication Vol 30(4) Apr 2000, 197-206.
  • Gordon-Salant, S. M. (1981). Effects of speech maskers on speech perception by normal and hearing-impaired listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Grafe, T. U. (1996). The function of call alternation in the African reed frog ( Hyperolius marmoratus ): Precise call timing prevents auditory masking: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Vol 38(3) Mar 1996, 149-158.
  • Grant, K. W. (2001). The effect of speechreading on masked detection thresholds for filtered speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2272-2275.
  • Grantham, D. W. (1976). The role of time-varying cues in monaural and binaural detection of tones: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Grantham, D. W., & Bacon, S. P. (1991). Binaural modulation masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(3) Mar 1991, 1340-1349.
  • Grauer, R. A., & Dunn, B. E. (1980). Central fatigue in a perstimulatory paradigm: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 20(2) Apr 1980, 97-106.
  • Gray, L. (1993). Developmental changes in chickens' masked thresholds: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 26(8) Dec 1993, 447-457.
  • Green, D. M. (1992). On the similarity of two theories of comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(3) Mar 1992, 1769.
  • Green, D. M., & Forrest, T. G. (1989). Temporal gaps in noise and sinusoids: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(3) Sep 1989, 961-970.
  • Gregan, M. J., Bacon, S. P., & Lee, J. (1998). Masking by sinusoidally amplitude-modulated tonal maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(2) Feb 1998, 1012-1021.
  • Griffiths, Y. M., Hill, N. I., Bailey, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2003). Auditory temporal order discrimination and backward recognition masking in adults with dyslexia: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 46(6) Dec 2003, 1352-1366.
  • Grimes, C. T. (1978). The psychophysical tuning curve: A parametric study: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Grimes, C. T., & Wright, H. N. (1982). The influence of probe-tone parameters on the shape of psychophysical tuning curves in normal-hearing listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(3) Sep 1982, 394-401.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W. (1989). Comodulation masking release using SAM tonal complex maskers: Effects of modulation depth and signal position: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(3) Mar 1989, 1276-1284.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W. (1990). The effect of modulation coherence on signal threshold in frequency-modulated noise bands: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(2) Aug 1990, 703-710.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W. (1990). The effect of signal-frequency uncertainty on comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(3) Mar 1990, 1272-1277.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W. (1992). Comodulation masking release for speech stimuli: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1042-1050.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W. (1993). Comodulation masking release: Is comodulation sufficient? : Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(5) May 1993, 2896-2902.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W., III. (1996). Across-frequency processing of multiple modulation patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(1) Jan 1996, 534-541.
  • Grose, J. H., & Hall, J. W., III. (1998). Masker fluctation and the masking-level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(5, Pt 1) May 1998, 2590-2594.
  • Grose, J. H., Hall, J. W., III, & Dev, M. B. (1997). MLD in children: Effects of signal and masker bandwidths: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 40(4) Aug 1997, 955-959.
  • Grose, J. H., Poth, E. A., & Peters, R. W. (1994). Masking level differences for tones and speech in elderly listeners with relatively normal audiograms: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 37(2) Apr 1994, 422-428.
  • Gunji, A., Hoshiyama, M., & Kakigi, R. (2001). Auditory response following vocalization: A magnetoencephalographic study: Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 112(3) Mar 2001, 514-520.
  • Gunn, W. J. (1973). Possible involvement of the acoustic reflex in loudness shifts resulting from a remote masker: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 13(1) Jan 1973, 10-13.
  • Gusev, E. K. (1982). Quantitative characteristic of central acoustic masking: Psikologicheskii Zhurnal Vol 3(3) May-Jun 1982, 70-83.
  • Gusjew, J. K. (1978). Conformity of central acoustic masking: Zeitschrift fur Psychologie mit Zeitschrift fur angewandte Psychologie Vol 186(3) 1978, 311-340.
  • Gustafsson, H. A., & Arlinger, S. D. (1994). Masking of speech by amplitude-modulated noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(1) Jan 1994, 518-529.
  • Guzman, S. J. (1999). Buildup of the precedence effect: Fusion, masking, localization and lateralization of reflections. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Hafter, E. R., Carrier, S. C., & Stephan, F. K. (1973). Direct comparison of lateralization and the MLD for monaural signals in gated noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 53(6) Jun 1973, 1553-1559.
  • Hainsworth, K. R. (1995). Spectral completion: The restoration of speech intelligibility by adding noise to spectral gaps. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Hall, J. L. (1997). Asymmetry of masking revisited: Generalization of masker and probe bandwidth: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(2) Feb 1997, 1023-1033.
  • Hall, J. L., & Lummis, R. C. (1973). Thresholds for click pairs masked by band-stop noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(3) Sep 1973, 593-599.
  • Hall, J. W., III. (1986). "Binaural frequency selectivity" and CMR. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Hall, J. W., Buss, E., Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (2004). Developmental Effects in the Masking-Level Difference: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 47(1) Feb 2004, 13-20.
  • Hall, J. W., Cokely, J. A., & Grose, J. H. (1988). Combined monaural and binaural masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(5) May 1988, 1839-1845.
  • Hall, J. W., Davis, A. C., Haggard, M. P., & Pillsbury, H. C. (1988). Spectro-temporal analysis in normal-hearing and cochlear-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(4) Oct 1988, 1325-1331.
  • Hall, J. W., & Derlacki, E. L. (1988). Binaural hearing after middle ear surgery: Masking-level difference for interaural time and amplitude cues: Audiology Vol 27(2) Mar-Apr 1988, 89-98.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1988). Comodulation masking release: Evidence for multiple cues: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(5) Nov 1988, 1669-1675.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1989). Spectrotemporal analysis and cochlear hearing impairment: Effects of frequency selectivity, temporal resolution, signal frequency, and rate of modulation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(6) Jun 1989, 2550-2562.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1990). Comodulation masking release and auditory grouping: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(1) Jul 1990, 119-125.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1991). Relative contributions of envelope maxima and minima to comodulation masking release: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 43A(3) Aug 1991, 349-372.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1991). Some effects of auditory grouping factors on modulation detection interference (MDI): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(6) Dec 1991, 3028-3035.
  • Hall, J. W., III, & Grose, J. H. (1992). Masking release for gap detection. New York, NY: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1994). The effect of conductive hearing loss on the masking-level difference: Insert versus standard earphones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(5, Pt 1) May 1994, 2652-2657.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1994). Effect of otitis media with effusion on comodulation masking release in children: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 37(6) Dec 1994, 1441-1449.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1994). Signal detection in complex comodulated backgrounds by normal-hearing and cochlear-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(1) Jan 1994, 435-443.
  • Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1995). Amplitude discrimination in masking release paradigms: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(2, Pt 1) Aug 1995, 847-852.
  • Hall, J. W., III, & Grose, J. H. (1997). The relation between gap detection, loudness, and loudness growth in noise-masked normal-hearing listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(2) Feb 1997, 1044-1049.
  • Hall, J. W., III, Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (1997). Auditory development in complex tasks of comodulation masking release: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 40(4) Aug 1997, 946-954.
  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Dev, M. B. (1997). Signal detection and pitch ranking in conditions of masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(3) Sep 1997, 1746-1754.
  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Haggard, M. P. (1988). Comodulation masking release for multicomponent signals: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(2) Feb 1988, 677-686.
  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Haggard, M. P. (1990). Effects of flanking band proximity, number, and modulation pattern on comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(1) Jan 1990, 269-283.
  • Hall, J. W., III, Grose, J. H., & Hartmann, W. M. (1998). The masking-level difference in low-noise noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(5, Pt 1) May 1998, 2573-2577.
  • Hall, J. W., III, Grose, J. H., & Hatch, D. R. (1996). Effects of masker gating for signal detection in unmodulated and modulated bandlimited noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(4, Pt 1) Oct 1996, 2365-2372.
  • Hall, J. W., III, Grose, J. H., & Mendoza, L. (1995). Across-channel processes in masking. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Moore, B. C. J. (1993). Influence of frequency selectivity on comodulation masking release in normal-hearing listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 36(2) Apr 1993, 410-423.
  • Hall, J. W., & Harvey, A. D. (1985). The binaural masking level difference as a function of frequency, masker level and masking bandwidth in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners: Audiology Vol 24(1) Jan-Feb 1985, 25-31.
  • Hall, J. W., & Helfer, K. (1988). The effect of masker interaural phase on the detection of a monaural signal: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(5) May 1988, 1972-1976.
  • Hall, J. W., Tyler, R. S., & Fernandes, M. A. (1984). Factors influencing the masking level difference in cochlear hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 27(1) Mar 1984, 145-154.
  • Hallmo, P., Sundby, A., & Mair, I. W. (1992). High-frequency audiometry: Masking of air- and bone-conduction signals: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 21(2) 1992, 115-121.
  • Halmiova, O., & Sebova, E. (1978). Effect of properties of the nervous system and activation level on recall: Studia Psychologica Vol 20(4) 1978, 305-308.
  • Hammond, T. J., & Bailey, W. J. (2003). Eavesdropping and defensive auditory masking in an Australian bushcricket, Caedicia (Phaneropterinae: Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera): Behaviour Vol 140(1) Jan 2003, 79-95.
  • Hanna, T. E. (1984). Discrimination of reproducible noise as a function of bandwidth and duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 36(5) Nov 1984, 409-416.
  • Hannigan, M. L., Shelton, T. S., Franks, J. J., & Bransford, J. D. (1980). The effects of episodic and semantic memory on the identification of sentences masked by white noise: Memory & Cognition Vol 8(3) May 1980, 278-284.
  • Hant, J. J., & Alwan, A. (2003). A psychoacoustic-masking model to predict the perception of speech-like stimuli in noise: Speech Communication Vol 40(3) May 2003, 291-313.
  • Hant, J. J., Strope, B. P., & Alwan, A. A. (1997). A psychoacoustic model for the noise making of plosive bursts-super(a)): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(5, Pt 1) May 1997, 2789-2802.
  • Hardcastle, W. J. (1975). Some aspects of speech production under controlled conditions of oral anesthesia and auditory masking: Journal of Phonetics Vol 3(4) Oct 1975, 197-214.
  • Hargus, S. E., & Gordon-Salant, S. (1995). Accuracy of Speech Intelligibility Index predictions for noise-masked young listeners with normal hearing and for elderly listeners with hearing impairment: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 38(1) Feb 1995, 234-243.
  • Harris, D. M. (1978). Forward masking and recovery from short-term adaptation in single auditory-nerve fibers: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Harris, J. (1994). Brain lesions, central masking, and dichotic speech perception: Brain and Language Vol 46(1) Jan 1994, 96-108.
  • Harris, J. L., Salus, D., Rerecich, R., & Larsen, D. (1996). Distinguishing detection from identification in subliminal auditory perception: A review and critique of Merikle's study: Journal of General Psychology Vol 123(1) Jan 1996, 41-50.
  • Harris, J. P. (1983). Brain lesions, central masking, and dichotic speech perception: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Harris, R. W., Brey, R. H., Miller, R. W., & Channell, R. W. (1992). Influence of masker bandwidth on binaural masking level differences: Audiology Vol 31(4) Jul-Aug 1992, 196-204.
  • Harrison, J., & Buchwald, J. (1982). Auditory brainstem responses in the aged cat: Neurobiology of Aging Vol 3(3) Fal 1982, 163-171.
  • Hartley, D. E. H., & Moore, D. R. (2002). Auditory processing efficiency deficits in children with developmental language impairments: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(6) Dec 2002, 2962-2966.
  • Hartley, D. E. H., Wright, B. A., Hogan, S. C., & Moore, D. R. (2000). Age-related improvements in auditory backward and simultaneous masking in 6- to 10-year-old children: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 43(6) Dec 2000, 1402-1415.
  • Hartley, L. R. (1981). Noise does not impair by masking: A reply to Poulton's "Composite model for human performance in continuous noise." Psychological Review Vol 88(1) Jan 1981, 86-89.
  • Hartmann, W. M. (1993). Auditory demonstrations on compact disk for large N: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(1) Jan 1993, 1-16.
  • Hartmann, W. M., & Constan, Z. A. (2002). Interaural level differences and the level-meter model: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(3,Pt 1) Sep 2002, 1037-1045.
  • Hartmann, W. M., McAdams, S., & Smith, B. K. (1990). Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(4) Oct 1990, 1712-1724.
  • Hasbrouck, J. M. (1980). Performance of students with auditory figure-ground disorders under conditions of unilateral and bilateral ear occlusion: Journal of Learning Disabilities Vol 13(10) Dec 1980, 548-551.
  • Hatch, D. R., Arne, B. C., & Hall, J. W. (1995). Comodulation masking release (CMR): Effects of gating as a function of number of flanking bands and masker bandwidth: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(6) Jun 1995, 3768-3774.
  • Hawkins, H. L., & et al. (1974). Precategorical selective attention and tonal specificity in auditory recognition: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 103(3) Sep 1974, 530-538.
  • Hayden, P. A., Adams, M. R., & Jordahl, N. (1982). The effects of pacing and masking on stutterers' and nonstutterers' speech initiation times: Journal of Fluency Disorders Vol 7(1-1) Mar 1982, 9-19.
  • Hayden, P. A., Jordahl, N., & Adams, M. R. (1982). Stutterers' voice initiation times during conditions of novel stimulation: Journal of Fluency Disorders Vol 7(1-1) Mar 1982, 1-7.
  • Hayes, R. D., & Merigan, W. H. (2007). Mechanisms of sensitivity loss due to visual cortex lesions in humans and macaques: Cerebral Cortex Vol 17(5) May 2007, 1117-1128.
  • Heath, S. M., & Hogben, J. H. (2004). The reliability and validity of tasks measuring perception of rapid sequences in children with dyslexia: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Vol 45(7) Oct 2004, 1275-1287.
  • Heckenmueller, E. G., & Dember, W. N. (1965). A forced-choice indicator for use with Werner's disc-ring pattern in studies of backward masking: Psychonomic Science Vol 3(4) 1965, 167-168.
  • Helzer, J. R., Champlin, C. A., & Gillam, R. B. (1996). Auditory temporal resolution in specifically language-impaired and age-matched children: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 83(3, Pt 2) Dec 1996, 1171-1181.
  • Henning, G. B. (1974). Lateralization and the binaural masking-level difference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(6) Jun 1974, 1259-1262.
  • Henry, K. R. (1989). Detuning of cochlear action potential tuning curves at high sound pressure levels: Influence of temporal, spectral and intensity variables: Audiology Vol 28(1) Jan-Feb 1989, 19-36.
  • Henry, K. R. (1992). Enhancement of the compound action potential: Effects of adaptation and unmasking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(5) May 1992, 2824-2830.
  • Herr, D. W., Vo, K. T., King, D., & Boyes, W. K. (1996). Possible confounding effects of strobe "clicks" on flash evoked potentials in rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 59(2) Feb 1996, 325-340.
  • Hicks, M. L., & Bacon, S. P. (1991). The effect of pure-tone forward masking on overshoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(1) Jul 1991, 228-230.
  • Hicks, M. L., & Bacon, S. P. (1995). Some factors influencing comodulation masking release and across-channel masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(5, Pt 1) Nov 1995, 2504-2514.
  • Hicks, M. L., & Bacon, S. P. (1999). Effects of aspirin on psychophysical measures of frequency selectivitiy, two-tone suppression, and growth of masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(3, Pt 1)) Sep 1999, 1436-1451.
  • Higgins, M. B., & Turner, C. W. (1990). Summation bandwidths at threshold in normal and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(6) Dec 1990, 2625-2630.
  • Hill, P. R., Hartley, D. E. H., Glasberg, B. R., Moore, B. C. J., & Moore, D. R. (2004). Auditory Processing Efficiency and Temporal Resolution in Children and Adults: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 47(5) Oct 2004, 1022-1029.
  • Hine, J. E., Martin, R. L., & Moore, D. R. (1994). Free-field binaural unmasking in ferrets: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 108(1) Feb 1994, 196-205.
  • Hines, T. (1999). A demonstration of auditory top-down processing: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers Vol 31(1) Feb 1999, 55-56.
  • Hofer, S. B., & Klump, G. M. (2003). Within-and Across-Channel Processing in Auditory Masking: A Physiological Study in the Songbird Forebrain: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 23(13) Jul 2003, 5732-5739.
  • Hoffman, I., & Levitt, H. (1978). A note on simultaneous and interleaved masking: Journal of Communication Disorders Vol 11(2-3) Apr 1978, 207-213.
  • Holding, D., Loeb, M., & Yoder, D. (1972). "Masking" versus interference in pitch perception: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 12(3) Jul 1972, 247-254.
  • Hoppe, S. A., & Langford, T. L. (1974). Binaural interaction in cat and man: I. Signal detection and noise cross correlation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(6) Jun 1974, 1263-1265.
  • Horii, Y., & Weinberg, B. (1975). Intelligibility characteristics of superior esophageal speech presented under various levels of masking noise: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 18(3) Sep 1975, 413-419.
  • Hornsby, B. W. Y., & Ricketts, T. A. (2007). Directional benefit in the presence of speech and speechlike maskers: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 18(1) Jan 2007, 5-16.
  • Hornsby, B. W. Y., Ricketts, T. A., & Johnson, E. E. (2006). The Effects of Speech and Speechlike Maskers on Unaided and Aided Speech Recognition in Persons with Hearing Loss: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 17(6) Jun 2006, 432-447.
  • Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R., & Ahlstrom, J. B. (2002). Recognition of low-pass filtered consonants in noise with normal and impaired high-frequency hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(1,Pt1) Jan 2002, 409-416.
  • Houtgast, T. (1972). Psychophysical evidence for lateral inhibition in hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1885-1894.
  • Houtgast, T. (1989). Frequency selectivity in amplitude-modulation detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(4) Apr 1989, 1676-1680.
  • Houtsma, A. J. M., & Beerends, J. G. (1986). The role of aural frequency analysis in pitch perception with simultaneous complex tones. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Howard, M. R. (1977). A spectrographic analysis of selected vocal parameters of the fluent utterances of stutterers and nonstutterers under conditions of auditory masking and voluntarily increased vocal intensity: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Howell, P., Rosen, S., Hannigan, G., & Rustin, L. (2000). Auditory backward-masking performance by children who stutter and its relation to dysfluency rate: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 90(2) Apr 2000, 355-363.
  • Hubner, R., & Ellermeier, W. (1993). Additivity of loudness across critical bands: A critical test: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 54(2) Aug 1993, 185-189.
  • Humes, L. E. (1980). Temporary threshold shift for masked pure tones: Audiology Vol 19(4) Jul-Aug 1980, 335-345.
  • Humes, L. E. (1990). Masking of tone bursts by modulated noise in normal, noise-masked normal, and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 33(1) Mar 1990, 3-8.
  • Humes, L. E., Espinoza-Varas, B., & Watson, C. S. (1988). Modeling sensorineural hearing loss: I. Model and retrospective evaluation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(1) Jan 1988, 188-202.
  • Humes, L. E., & Jesteadt, W. (1989). Models of the additivity of masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(3) Mar 1989, 1285-1294.
  • Humes, L. E., & Koval, C. B. (1981). Temporary threshold shifts for masked pure tones: II. Broad-band masker: Audiology Vol 20(4) Jul-Aug 1981, 325-335.
  • Humes, L. E., Lee, L. W., & Jesteadt, W. (1992). Two experiments on the spectral boundary conditions for nonlinear additivity of simultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(5) Nov 1992, 2598-2606.
  • Humes, L. E., & Ochs, M. G. (1982). Use of contralateral masking in the measurement of the auditory brainstem response: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(4) Dec 1982, 528-535.
  • Idson, W. L. (1978). The perception of structure in auditory patterns: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Idson, W. L., & Massaro, D. W. (1976). Cross-octave masking of single tones and musical sequences: The effects of structure on auditory recognition: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 19(2) Feb 1976, 155-175.
  • Idson, W. L., & Massaro, D. W. (1977). Perceptual processing and experience of auditory duration: Sensory Processes Vol 1(4) Aug 1977, 316-337.
  • Idson, W. L., & Massaro, D. W. (1980). The role of perceived duration in the identification of vowels: Journal of Phonetics Vol 8(4) Oct 1980, 407-425.
  • Ingham, J. G. (1963). Cross-masking in neurotic patients: British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology 3(1) 1963, 16-19.
  • Irino, T., & Patterson, R. D. (1996). Temporal asymmetry in the auditory system: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(4, Pt 1) Apr 1996, 2316-2331.
  • Irino, T., & Patterson, R. D. (2001). A compressive gammachirp auditory filter for both physiological and psychophysical data: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2008-2022.
  • Isabelle, S. K., & Colburn, H. S. (1991). Detection of tones in reproducible narrow-band noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(1) Jan 1991, 352-359.
  • Ison, J. R., & Agrawal, P. (1998). The effect of spatial separation of signal and noise on masking in the free field as a function of signal frequency and age in the mouse: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(3, Pt 1)) Sep 1998, 1689-1695.
  • Iverson, G. J., & Pavel, M. (1981). On the functional form of partial masking functions in psychoacoustics: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 24(1) Aug 1981, 1-20.
  • Ivey, R. G., & Goldstein, R. (1984). Middle-component AERs during backward masking: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 24(4) Oct 1984, 279-297.
  • Jagadish, A. (1982). Study of the relationship between the acoustic threshold and threshold of octave masking: Journal of the All-India Institute of Speech & Hearing Vol 13 1982, 140-143.
  • James, C., Blamey, P., Shallop, J. K., Incerti, P. V., & Nicholas, A. M. (2001). Contralateral masking in cochlear implant users with residual hearing in the non-implanted ear: Audiology & Neurotology Vol 6(2) Mar-Apr 2001, 87-97.
  • Janack, T., Pastizzo, M. J., & Feldman, L. B. (2004). When orthographic neighbors fail to facilitate: Brain and Language Vol 90(1-3) Jul-Sep 2004, 441-452.
  • Jerger, J., Hannley, M., & Rivera, V. (1982). Auditory brainstem response and the masking level difference: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Vol 388 Jun 1982, 466-470.
  • Jesteadt, W., Neff, D. L., Humes, L., & Leek, M. R. (1997). Modeling hearing loss as an additional source of masking. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Jiang, D., McAlpine, D., & Palmer, A. R. (1997). Detectability index measures of binaural masking level difference across populations of inferior colliculus neurons: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 17(23) Dec 1997, 9331-9339.
  • Johansson, M., & Arlinger, S. D. (2002). Binaural masking level difference for speech signals in noise: International Journal of Audiology Vol 41(5) Jul 2002, 279-284.
  • John, M. S., Lins, O. G., Boucher, B. L., & Picton, T. W. (1998). Multiple auditory steady-state responses (MASTER): Stimulus and recording parameters: Audiology Vol 37(2) Mar-Apr 1998, 59-82.
  • Johnson, T. E. (1985). The effects of complex partial masking on loudness growth of pure tones and speech: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Jokinen, K. (1973). Presbyacusis: VI. Masking of speech: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 76(6) Dec 1973, 426-430.
  • Kallman, H. J., Beckstead, J. W., & Cameron, P. A. (1988). Ipsilateral and contralateral masking of duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 43(1) Jan 1988, 31-37.
  • Kallman, H. J., & Brown, S. C. (1986). Auditory backward recognition masking with well-practiced listeners: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 26(4) Oct 1986, 239-246.
  • Kallman, H. J., Hirtle, S. C., & Davidson, D. (1986). Recognition masking of auditory duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 40(1) Jul 1986, 45-52.
  • Kallman, H. J., & Massaro, D. W. (1979). Similarity effects in backward recognition masking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 5(1) Feb 1979, 110-128.
  • Kallman, H. J., & Massaro, D. W. (1983). Backward masking, the suffix effect, and preperceptual storage: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 9(2) Apr 1983, 312-327.
  • Kallman, H. J., & Morris, M. D. (1984). Backward recognition masking as a function of ear of masking presentation: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 35(4) Apr 1984, 379-384.
  • Kallman, H. J., & Morris, M. D. (1984). Duration perception and auditory masking: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Vol 423 1984, 608-609.
  • Kallstrand, J., Montnemery, P., Nielzen, S., & Olsson, O. (2002). Auditory masking experiments in schizophrenia: Psychiatry Research Vol 113(1-2) Dec 2002, 115-125.
  • Kaplan, H., & Pickett, J. M. (1984). Release from first-formant masking in presbyacusis: Audiology Vol 23(2) Mar 1984, 165-180.
  • Karlovich, R. S., & Osier, H. A. (1977). Remote masking generated by high-frequency two-tone complexes: Audiology Vol 16(6) 1977, 507-521.
  • Kashino, M., & Warren, R. M. (1996). Binaural release from temporal induction: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 58(6) Aug 1996, 899-905.
  • Kawase, T., Delgutte, B., & Liberman, M. C. (1993). Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex: I. Enhancement of auditory-nerve response to masked tones: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 70(6) Dec 1993, 2533-2549.
  • Kawase, T., & Liberman, C. (1993). Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex: I. Enhancement of compound action potentials to masked tones: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 70(6) Dec 1993, 2519-2532.
  • Keiser, J. H. (1980). Time course of overshoot phenomenon in psychophysical masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kemp, S., & George, R. N. (1992). Masking of tinnitus induced by sound: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 35(5) Oct 1992, 1169-1179.
  • Kevanishvili, Z. S., Gobsh, G., Gamgeveli, Z. A., & Gvelesiani, T. G. (1992). Investigation of evoked otoacoustic emission in a paradigm of forward masking: Sensory Systems Vol 6(1) Jan-Mar 1992, 42-47.
  • Kidd, G., Jr., Mason, C. R., & Arbogast, T. L. (2002). Similarity, uncertainty, and masking in the identification of nonspeech auditory patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(3) Mar 2002, 1367-1376.
  • Kidd, G., Jr., Mason, C. R., Arbogast, T. L., Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, B. D. (2003). Informational masking caused by contralateral stimulation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(3) Mar 2003, 1594-1603.
  • Kidd, G., Jr., Mason, C. R., Brughera, A., & Chiu, C.-Y. P. (2003). Discriminating harmonicity: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(2) Aug 2003, 967-977.
  • Kidd, G., Mason, C. R., Deliwala, P. S., Woods, W. S., & et al. (1994). Reducing informational masking by sound segregation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(6) Jun 1994, 3475-3480.
  • Kidd, G., Jr., Mason, C. R., & Richards, V. M. (2003). Multiple bursts, multiple looks, and stream coherence in the release from informational masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(5) Nov 2003, 2835-2845.
  • Kidd, G., Mason, C. R., & Rohtla, T. L. (1995). Binaural advantage for sound pattern identification: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(4) Oct 1995, 1977-1986.
  • Kidd, G., Jr., Mason, C. R., Rohtla, T. L., & Deliwala, P. S. (1998). Release from masking due to spatial separation of sources in the identification of nonspeech auditory patterns: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(1) Jul 1998, 422-431.
  • Kidd, G., & Wright, B. A. (1994). Improving the detectability of a brief tone in noise using forward and backward masker fringes: Monotic and dichotic presentations: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(2) Feb 1994, 962-967.
  • Kidd, G. D. (1982). Tonal forward masking: Psychophysical estimates of the auditory representation of sound spectra over time: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kimberley, B. P., Nelson, D. A., & Bacon, S. P. (1989). Temporal overshoot in simultaneous-masked psychophysical tuning curves from normal and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(4) Apr 1989, 1660-1665.
  • King, C. D. (1996). A neurophysiologic correlate of comodulation masking release. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Kinoshita, S., & Woollams, A. (2002). The masked onset priming effect in naming: Computation of phonology or speech planning? : Memory & Cognition Vol 30(2) Mar 2002, 237-245.
  • Kirman, J. H. (1986). Vibrotactile frequency recognition: Forward and backward masking effects: Journal of General Psychology Vol 113(2) Apr 1986, 147-158.
  • Klein, A. J., & Dubno, J. R. (1993). Minimal upward spread of masking: Correlations with speech and auditory brainstem response masked thresholds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(6) Jun 1993, 3422-3430.
  • Klein, A. J., Mills, J. H., & Adkins, W. Y. (1990). Upward spread of masking, hearing loss, and speech recognition in young and elderly listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(3) Mar 1990, 1266-1271.
  • Klich, R. J., & May, G. M. (1982). Spectrographic study of vowels in stutterers' fluent speech: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(3) Sep 1982, 364-370.
  • Kluk, K., & Moore, B. C. J. (2006). Detecting dead regions using psychophysical tuning curves: A comparison of simultaneous and forward masking: International Journal of Audiology Vol 45(8) Aug 2006, 463-476.
  • Klump, G. M., & Nieder, A. (2001). Release from masking in fluctuating background noise in a songbird's auditory forebrain: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 12(9) Jul 2001, 1825-1829.
  • Koehnke, J. D. (1983). Masking patterns in binaural detection and interaural time discrimination: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kohlrausch, A. (1988). Auditory filter shape derived from binaural masking experiments: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(2) Aug 1988, 573-583.
  • Kohlrausch, A. (1990). Binaural masking experiments using noise maskers with frequency-dependent interaural phase differences: I. Influence of signal and masker duration: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(4) Oct 1990, 1737-1748.
  • Kohlrausch, A. (1990). Binaural masking experiments using noise maskers with frequency-dependent interaural phase differences: II. Influence of frequency and interaural-phase uncertainty: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(4) Oct 1990, 1749-1756.
  • Kohlrausch, A., & Sander, A. (1995). Phase effects in masking related to dispersion in the inner ear: II. Masking period patterns of short targets: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(3) Mar 1995, 1817-1829.
  • Kollmeier, B., & Gilkey, R. H. (1990). Binaural forward and backward masking: Evidence for sluggishness in binaural detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(4) Apr 1990, 1709-1719.
  • Kollmeier, B., Gilkey, R. H., & Sieben, U. K. (1988). Adaptive staircase techniques in psychoacoustics: A comparison of human data and a mathematical model: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(5) May 1988, 1852-1862.
  • Kopco, N., & Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2003). Spatial unmasking of nearby pure-tone targets in a simulated anechoic environment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(5) Nov 2003, 2856-2870.
  • Koroleva, I. V., & Novitskii, N. Y. (1997). Studies of the interference resistance of the auditory system in children using auditory brainstem evoked potentials: Sensory Systems Vol 11(2) Apr-Jun 1997, 105-111.
  • Koroleva, T. V., Lyublinskaya, V. V., Kornev, A. N., & Yagunova, E. V. (1998). Perception of noise-masked words by normal children aged 4-7 years and children with expressive speech lesions of central etiology: Sensory Systems Vol 12(3) Jul-Sept 1998, 201-209.
  • Koyama, S., Gunji, A., Yabe, H., Yamada, R.-A., Oiwa, S., Kubo, R., et al. (2000). The masking effect in foreign speech sounds perception revealed by neuromagnetic responses: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 11(17) Nov 2000, 3765-3769.
  • Kramer, M. B. (1978). Two-tone suppression in hearing-impaired listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kramer, M. B., Green, D., & Guitar, B. (1987). A comparison of stutterers and nonstutterers on masking level differences and synthetic sentence identification tasks: Journal of Communication Disorders Vol 20(5) Oct 1987, 379-390.
  • Kramer, S. J. (1981). Forward masking of auditory nerve (N-sub-1) and brainstem responses (wave V) in humans: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Krumbholz, K., Patterson, R. D., & Nobbe, A. (2001). Asymmetry of masking between noise and iterated rippled noise: Evidence for time-interval processing in the auditory system: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(4) Oct 2001, 2096-2107.
  • Krummel, S., Petrosino, L., & Fucci, D. J. (1991). Effects of auditory disruption of lingual tactile sensitivity in skilled and unskilled speaking conditions: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 73(2) Oct 1991, 531-538.
  • Kujala, T., Belitz, S., Tervaniemi, M., & Naatanen, R. (2003). Auditory sensory memory disorder in dyslexic adults as indexed by the mismatch negativity: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 17(6) Mar 2003, 1323-1327.
  • Kwon, B. J. (2002). Comodulation masking release in consonant recognition: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(2) Aug 2002, 634-641.
  • Kwon, B. J., & Turner, C. W. (2001). Consonant identification under maskers with sinusoidal modulation: Masking release or modulation interference? : Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(2) Aug 2001, 1130-1140.
  • Lackner, J. R. (1972). An auditory illusion of depth: Cognition Vol 1(2-3) 1972, 201-209.
  • Lakey, J. R. (1974). Temporal masking-level differences: The effect of mask duration: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lang, H. G., & Pocobello, D. E. (1991). Deafness simulation via speech-masking: An experiential learning strategy: Journal of the Academy of Rehabilitative Audiology Vol 24 1991, 133-141.
  • Langemann, U., Gauger, B., & Klump, G. M. (1998). Auditory sensitivity in the great tit: Perception of signals in the presence and absence of noise: Animal Behaviour Vol 56(3) Sep 1998, 763-769.
  • Langemann, U., & Klump, G. M. (2001). Signal detection in amplitude-modulated maskers. I. Behavioural auditory thresholds in a songbird: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 13(5) Mar 2001, 1025-1032.
  • Langendijk, E. H. A., Kistler, D. J., & Wightman, F. L. (2001). Sound localization in the presence of one or two distracters: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2123-2134.
  • Langford, T. L. (1983). Neural responses elicited by stimuli associated with masking of low-frequency, monaural tonal signals by noise: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 50(6) Dec 1983, 1516-1521.
  • Langford, T. L., & Jeffress, L. A. (1964). Effect of noise crosscorrelation on binaural signal detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 36(8) 1964, 1455-1458.
  • Langhans, A., & Kohlrausch, A. (1992). Differences in auditory performance between monaural and diotic conditions: I. Masked thresholds in frozen noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(6) Jun 1992, 3456-3470.
  • Langhans, A., & Kohlrausch, A. (1992). Spectral integration of broadband signals in diotic and dichotic masking experiments: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 317-326.
  • Larsby, B., & Arlinger, S. (1994). Speech recognition and just-follow-conversation tasks for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners with different maskers: Audiology Vol 33(3) May-Jun 1994, 165-176.
  • Larson-Prior, L. J., Hart, M. T., & Jewett, D. L. (2004). Neural processing of high-rate auditory stimulation under conditions of various maskers: Neurocomputing: An International Journal Vol 58-60 2004, 993-998.
  • Laucius, G., & Young, I. M. (1972). Contralateral masking effects on auditory thresholds: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 12(4) Oct 1972, 271-275.
  • Lazarus-Mainka, G., & Hormann, H. (1978). Lateral hemispheric perception of sentences covered by noise dependent on acoustic and linguistic factors: Psychologische Beitrage Vol 20(3) 1978, 375-389.
  • Lechner, B. K. (1979). The effects of delayed auditory feedback and masking on the fundamental frequency of stutterers and nonstutterers: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 22(2) Jun 1979, 343-353.
  • LeCompte, D. C. (1995). An irrelevant speech effect with repeated and continuous background speech: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 2(3) Sep 1995, 391-397.
  • Lee, J., & Bacon, S. P. (1998). Psychophysical suppression as a function of signal frequency: Noise and tonal maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(2, Pt 1) Aug 1998, 1013-1022.
  • Lee, L. W.-L. (1994). Loudness of complex stimuli. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Leech, R., Aydelott, J., Symons, G., Carnevale, J., & Dick, F. (2007). The development of sentence interpretation: Effects of perceptual, attentional and semantic interference: Developmental Science Vol 10(6) Nov 2007, 794-813.
  • Leek, M. R., Brown, M. E., & Dorman, M. F. (1991). Informational masking and auditory attention: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 50(3) Sep 1991, 205-214.
  • Legge, G. E., Cohen, M. A., & Stromeyer, C. F. (1978). Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns: Perception Vol 7(2) 1978, 161-166.
  • Leshowitz, B., & Cudahy, E. (1972). Masking with continuous and gated sinusoids: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1921-1929.
  • Leshowitz, B., Wightman, F. L., & Macmillan, N. A. (1972). On the importance of considering the signal's frequency spectrum: Some comments on Macmillan's "Detection and recognition of increments and decrements in auditory intensity" experiment: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 12(2-B) Aug 1972, 209-212.
  • Levanen, S., & Sams, M. (1997). Disrupting human auditory change detection: Chopin is superior to white noise: Psychophysiology Vol 34(3) May 1997, 258-265.
  • Lewis, H. D., Benignus, V. A., Muller, K. E., Malott, C. M., & et al. (1988). Babble and random-noise masking of speech in high and low context cue conditions: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 31(1) Mar 1988, 108-114.
  • Li, L., Daneman, M., Qi, J. G., & Schneider, B. A. (2004). Does the Information Content of an Irrelevant Source Differentially Affect Spoken Word Recognition in Younger and Older Adults? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 30(6) Dec 2004, 1077-1091.
  • Li, X. (1993). On integrality of perceptual attributes determined by sound source and filter: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Libbey, B. W. (2003). Reverberant word intelligibility and psychological models of dereverberation. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Liebenthal, E., & Pratt, H. (1999). Human auditory cortex electrophysiological correlates of the precedence effect: Binaural echo lateralization suppression: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(1) Jul 1999, 291-303.
  • Lin, W.-Y., & Feng, A. S. (2003). GABA Is Involved in Spatial Unmasking in the Frog Auditory Midbrain: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 23(22) Nov 2003, 8143-8151.
  • Lindenman, M. (1980). The relations between marihuana smoking and selected measures of human hearing: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lindsey, J. W. (1972). Binaural analysis as a function of physiological masking: The cardiac cycle: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Yin, T. C. T. (1998). Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat. I. Correlates of psychophysics: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 80(3) Sep 1998, 1285-1301.
  • Litovsky, R. Y., & Yin, T. C. T. (1998). Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat. II. Neural mechanisms: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 80(3) Sep 1998, 1302-1316.
  • Loeb, M., & Holding, D. H. (1972). Delayed interference in pitch judgments: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 12(4) Oct 1972, 336-339.
  • Loeb, M., & Holding, D. H. (1975). Backward interference by tones or noise in pitch perception as a function of practice: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 18(3) Sep 1975, 205-208.
  • Loewen, L. J., & Suedfeld, P. (1992). Cognitive and arousal effects of masking office noise: Environment and Behavior Vol 24(3) May 1992, 381-395.
  • Lohr, B., Wright, T. F., & Dooling, R. J. (2003). Detection and discrimination of natural calls in masking noise by birds: Estimating the active space of signal: Animal Behaviour Vol 65(4) Apr 2003, 763-777.
  • Loren, C. A., Colcord, R. D., & Rastatter, M. P. (1986). Effects of auditory masking by white noise on variability of fundamental frequency during highly similar productions of spontaneous speech: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 63(3) Dec 1986, 1203-1206.
  • Lorenzi, C., Gatehouse, S., & Lever, C. (1999). Sound localization in noise in hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(6) Jun 1999, 3454-3463.
  • Lorenzi, C., Husson, M., Ardoint, M., & Debruille, X. (2006). Speech masking release in listeners with flat hearing loss: Effects of masker fluctuation rate on identification scores and phonetic feature reception: International Journal of Audiology Vol 45(9) Sep 2006, 487-495.
  • Lorenzi, C., Micheyl, C., Berthommier, F., & Portalier, S. (1997). Modulation masking in listeners with sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 40(1) Feb 1997, 200-207.
  • Lotto, A. J., Sullivan, S. C., & Holt, L. L. (2003). Central locus for nonspeech context effects on phonetic identification (L): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(1) Jan 2003, 53-56.
  • Loven, F. C., & Collins, M. J. (1988). Reverberation, masking, filtering, and level effects on speech recognition performance: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 31(4) Dec 1988, 681-695.
  • Loven, F. Y. (1986). Some effects of reverberation, filtering, masking, and speech level on speech identification ability of normal hearing listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lu, C.-T., & Wang, H.-C. (2003). Enhancement of single channel speech based on masking property and wavelet transform: Speech Communication Vol 41(2-3) Oct 2003, 409-427.
  • Lu, Z.-L., & Dosher, B. A. (2001). Characterizing the spatial-frequency sensitivity of perceptual templates: Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science & Vision Vol 18(9) Sep 2001, 2041-2053.
  • Lucker, J. R. (1980). Effects of signal length and competing message content on auditory selective attention in normal achieving and learning disabled children: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lutfi, R. A., Kistler, D. J., Callahan, M. R., & Wightman, F. L. (2003). Psychometric functions for informational masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(6,Pt1) Dec 2003, 3273-3282.
  • Lutfi, R. A., Kistler, D. J., Oh, E. L., Wightman, F. L., & Callahan, M. R. (2003). One factor underlies individual differences in auditory informational masking within and across age groups: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 65(3) Apr 2003, 396-406.
  • Lynn, G. K. (1975). Interactions of backward and forward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lyzenga, J., & Carlyon, R. P. (1999). Center frequency modulation detection for harmonic complexes resembling vowel formants and its interference by off-frequency maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(5) May 1999, 2792-2806.
  • Ma, C., & O'Shaughnessy, D. (1994). The masking of narrowband noise by broadband harmonic complex sounds and implications for the processing of speech sounds: Speech Communication Vol 14(2) Apr 1994, 103-118.
  • Macdonald, J. A. (2004). An ideal observer approach to temporal auditory masking and speech intelligibility. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Mackersie, C. L. (1995). Comodulation masking release in frozen and randomly-selected noise. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Mackersie, C. L., Prida, T. L., & Stiles, D. (2001). The role of sequential stream segregation and frequency selectivity in the perception of simultaneous sentences by listeners with sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 44(1) Feb 2001, 19-28.
  • Mallard, A. R., & Webb, W. G. (1980). The effects of auditory and visual "distractors" on the frequency of stuttering: Journal of Communication Disorders Vol 13(3) May 1980, 207-212.
  • Malone, B. J., Scott, B. H., & Semple, M. N. (2002). Context-dependent adaptive coding of interaural phase disparity in the auditory cortex of awake macaques: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 22(11) Jun 2002, No Pagination Specified.
  • Manning, W. H., & Hadley, S. W. (1987). Predicting articulatory performance during treatment breaks: Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol 18(1) Jan 1987, 15-22.
  • Manning, W. H., Keappock, N. E., & Stick, S. L. (1976). The use of auditory masking to estimate automatization of correct articulatory production: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders Vol 41(2) May 1976, 143-149.
  • Manning, W. H., Louko, L. J., & DiSalvo, V. S. (1978). A right-ear effect for auditory feedback control of children's newly acquired phonemes: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 21(3) Sep 1978, 580-588.
  • Manning, W. H., Riensche, L. L., & McCarty, G. A. (1979). Articulatory effects of monaural and binaural masking in normal speaking adults wearing palatal appliances: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 22(4) Dec 1979, 773-783.
  • Manning, W. H., Wittstruck, M. L., Loyd, R. R., & Campbell, T. F. (1977). Automatization of correct production at two levels of articulatory acquisition: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders Vol 42(3) Aug 1977, 358-363.
  • March, L., Cienfuegos, A., Goldbloom, L., Ritter, W., Cowan, N., & Javitt, D. C. (1999). Normal time course of auditory recognition in schizophrenia, despite impaired precision of the auditory sensory ("echoic") memory code: Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol 108(1) Feb 1999, 69-75.
  • Margolis, R. H., Dubno, J. R., & Hunt, S. M. (1981). Detection of tones in band-reject noise: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(3) Sep 1981, 336-344.
  • Margolis, R. H., & Goldberg, S. M. (1980). Auditory frequency selectivity in normal and presbycusic subjects: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 23(3) Sep 1980, 603-613.
  • Marsh, J. T., Brown, W. S., & Smith, J. C. (1975). Far-field recorded frequency-following responses: Correlates of low pitch auditory perception in humans: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 38(2) Feb 1975, 113-119.
  • Martin, B. A., Kurtzberg, D., & Stapels, D. R. (1999). The effects of dencreased audibility produced by high-pass noise masking on N1 and the mismatch negativity to speech sounds /ba/ and /da: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 42(2) Apr 1999, 271-286.
  • Martin, F. N. (1974). Minimum effective masking levels in threshold audiometry: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders Vol 39(3) Aug 1974, 280-285.
  • Martin, F. N., Bailey, H. A., Jr., & Pappas, J. J. (1965). The effect of central masking on threshold for speech: Journal of Auditory Research 5(4) 1965, 293-296.
  • Marvit, P., Florentine, M., & Buus, S. (2003). A comparison of psychophysical procedures for level-discrimination thresholds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(6) Jun 2003, 3348-3378.
  • Masaki, H., Tanaka, H., Takasawa, N., & Yamazaki, K. (2001). Error-related brain potentials elicited by vocal errors: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 12(9) Jul 2001, 1851-1855.
  • Mason, C. R., Idrobo, F., Early, S. J., Abibi, A., Zheng, L., Harrison, J. M., et al. (2003). CS-dependent response probability in an auditory masked-detection task: Considerations based on models of Pavlovian conditioning: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 56B(2) May 2003, 193-205.
  • Massaro, D. W. (1973). A comparison of forward versus backward recognition masking: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 100(2) Oct 1973, 434-436.
  • Massaro, D. W., & Burke, D. (1991). Perceptual development and auditory backward recognition masking: Developmental Psychology Vol 27(1) Jan 1991, 85-96.
  • Massaro, D. W., & Idson, W. L. (1976). Temporal course of perceived auditory duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 20(5) Nov 1976, 331-352.
  • Massaro, D. W., & Idson, W. L. (1977). Backward recognition masking in relative pitch judgments: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 45(1) Aug 1977, 87-97.
  • Massaro, D. W., & Idson, W. L. (1978). Target-mask similarity in backward recognition masking of perceived tone duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 24(3) Sep 1978, 225-236.
  • Massaro, D. W., & Idson, W. L. (1978). Temporal course of perceived vowel duration: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 21(1) Mar 1978, 37-55.
  • McAlpine, D., Jiang, D., & Palmer, A. R. (1996). Binaural masking level differences in the inferior colliculus of the guinea pig: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(1) Jul 1996, 490-503.
  • McArthur, G., & Bishop, D. (2002). Event-related potentials reflect individual differences in age-invariant auditory skills: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 13(8) Jun 2002, 1079-1082.
  • McArthur, G. M., & Hogben, J. H. (2001). Auditory backward recognition masking in children with a specific language impairment and children with a specific reading disability: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(3) Mar 2001, 1092-1100.
  • McClean, M. (1977). Effects of auditory masking on lip movements during speech: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 20(4) Dec 1977, 731-741.
  • McDermott, J. C. (1981). Cochlear initiation sites of the human auditory frequency following potentials: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • McDermott, J. C. (1983). Cochlear initiation sites of the frequency following potential: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 12(2) 1983, 97-102.
  • McDowell, D., Reynolds, B., & Magaro, P. (1975). The integration defect in paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenia: Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol 84(6) Dec 1975, 629-636.
  • McFadden, D. (1988). Absence of overshoot in a dichotic masking condition: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(4) Apr 1988, 1685-1687.
  • McFadden, D. (1989). Spectral differences in the ability of temporal gaps to reset the mechanisms underlying overshoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(1) Jan 1989, 254-261.
  • McFadden, D., & Wright, B. A. (1987). Comodulation masking release in a forward-masking paradigm: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1615-1620.
  • McFadden, D., & Wright, B. A. (1990). Temporal decline of masking and comodulation detection differences: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(2) Aug 1990, 711-724.
  • McFadden, D., & Wright, B. A. (1992). Temporal decline of masking and comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(1) Jul 1992, 144-156.
  • McFadden, S. L., Henderson, D., & Quaranta, A. (1997). Remote masking in normal-hearing and noise-exposed chinchillas: Audiology & Neurotology Vol 2(3) May-Jun 1997, 128-138.
  • McGuire, R. A., & Rastatter, M. P. (1985). Masking effects on formant frequency structure variability under selected speaking conditions: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 25(2) Apr 1985, 73-80.
  • Mendoza, L., Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1995). Within- and across-channel processes in modulation detection interference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(5, Pt 1) May 1995, 3072-3079.
  • Mershon, D. H., Ballenger, W. L., Little, A. D., McMurtry, P. L., & et al. (1989). Effects of room reflectance and background noise on perceived auditory distance: Perception Vol 18(3) 1989, 403-416.
  • Meshcherskaya, L. N. (1985). Use of external acoustic influence for correction of stuttering: Defektologiya No 3 1985, 14-19.
  • Meyer, T. A. (1996). Simulating the effects of aging and hearing loss on the masking-level difference. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Michaud, C. M. (1981). Masking level differences in patients with brainstem lesions: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Micheyl, C., & Collet, L. (1994). Interrelations between psychoacoustical tuning curves and spontaneous and evoked otoacoustic emissions: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 23(3) 1994, 171-178.
  • Millar, K. (1979). Noise and the "rehearsal-masking hypothesis." British Journal of Psychology Vol 70(4) Nov 1979, 565-577.
  • Miller, T. H. (1974). Interactive effects of stimulus bandwidth and type of masker presentation on the function relating detection performance to signal duration: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mills, J. H., Dubno, J. R., & He, N.-j. (1996). Masking by ipsilateral and contralateral maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(5, Pt 1) Nov 1996, 3336-3344.
  • Mitchell, C. (1976). Frequency specificity of the N-sub-1 potential from the cochlear nerve under various stimulus conditions: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 16(4) 1976, 247-255.
  • Mitchell, C. (1983). The masking of tinnitus with pure tones: Audiology Vol 22(1) Jan-Feb 1983, 73-87.
  • Mitchell, L. M., & Singh, S. (1974). Perceptual structure of 16 prevocalic English consonants sententially embedded: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(6) Jun 1974, 1355-1357.
  • Miyazaki, K. (1978). The temporal masking pattern of gated or repeated bursts of wide-band noise: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 37(1-4) 1978, 76-86.
  • Miyazaki, K., & Sasaki, T. (1981). Auditory masking patterns in forward-, backward-, and simultaneous-masking situations: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 52(2) Jun 1981, 106-112.
  • Miyazaki, K. i., & Sasaki, T. (1984). Pure-tone masking patterns in nonsimultaneous masking conditions: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 26(2) 1984, 110-119.
  • Monaci, G., Menegaz, G., Susstrunk, S., & Knoblauch, K. (2004). Chromatic contrast detection in spatial chromatic noise: Visual Neuroscience Vol 21(3) May-Jun 2004, 291-294.
  • Montgomery, C. R., & Clarkson, M. G. (1997). Infants' pitch perception: Masking by low- and high-frequency noises: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(6) Dec 1997, 3665-3672.
  • Montnemery, P., Almqvist, B., & Harris, S. (1992). Recognition of telegraphy signs at different listening levels and frequencies: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 21(4) 1992, 255-260.
  • Montnemery, P., Almqvist, B., & Harris, S. (1995). Performance of electronic morse decoders in decoding telegraphy masked by noise: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 24(1) 1995, 57-62.
  • Montnemery, P., & Harris, S. (1995). Recognition of telegraphy in hearing-impaired telegraph operators: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 24(2) 1995, 107-115.
  • Moore, B. C. (1973). Some experiments relating to the perception of complex tones: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 25(4) Nov 1973, 451-475.
  • Moore, B. C. (1986). Parallels between frequency selectivity measured psychophysically and in cochlear mechanics: Scandinavian Audiology Suppl 25 1986, 139-152.
  • Moore, B. C., & Glasberg, B. R. (1990). Frequency discrimination of complex tones with overlapping and non-overlapping harmonics: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(5) May 1990, 2163-2177.
  • Moore, B. C., Glasberg, B. R., Gaunt, T., & Child, T. (1991). Across-channel masking of changes in modulation depth for amplitude- and frequency-modulated signals: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 43A(3) Aug 1991, 327-347.
  • Moore, B. C., Glasberg, B. R., & Simpson, A. (1992). Evaluation of a method of simulating reduced frequency selectivity: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(6) Jun 1992, 3402-3423.
  • Moore, B. C., Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Schooneveldt, G. P. (1990). Some factors affecting the magnitude of comodulation masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(4) Oct 1990, 1694-1702.
  • Moore, B. C., & Jorasz, U. (1992). Detection of changes in modulation depth of a target sound in the presence of other modulated sounds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1051-1061.
  • Moore, B. C., & Raab, D. H. (1974). Pure-tone intensity discrimination: Some experiments relating to the "near-miss" to Weber's law: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(5) May 1974, 1049-1054.
  • Moore, B. C., & Schooneveldt, G. P. (1990). Comodulation masking release as a function of bandwidth and time delay between on-frequency and flanking-band maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(2) Aug 1990, 725-731.
  • Moore, B. C., & Shailer, M. J. (1991). Comodulation masking release as a function of level: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(2, Pt 1) Aug 1991, 829-835.
  • Moore, B. C., Shailer, M. J., & Black, M. J. (1993). Dichotic interference effects in gap detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(4, Pt 1) Apr 1993, 2130-2133.
  • Moore, B. C., Shailer, M. J., Hall, J. W., & Schooneveldt, G. P. (1993). Comodulation masking release in subjects with unilateral and bilateral hearing impairment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(1) Jan 1993, 435-451.
  • Moore, B. C. J. (1995). Frequency analysis and masking. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Moore, B. C. J. (1999). Modulation minimizes masking: Nature Vol 397(6715) Jan 1999, 108-109.
  • Moore, B. C. J. (2001). Basic auditory processes. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Moore, B. C. J., Alcantara, J. I., & Dau, T. (1998). Masking patterns for sinusoidal and narrow-band noise maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(2, Pt 1) Aug 1998, 1023-1038.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Borrill, S. J. (2002). Tests of a within-channel account of comodulation detection differences: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(5,Pt1) Nov 2002, 2099-2109.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Glasberg, B. R. (1987). Factors affecting thresholds for sinusoidal signals in narrow-band maskers with fluctuating envelopes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(1) Jul 1987, 69-79.
  • Moore, B. C. J., Glasberg, B. R., van der Heijden, M., Houtsma, A. J. M., & et al. (1995). Comparison of auditory filter shapes obtained with notched-noise and noise-tone maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(2) Feb 1995, 1175-1182.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Jorasz, U. (1996). Modulation discrimination interference and comodulation masking release as a function of the number and spectral placement of narrow-band noise modulators: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(4, Pt 1) Oct 1996, 2373-2381.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Oxenham, A. J. (1998). Psychoacoustic consequences of compression in the peripheral auditory system: Psychological Review Vol 105(1) Jan 1998, 108-124.
  • Moore, B. C. J., Sek, A., & Glasberg, B. R. (1999). Modulation masking produced by beating modulators: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(2) Aug 1999, 908-918.
  • Moore, B. C. J., Sek, A., & Shailer, M. J. (1995). Modulation discrimination interference for narrow-band noise modulators: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(4) Apr 1995, 2493-2497.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Shailer, M. J. (1994). Effects of harmonicity, modulator phase, and number of masker components on modulation discrimination interference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(6) Jun 1994, 3555-3560.
  • Moore, B. C. J., & Vickers, D. A. (1997). The role of spread excitation and suppression in simultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(4) Oct 1997, 2284-2290.
  • Moore, D. R., Hutchings, M. E., & Meyer, S. E. (1991). Binaural masking level differences in children with a history of otitis media: Audiology Vol 30(2) 1991, 91-101.
  • Moore, M. J. (1981). Response latencies following masked-tone stimulation before and after COCB lesion in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Moore, M. S., & Adams, M. R. (1985). The Edinburgh Masker: A clinical analog study: Journal of Fluency Disorders Vol 10(4) Dec 1985, 281-290.
  • Morgan, M. D., Triana, M. A., & Milroy, T. J. (2004). The Effect of Auditory Feedback on Phonation Threshold Pressure Measurement: Journal of Voice Vol 18(1) Mar 2004, 46-55.
  • Mori, S., & Ward, L. M. (1990). Unmasking the magnitude estimation response: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 44(1) Mar 1990, 58-68.
  • Mori, S., & Ward, L. M. (1992). Intensity and frequency resolution: Masking of absolute identification and fixed and roving discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 246-255.
  • Morita, T., Fujiki, N., Nagamine, T., Hiraumi, H., Naito, Y., Shibasaki, H., et al. (2006). Effects of continuous masking noise on tone-evoked magnetic fields in humans: Brain Research Vol 1087(1) May 2006, 151-158.
  • Mott, J. B. (1986). Effects of the temporal properties of a masker upon masking patterns: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mott, J. B., & Feth, L. L. (1986). Effects of the temporal properties of a masker upon simultaneous masking patterns. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Mullennix, J. W., & Howe, J. N. (1999). Selective attention in perceptual adjustments to voice: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 89(2) Oct 1999, 447-457.
  • Mulligan, B. E., & Cornelius, P. T. (1972). Note on psychometric invariance of detection functions: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 52(4, Pt 2) Oct 1972, 1207-1208.
  • Mulligan, B. E., Lewis, S. A., Faupel, M. L., Goodman, L. S., & et al. (1987). Enhancement and masking of loudness by environmental factors: Vegetation and noise: Environment and Behavior Vol 19(4) Jul 1987, 411-443.
  • Murnane, O., & Turner, C. W. (1991). Growth of masking in sensorineural hearing loss: Audiology Vol 30(5) Sep-Oct 1991, 275-285.
  • Nabelek, A. K., Letwoski, T. R., & Tucker, F. M. (1989). Reverberant overlap- and self-masking in consonant identification: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(4) Oct 1989, 1259-1265.
  • Nabelek, A. K., & Robinette, L. (1978). Reverberation as a parameter in clinical testing: Audiology Vol 17(3) May-Jun 1978, 239-259.
  • Nakajima, T., & Ando, Y. (1991). Effects of a single reflection with varied horizontal angle and time delay on speech intelligibility: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(6) Dec 1991, 3173-3179.
  • Nandur, V. U. (1982). Effect of binaural masking noise on stuttering: A spectographic analysis: Journal of the All-India Institute of Speech & Hearing Vol 13 1982, 164-169.
  • Navarro, R. (1996). Effects of ear canal occlusion and masking on the perception of voice: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 82(1) Feb 1996, 199-208.
  • Needleman, A. R., & Crandell, C. C. (1997). Speech perception in noise by listeners with hearing impairment and simulated sensorineural hearing loss. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Neff, D. L. (1991). Forward masking by maskers of uncertain frequency content: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(3) Mar 1991, 1314-1323.
  • Neff, D. L. (1995). Signal properties that reduce masking by simultaneous, random-frequency maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(4) Oct 1995, 1909-1920.
  • Neff, D. L., & Callaghan, B. P. (1988). Effective properties of multicomponent simultaneous maskers under conditions of uncertainty: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(5) May 1988, 1833-1838.
  • Neff, D. L., & Dethlefs, T. M. (1995). Individual differences in simultaneous masking with random-frequency, multicomponent maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(1) Jul 1995, 125-134.
  • Neff, D. L., Dethlefs, T. M., & Jesteadt, W. (1993). Informational masking for multicomponent maskers with spectral gaps: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(6) Dec 1993, 3112-3126.
  • Neff, D. L., & Green, D. M. (1987). Masking produced by spectral uncertainty with multicomponent maskers: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 41(5) May 1987, 409-415.
  • Neff, D. L., & Jesteadt, W. (1996). Intensity discrimination in the presence of random-frequency, multicomponent maskers and broadband noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(4, Pt 1) Oct 1996, 2289-2298.
  • Neff, D. L., Kessler, C. J., & Dethlefs, T. M. (1996). Sex differences in simultaneous masking with random-frequency maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(4, Pt 1) Oct 1996, 2547-2550.
  • Nelken, I., Rotman, Y., & Yosef, O. B. (1999). Responses of auditory-cortex neurons to structural features of natural sounds: Nature Vol 397(6715) Jan 1999, 154-157.
  • Nelson, D. A. (1979). Two-tone masking and auditory critical bandwidths: Audiology Vol 18(4) Jul-Aug 1979, 279-306.
  • Nelson, D. A. (1991). High-level psychophysical tuning curves: Forward masking in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 34(6) Dec 1991, 1233-1249.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Bilger, R. C. (1974). Pure-tone octave masking in listeners with sensorineural hearing loss: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 17(2) Jun 1974, 252-269.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Bilger, R. C. (1974). Pure-tone octave masking in normal-hearing listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 17(2) Jun 1974, 223-251.
  • Nelson, D. A., Chargo, S. J., Kopun, J. G., & Freyman, R. L. (1990). Effects of stimulus level on forward-masked psychophysical tuning curves in quiet and in noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(5) Nov 1990, 2143-2151.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Donaldson, G. S. (2001). Psychophysical recovery from single-pulse forward masking in electric hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(6) Jun 2001, 2921-2933.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Donaldston, G. S. (2002). Psychophysical recovery from pulse-train forward masking in electric hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(6) Dec 2002, 2932-2947.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Schroder, A. C. (1996). Release from upward spread of masking in regions of high-frequency hearing loss: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(4, Pt 1) Oct 1996, 2266-2277.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Schroder, A. C. (1997). Linearized response growth inferred from growth-of-masking slopes in ears with cochlear hearing loss: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(4, Pt 1) Apr 1997, 2186-2201.
  • Neuert, V., Verhey, J. L., & Winter, I. M. (2004). Responses of Dorsal Cochlear Nucleus Neurons to Signals in the Presence of Modulated Maskers: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 24(25) Jun 2004, 5789-5797.
  • Newman, C. W. (1982). Differences in an auditory backward recognition-masking task between young and elderly samples: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Newman, R. S. (2006). Perceptual restoration in toddlers: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 68(4) May 2006, 625-642.
  • Nieder, A., & Klump, G. M. (2001). Signal detection in amplitude-modulated maskers. II. Processing in the songbird's auditory forebrain: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 13(5) Mar 2001, 1033-1044.
  • Niemiec, A. J., Yost, W. A., & Shofner, W. P. (1992). Behavioral measures of frequency selectivity in the chinchilla: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(5) Nov 1992, 2636-2649.
  • Nishimura, T., Nakagawa, S., Sakaguchi, T., Hosoi, H., & Tonoike, M. (2003). Effect of a forward masker on the N1m amplitude: Varying the signal delay: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 14(6) Mar 2003, 891-893.
  • Nizami, L., Reimer, J. F., & Jesteadt, W. (2002). The mid-level hump at 2 kHz: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(2) Aug 2002, 642-653.
  • Nizami, L., & Schneider, B. A. (1999). The fine structure of the recovering auditory detection threshold: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(2) Aug 1999, 1187-1190.
  • Nober, E. H., & Simmons, J. Q. (1981). Comparison of auditory stimulus processing in normal and autistic adolescents: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Vol 11(2) Jun 1981, 175-189.
  • Noffsinger, D., Martinez, C. D., & Schaefer, A. B. (1982). Auditory brainstem responses and masking level differences from persons with brainstem lesion: Scandinavian Audiology Suppl 15 1982, 81-93.
  • Noizet, G., Bleuchot, S., & Henry, R. (1973). Influence of syntactic structure on the strategies used in perceptual decoding of recorded sentences: Cahiers de Psychologie Vol 16(2-4) 1973, 149-170.
  • Norton, S. J., & Mott, J. B. (1987). Effects of auditory fatigue on psychophysical estimates of cochlear nonlinearities: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(1) Jul 1987, 80-87.
  • Novak, R. E., & Anderson, C. V. (1982). Differentiation of types of presbycusis using the masking-level difference: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(4) Dec 1982, 504-508.
  • Nozza, R. J. (1987). The binaural masking level difference in infants and adults: Developmental change in binaural hearing: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 10(1) Jan-Mar 1987, 105-110.
  • Nozza, R. J., Wagner, E. F., & Crandell, M. A. (1988). Binaural release from masking for a speech sound in infants, preschoolers, and adults: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 31(2) Jun 1988, 212-218.
  • Nozza, R. J., & Wilson, W. R. (1984). Masked and unmasked pure-tone thresholds of infants and adults: Development of auditory frequency selectivity and sensitivity: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 27(4) Dec 1984, 613-622.
  • Oates, P., & Stapells, D. R. (1997). Frequency specificity of the human auditory brainstem and middle latency responses to brief tones. I. High-pass noise masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(6) Dec 1997, 3597-3608.
  • Oates, P., & Stapells, D. R. (1997). Frequency specificity of the human auditory brainstem and middle latency responses to brief tones. II. Derived response analyses: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(6) Dec 1997, 3609-3619.
  • Oates, P. A., & Purdy, S. C. (2001). Frequency specificity of the human auditory brainstem and middle latency responses using notched noise masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(2) Aug 2001, 995-1009.
  • O'Connor, K., & Ison, J. R. (1991). Echoic memory in the rat: Effects of inspection time, retention interval, and the spectral composition of masking noise: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 17(4) Oct 1991, 377-385.
  • Odgers, R. P., & Colle, H. A. (1980). Status of spatial location in auditory sensory memory: Evidence from rapidly presented lists: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 6(5) Sep 1980, 546-557.
  • Oei, M. L. Y. M., Segenhout, J. M., Wit, H. P., & Albers, F. W. J. (2001). The vestibular evoked response to linear, alternating, acceleration pulses without acoustic masking as a parameter of vestibular function: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 121(1) Jan 2001, 62-67.
  • Oh, E. L. (1998). Auditory detection in uncertain noise backgrounds. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Oh, E. L., & Lufti, R. A. (1998). Nonmonotonicity of informational masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(6) Dec 1998, 3489-3499.
  • Oh, E. L., & Lutfi, R. A. (1999). Informational masking by everyday sounds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(6) Dec 1999, 3521-3528.
  • Okada, H. (1985). Simultaneous masking for pure tones by frequency modulated tones: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 44(1-4) 1985, 38-45.
  • Okada, H. (1986). Pitch shifts for pure tones caused by the contralateral presented tones: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 45(1-4) 1986, 28-36.
  • Okada, H., & Matsuoka, K. (1992). Effects of auditory imagery on the detection of a pure tone in white noise: Experimental evidence of the auditory perky effect: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 74(2) Apr 1992, 443-448.
  • Okanoya, K., & Dooling, R. J. (1987). Hearing in passerine and psittacine birds: A comparative study of absolute and masked auditory thresholds: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 101(1) Mar 1987, 7-15.
  • Olsho, L. W. (1985). Infant auditory perception: Tonal masking: Infant Behavior & Development Vol 8(4) Oct-Dec 1985, 371-384.
  • O'Malley, H. (1978). Auditory frequency selectivity and two-tone suppression in normal hearing human listeners: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Oshrin, S. E. (1978). The effect of noise exposure on the masking level difference: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Osman, E. (1973). Correlation model of binaural detection: Interaural amplitude ratio and phase variation for signal: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(2) Aug 1973, 386-389.
  • Osman, E., Schacknow, P. N., & Lin Tzuo, P.-F. (1973). Psychometric functions and a correlation model of binaural detection: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 14(2) Oct 1973, 371-374.
  • Osman, E., Tzuo, H., & Tzuo, P. L. (1980). Weber's law, the "near miss," and binaural detection: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 28(4) Oct 1980, 354-364.
  • Osman, E., Tzuo, H., & Tzuo, P. L. (1982). Binaural masking of tones by tones: Effects of uncertainty regarding the phase angle of addition of signal to masker: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 31(6) Jun 1982, 567-572.
  • Osman, E., Tzuo, P.-f. L., & Tzuo, H.-y. (1981). On the detection of a brief signal in the temporal center of a noise masker of varying duration: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 30(4) Oct 1981, 372-374.
  • Ott, R., & Curio, I. (1999). Masking of complex sound: Technical notes for realization of constant signal-noise-ratio: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 89(1) Aug 1999, 137-144.
  • Overson, G. J., Bacon, S. P., & Webb, T. M. (1996). The effect of level and relative frequency region on the recovery of overshoot: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 99(2) Feb 1996, 1059-1065.
  • Owen, G. A., & Burkard, R. (1991). Ipsilateral, contralateral, and binaural masking effects on the human brain-stem auditory-evoked responses to click stimuli: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(4, Pt 1) Apr 1991, 1760-1767.
  • Oxenham, A. J. (1998). Temporal integration at 6 kHz as a function of masker bandwidth: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(2) Feb 1998, 1033-1042.
  • Oxenham, A. J. (2001). Forward masking: Adaptation or integration? : Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(2) Feb 2001, 732-741.
  • Oxenham, A. J., & Dau, T. (2001). Reconciling frequency selectivity and phase effects in masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(3,Pt1) Sep 2001, 1525-1538.
  • Oxenham, A. J., Fligor, B. J., Mason, C. R., & Kidd, G., Jr. (2003). Informational masking and musical training: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(3) Sep 2003, 1543-1549.
  • Oxenham, A. J., & Moore, B. C. J. (1995). Additivity of masking in normally hearing and hearing-impaired subjects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(4) Oct 1995, 1921-1934.
  • Oxenham, A. J., & Moore, B. C. J. (1995). Overshoot and the "severe departure" from Weber's law: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(4) Apr 1995, 2442-2453.
  • Oxenham, A. J., & Moore, B. C. J. (1997). Modeling the effects of peripheral nonlinearity in listeners with normal and impaired hearing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Oxenham, A. J., & Plack, C. J. (1998). Suppression and the upward spread of masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(6) Dec 1998, 3500-3510.
  • Pahn, J., Dahl, R., & Pahn, E. (2001). Relationship between measures of voice penetrating capacity, Pahn's voice status, and selected parameters of the voice analysis program MDVP (Kay): Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica Vol 53(6) Nov-Dec 2001, 308-316.
  • Palmer, A. R., Jiang, D., & McAlpine, D. (2000). Neural responses in the inferior colliculus to binaural masking level differences created by inverting the noise in one ear: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 84(2) Aug 2000, 844-852.
  • Palmer, A. R., Rees, A., & Caird, D. (1992). Binaural masking and sensitivity to interaural delay in the inferior colliculus. New York, NY: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
  • Parker, B. J. (1993). Frequency resolution in preschool children and adults using a dichotic masking paradigm: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Parker, D. J., & Thornton, A. R. (1978). Frequency specific components of the cochlear nerve and brainstem evoked responses of the human auditory system: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 7(1) 1978, 53-60.
  • Parker, R. M. (1979). An investigation of auditory temporal processing under varying conditions of distortion: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Parkinson, S. R. (1978). An alternative interpretation of the stimulus suffix effect: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory Vol 4(4) Jul 1978, 362-369.
  • Parving, A., & Elberling, C. (1982). High-pass masking in the classification of low-frequency hearing loss: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 11(3) 1982, 173-178.
  • Pastore, R. E., Harris, L. B., & Friedman, C. J. (1980). Contralateral auditory change masking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 28(5) Nov 1980, 488-491.
  • Pastore, R. E., Harris, L. B., & Goldstein, L. (1980). Auditory forward and backward masking interaction: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 28(6) Dec 1980, 547-549.
  • Pastore, R. E., & MacLatchy, C. S. (1975). Some nonmasking auditory postsignal effects: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 17(5) May 1975, 455-459.
  • Patterson, R. D. (1974). Auditory filter shape: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(4) Apr 1974, 802-809.
  • Patterson, R. D., Unoki, M., & lrino, T. (2003). Extending the domain of center frequencies for the compressive gammachirp auditory filter: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(3) Sep 2003, 1529-1542.
  • Paul, R. G., & Barry, S. J. (1972). Observations on the effect of contralateral noise on intensive differential sensitivity: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 73(5) May 1972, 379-386.
  • Pavel, M. (1980). Homogeneity in complete and partial masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Peltxman, P., Kitterman, J. A., Ostwald, P. F., & Manchester, D. (1970). Effects of incubator noise on human hearing: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 10(4) Oct 1970, 335-339.
  • Penner, M. J. (1972). Neural or energy summation in a Poisson counting model: Journal of Mathematical Psychology Vol 9(3) Aug 1972, 286-293.
  • Penner, M. J. (1975). Persistence and integration: Two consequences of a sliding integrator: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 18(2) Aug 1975, 114-120.
  • Penner, M. J. (1980). Two-tone forward masking patterns and tinnitus: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 23(4) Dec 1980, 779-786.
  • Penner, M. J. (1983). The annoyance of tinnitus and the noise required to mask it: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 26(1) Mar 1983, 73-76.
  • Penner, M. J. (1983). Variability in matches to subjective tinnitus: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 26(2) Jun 1983, 263-267.
  • Penner, M. J. (1987). Masking of tinnitus and central masking: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 30(2) Jun 1987, 147-152.
  • Penner, M. J. (1988). The effect of continuous monaural noise on loudness matches to tinnitus: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 31(1) Mar 1988, 98-102.
  • Penner, M. J., Brauth, S., & Hood, L. (1981). The temporal course of the masking of tinnitus as a basis for inferring its origin: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(2) Jan 1981, 257-261.
  • Penner, M. J., & Cudahy, E. (1973). Critical masking interval: A temporal analog of the critical band: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(6) Dec 1973, 1531-1534.
  • Penner, M. J., Cudahy, E., & Jenkins, G. W. (1974). The effect of masker duration on forward and backward masking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 15(3) Jun 1974, 405-410.
  • Penner, M. M., Robinson, C. E., & Green, D. M. (1972). The critical masking interval: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 52(6, Pt 2) Dec 1972, 1661-1668.
  • Perez-Abalo, M. C., Valdes-Sosa, M. J., Bobes, M. A., Galan, L., & et al. (1988). Different functional properties of on and off components in auditory brainstem responses to tone bursts: Audiology Vol 27(5) Sep-Oct 1988, 249-259.
  • Perkins, M. E. (1983). Partial masking in lateralization and loudness of 4000-Hz tones: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Perrott, D. R., & Briggs, R. (1973). Effects of apparent motion of the masker upon the binaural masking function: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 13(4) Oct 1973, 328-332.
  • Peters, R. W., & Hall, J. W. (1994). Comodulation masking release for elderly listeners with relatively normal audiograms: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(5, Pt 1) Nov 1994, 2674-2682.
  • Petropavlovskaya, E. A. (2005). The Effects of Direct Sequential Masking on Evoked Potentials in the Auditory Area of the Cortex in Guinea Pigs: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 35(1) Jan 2005, 49-58.
  • Petropavlovskaya, E. A., & Al'tman, Y. A. (2002). Change in Auditory Image Movement Trajectories under Conditions of Direct Nonsimultaneous Masking: Human Physiology Vol 28(5) Sep-Oct 2002, 538-546.
  • Petrosino, L., Fucci, D., Ellis, L., & Harris, D. (1989). Effects of auditory masking on lingual vibrotactile magnitude-estimation scaling responses of young children: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 68(2) Apr 1989, 523-529.
  • Petrosino, L., Fucci, D., Harris, D., & Ellis, L. (1988). Effect of auditory masking on lingual vibrotactile thresholds and magnitude estimation scaling responses: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 67(2) Oct 1988, 491-494.
  • Petrosino, L., Fucci, D., & Lowe, R. (1981). Role of the Lombard effect in lingual vibrotactile thresholds: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 52(1) Feb 1981, 263-268.
  • Phillips, D. P. (1987). Stimulus intensity and loudness recruitment: Neural correlates: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(1) Jul 1987, 1-12.
  • Phillips, D. P. (1990). Neural representation of sound amplitude in the auditory cortex: Effects of noise masking: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 37(3) Mar 1990, 197-214.
  • Phillips, D. P., & Kelly, J. B. (1992). Effects of continuous noise maskers on tone-evoked potentials in cat primary auditory cortex: Cerebral Cortex Vol 2(2) Mar-Apr 1992, 134-140.
  • Phillips, D. P., Vigneault-MacLean, B. K., Boehnke, S. E., & Hall, S. E. (2003). Acoustic Hemifields in the Spatial Release from Masking of Speech by Noise: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 14(9) Nov 2003, 518-524.
  • Picard, M., & Couture-Metz, F. (1985). Upward spread of masking in normal and impaired ears: Audiology Vol 24(2) Mar-Apr 1985, 81-91.
  • Pichora-Fuller, M. K., & Schneider, B. A. (1992). The effect of interaural delay of the masker on masking-level differences in young and old adults: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(4, Pt 1) Apr 1992, 2129-2135.
  • Pichora-Fuller, M. K., & Schneider, B. A. (1998). Masking-level differences in older adults: The effect of the level of the masking noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 60(7) Oct 1998, 1197-1205.
  • Pickles, J. O. (1976). The noradrenaline-containing innervation of the cochlear nucleus and the detection of signals in noise: Brain Research Vol 105(3) 1976, 591-596.
  • Pickles, J. O., & Comis, S. D. (1973). Role of centrifugal pathways to cochlear nucleus in detection of signals in noise: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 36(6) Nov 1973, 1131-1137.
  • Plack, C. J. (1996). Loudness enhancement and intensity discrimination under forward and backward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(2, Pt 1) Aug 1996, 1024-1030.
  • Plack, C. J. (1996). Temporal factors in referential intensity coding: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(2, Pt 1) Aug 1996, 1031-1042.
  • Plack, C. J., Carlyon, R. J., & Viemeister, N. F. (1995). Intensity discrimination under forward and backward masking: Role of referential coding: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(2) Feb 1995, 1141-1149.
  • Plack, C. J., & Drga, V. (2003). Psychophysical evidence for auditory compression at low characteristic frequencies: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(3) Mar 2003, 1574-1586.
  • Plack, C. J., & Oxenham, A. J. (1998). Basilar-membrane nonlinearity and the growth of forward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(3) Mar 1998, 1598-1608.
  • Plack, C. J., & Viemeister, N. F. (1992). The effects of notched noise on intensity discrimination under forward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(4, Pt 1) Oct 1992, 1902-1910.
  • Plack, C. J., & Viemeister, N. F. (1992). Intensity discrimination under backward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(6) Dec 1992, 3097-3101.
  • Plack, C. J., & Viemeister, N. F. (1993). Suppression and the dynamic range of hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(2) Feb 1993, 976-982.
  • Plomp, R. (1965). Detectability threshold for combination tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 37(6) 1965, 1110-1123.
  • Pohlmann, L. D., & Sorkin, R. D. (1974). Binaural masking level differences for pulse train signals of differing interaural correlation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(6) Jun 1974, 1293-1298.
  • Polich, J., Howard, L., & Starr, A. (1985). Stimulus frequency and masking as determinants of P300 latency in event-related potentials from auditory stimuli: Biological Psychology Vol 21(4) Dec 1985, 309-318.
  • Pollack, I. (1964). Interaction of forward and backward masking: Journal of Auditory Research 4(1) 1964, 63-67.
  • Pollack, I. (1973). Forward, backward and combined masking: Implications for an auditory integration period: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 25(3) Aug 1973, 424-432.
  • Pollmann, S., Maertens, M., von Cramon, D. Y., Lepsien, J., & Hugdahl, K. (2002). Dichotic listening in patients with splenial and nonsplenial callosal lesions: Neuropsychology Vol 16(1) Jan 2002, 56-64.
  • Postma, A., & Kolk, H. H. (1992). The effects of noise masking and required accuracy on speech errors, disfluencies, and self-repairs: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 35(3) Jun 1992, 537-544.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1976). Continuous noise interferes with work by masking auditory feedback and inner speech: Applied Ergonomics Vol 7(2) Jun 1976, 79-84.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1977). Continuous intense noise masks auditory feedback and inner speech: Psychological Bulletin Vol 84(5) Sep 1977, 977-1001.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1978). A new look at the effects of noise: A rejoinder: Psychological Bulletin Vol 85(5) Sep 1978, 1068-1079.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1978). A note on the masking of acoustic clicks: Applied Ergonomics Vol 9(2) Jun 1978, 103.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1980). Psychology of the scientist: XLI. Continuous noise can degrade performance when using badly designed equipment: A case history: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 50(1) Feb 1980, 319-330.
  • Poulton, E. C. (1981). Not sop Rejoinder to Hartley on masking by continuous noise: Psychological Review Vol 88(1) Jan 1981, 90-92.
  • Puleo, J. S. (1977). Contralateral cueing effects on information processing during backward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Punch, J., & Carhart, R. (1973). Influence of interaural phase on forward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(4) Oct 1973, 897-904.
  • Purdy, S. C., Houghton, J. M., Keith, W. J., & Greville, K. A. (1989). Frequency-specific auditory brainstem responses: Effective masking levels and relationship to behavioural thresholds in normal hearing adults: Audiology Vol 28(2) Mar-Apr 1989, 82-91.
  • Qin, M. K., & Oxenham, A. J. (2003). Effects of simulated cochlear-implant processing on speech reception in fluctuating maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 446-454.
  • Quaranta, A., Cassano, P., & Cervellera, G. (1978). Clinical value of the tonal masking level difference: Audiology Vol 17(3) May-Jun 1978, 232-238.
  • Quaranta, A., & Cervellera, G. (1974). Masking level difference in normal and pathological ears: Audiology Vol 13(5) Sep 1974, 428-431.
  • Quaranta, A., Salonna, I., & Bellomo, F. (1992). Psychoacoustical performance in children: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 21(4) 1992, 265-267.
  • Rakerd, B., Hartmann, W. M., & Hsu, J. (2000). Echo suppression in the horizontal and median sagittal planes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(2) Feb 2000, 1061-1064.
  • Raney, J. J. (1991). Discrimination of changes in complex auditory stimuli: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Raney, J. J., Richards, V. M., Onsan, Z. A., & Green, D. M. (1989). Signal uncertainty and psychometric functions in profile analysis: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(3) Sep 1989, 954-960.
  • Rankovic, C. M. (1997). Prediction of speech reception for listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Rankovic, C. M., Freyman, R. L., & Zurek, P. M. (1992). Potential benefits of adaptive frequency-gain characteristics for speech reception in noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 354-362.
  • Rappold, P. W. (1992). An investigation of the contribution of auditory-grouping to the comodulation masking release effect: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Raslear, T. G. (1989). Discriminability, loudness, and masking in the rat (Rattus norvegicus): A confirmation and extension: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 103(3) Sep 1989, 289-296.
  • Raslear, T. G., Pierrel-Sorrentino, R., & Rudnick, F. (1983). Loudness scaling and masking in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 97(3) Jun 1983, 392-398.
  • Raslear, T. G., Shurtleff, D., & Simmons, L. (1992). Loudness bisection and masking in the rat (Rattus norvegicus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 106(4) Dec 1992, 374-382.
  • Rastatter, M., & Rivers, C. (1983). The effects of short-term auditory masking on fundamental frequency variability: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 23(1) Jan 1983, 33-42.
  • Ratnam, R., & Feng, A. S. (1998). Detection of auditory signals by frog inferior collicular neurons in the presence of spatially separated noise: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 80(6) Dec 1998, 2848-2859.
  • Raz, N., Millman, D., & Moberg, P. J. (1990). Mechanism of age-related differences in frequency discrimination with backward masking: Speed of processing or stimulus persistence? : Psychology and Aging Vol 5(4) Dec 1990, 475-481.
  • Raz, N., Willerman, L., Ingmundson, P., & Hanlon, M. (1983). Aptitude-related differences in auditory recognition masking: Intelligence Vol 7(2) Apr-Jun 1983, 71-90.
  • Raz, N., Willerman, L., & Yama, M. (1987). On sense and senses: Intelligence and auditory information processing: Personality and Individual Differences Vol 8(2) 1987, 201-210.
  • Reed, C. M., & Bilger, R. C. (1973). A comparative study of S/N0 and E/N0: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 53(4) Apr 1973, 1039-1044.
  • Reeves, A. (1982). Metacontrast U-shaped functions derive from two monotonic processes: Perception Vol 11(4) 1982, 415-426.
  • Relkin, E. M., & Turner, C. W. (1988). A reexamination of forward masking in the auditory nerve: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(2) Aug 1988, 584-591.
  • Renouf, D. (1980). Masked hearing thresholds of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) in air: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 20(4) Oct 1980, 263-269.
  • Repp, B. H., & Frost, R. (1988). Detectability of words and nonwords in two kinds of noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(5) Nov 1988, 1929-1932.
  • Revoile, S., Pickett, J. M., & Wilson, M. P. (1981). Masking of noise bursts by an adjacent vowel for hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(4) Dec 1981, 576-579.
  • Richards, A. M. (1973). Loudness growth under masking: Relation to true sensorineural impairment: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 16(4) Dec 1973, 597-607.
  • Richards, A. M. (1977). Loudness perception for short-duration tones in masking noise: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 20(4) Dec 1977, 684-693.
  • Richards, V. M. (1994). Experiments related to the detection of a tone masked by another tone: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(6) Jun 1994, 3481-3498.
  • Richards, V. M., Buss, E., & Tian, L. (1997). Effects of modulator phase for comodulation masking release and modulation detection interference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(1) Jul 1997, 468-476.
  • Richards, V. M., Buss, E., & Tian, L. (1998). The effects of different patterns and uncertainty for the detection of a tone added to SAM complex tonal maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(2) Feb 1998, 1058-1066.
  • Richards, V. M., Tang, Z., & Kidd, G. D., Jr. (2002). Informational masking with small set sizes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(3) Mar 2002, 1359-1366.
  • Richardson, L. M., Busby, P. A., & Clark, G. M. (1998). Modulation detection interference in cochlear implant subjects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 104(1) Jul 1998, 442-452.
  • Riensche, L. L., Beasley, D. S., & Lamb, L. E. (1983). Adults' item and order errors on sequences of time-compressed rhyming words: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 23(2) Apr 1983, 95-100.
  • Riensche, L. L., Stahl, C., Cravens, C., & Lamb, L. (1983). Children's item and order errors on time-compressed, sequenced rhyming words: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 23(1) Jan 1983, 1-8.
  • Ries, D. T. (2002). Temporal-masking patterns of ramped- and damped maskers and their relation to subjective duration and loudness. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Rivers, C. L. (1985). The effects of short-term auditory masking and multitalker noise on fundamental frequency variability in the voices of children and adults: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Rivers, C. L., & Rastatter, M. P. (1985). The effects of multitalker and masker noise on fundamental frequency variability during spontaneous speech for children and adults: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 25(1) Jan 1985, 37-45.
  • Rizzo, S. R. (1981). Effects of backward masking on human auditory brainstem responses: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Roberts, B., & Moore, B. C. (1990). The influence of extraneous sounds on the perceptual estimation of first-formant frequency in vowels: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(6) Dec 1990, 2571-2583.
  • Robinette, M. S., & Brey, R. H. (1984). Real-ear masking calibration for portable pure-tone audiometers: Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools Vol 15(4) Oct 1984, 289-294.
  • Robinson, C. E., & Pollack, I. (1973). Interaction between forward and backward masking: A measure of the integrating period of the auditory system: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 53(5) May 1973, 1313-1316.
  • Robinson, D., & Dolan, T. R. (1972). Effect of signal frequency on the MLD for uncorrelated noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1945-1946.
  • Robinson, D. E., Langford, T. L., & Yost, W. A. (1974). Masking of tones by tones and of noise by noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 15(1) Feb 1974, 159-167.
  • Robinson, D. E., & Trahiotis, C. (1972). Effects of signal duration and masker duration on detectability under diotic and dichotic listening conditions: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 12(4) Oct 1972, 333-334.
  • Robinson, D. N., & Sabat, S. R. (1975). Elimination of auditory evoked responses during auditory shadowing: Physiological Psychology Vol 3(1) Mar 1975, 26-28.
  • Rose, M. M., & Moore, B. C. J. (1997). Perceptual grouping of tone sequences by normally hearing and hearing-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(3) Sep 1997, 1768-1778.
  • Rosen, S., & Baker, R. J. (1994). Acoustic reflexes in the measurement of auditory filters at high levels in normal listeners: Audiology Vol 33(1) Jan-Feb 1994, 37-46.
  • Rosen, S., Baker, R. J., & Darling, A. (1998). Auditory filter nonlinearity at 2 kHz in normal hearing listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(5, Pt 1) May 1998, 2539-2550.
  • Rosen, S., & Eva, M. (2001). Is there a relationship between speech and nonspeech auditory processing in children with dyslexia? : Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 44(4) Aug 2001, 720-736.
  • Rosen, S., & Stock, D. (1992). Auditory filter bandwidths as a function of level at low frequencies (125 Hz-2 kHz): Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(2, Pt 1) Aug 1992, 773-781.
  • Rosen, S., & Stock, D. (1992). "Auditory filter bandwidths as a function of level at low frequencies (125 Hz-2 kHz)": Erratum: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(6) Dec 1992, 3438.
  • Rosenberg, A. E. (1965). Effect of masking on the pitch of periodic pulses: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38(5) 1965, 747-758.
  • Rosenhamer, H., & Holmkvist, C. (1983). Will contralateral white noise interfere with the monaurally click-evoked brainstem response? : Scandinavian Audiology Vol 12(1) 1983, 11-14.
  • Rotter, M. A. (1982). Variables related to speech discrimination in competition for normal hearing subjects: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Roush, W. J. (1982). Binaural fusion, masking level differences, and auditory brainstem responses in children with auditory processing disorders: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Saberi, K., Dostal, L., Sadralodabai, T., Bull, V., & et al. (1991). Free-field release from masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 90(3) Sep 1991, 1355-1370.
  • Sach, A. J., & Bailey, P. J. (2004). Some characteristics of auditory spatial attention revealed using rhythmic masking release: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 66(8) Nov 2004, 1379-1387.
  • Sadler, M. E. (1990). Effects of noise and amplitude upon the rate and nature of verbal transformations: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Sagharichi, M., & Fulton, R. T. (1983). Effect of a contralateral masking stimulus on auditory response performance: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 23(2) Apr 1983, 77-93.
  • Sagharichi, M., & Fulton, R. T. (1987). Effect of a contralateral masking stimulus on auditory response performance: Hearing Aid Journal Vol 5(2) Jan 1987, 37-47, 50-51.
  • Saldana, H. M. (1995). Auditory form based priming: The role of voice and phonological information. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Salisbury, D. F., Desantis, M. A., Shenton, M. E., & McCarley, R. W. (2002). The effect of background noise on P300 to suprathreshold stimuli: Psychophysiology Vol 39(1) Jan 2002, 111-115.
  • Salo, S. K., Lang, A. H., & Salmivalli, A. J. (1995). Effect of contralateral white noise masking on the mismatch negativity: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 24(3) 1995, 165-173.
  • Salo, S. K., Lang, A. H., Salmivalli, A. J., Johansson, R. K., & Peltola, M. S. (2003). Contralateral White Noise Masking Affects Auditory N1 and P2 Waves Differently: Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 17(4) 2003, 189-194.
  • Sandor, P. M. B., Pellieux, L., Godfroy, M., Ossard, G., & Dancer, A. (2004). Hearing Thresholds During Gz Acceleration with Masking Noise: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 75(11) Nov 2004, 952-955.
  • Sankeralli, M. J., & Mullen, K. T. (2001). Bipolar or rectified chromatic detection mechanisms? : Visual Neuroscience Vol 18(1) Jan-Feb 2001, 127-135.
  • Sasaki, T., Miyazaki, K., & Maruyama, K. (1979). Comparisons of forward and backward masking patterns and pulsation threshold patterns: Tohoku Psychologica Folia Vol 38(1-4) 1979, 36-43.
  • Saunders, J. C., Bock, G. R., & Fahrbach, S. E. (1978). Frequency selectivity in the parakeet (Melopsittacus undulatus) studied with narrow-band noise masking: Sensory Processes Vol 2(2) Jun 1978, 80-89.
  • Saunders, J. C., Else, P. V., & Bock, G. R. (1978). Frequency selectivity in the parakeet (Melopsittacus undulatus) studied with psychophysical tuning curves: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 92(3) Jun 1978, 406-415.
  • Saunders, S. S., & Salvi, R. J. (1995). Pure tone masking patterns in adult chickens before and after recovery from acoustic trauma: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(3) Sep 1995, 1365-1371.
  • Savel, S., & Bacon, S. P. (2003). Effectiveness of narrow-band versus tonal off-frequency maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 380-385.
  • Schacknow, P. N. (1976). The effect of noise bandwidth on auditory intensity discrimination: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Schairer, K. S., Nizami, L., Reimer, J. F., & Jesteadt, W. (2003). Effects of peripheral nonlinearity on psychometric functions for forward-masked tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(3) Mar 2003, 1560-1573.
  • Scharf, B. (1961). Complex sounds and critical bands: Psychological Bulletin Vol 58(3) May 1961, 205-217.
  • Scharf, B., Florentine, M., & Meiselman, C. H. (1976). Critical band in auditory lateralization: Sensory Processes Vol 1(2) Dec 1976, 109-126.
  • Schlauch, R. S., Clement, B. R., Ries, D. T., & DiGiovanni, J. J. (1999). Masker laterality and cueing in forward-masked intensity discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(2, Pt 1) Feb 1999, 822-828.
  • Schlauch, R. S., Lanthier, N., & Neve, J. (1997). Forward-masked intensity discrimination: Duration effects and spectral effects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(1) Jul 1997, 461-467.
  • Schlundt, C. E., Finneran, J. J., Carder, D. A., & Ridgway, S. H. (2000). Temporary shift in masked hearing thresholds of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, and white whales, Delphinapterus leucas, after exposure to intense tones: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(6) Jun 2000, 3496-3508.
  • Schmidt, S., & Zwicker, E. (1991). The effect of masker spectral asymmetry on overshoot in simultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(3) Mar 1991, 1324-1330.
  • Schneider, B. A., Bull, D., & Trehub, S. E. (1988). Binaural unmasking in infants: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(3) Mar 1988, 1124-1132.
  • Schneider, B. A., Morrongiello, B. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1990). Size of critical band in infants, children, and adults: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 16(3) Aug 1990, 642-652.
  • Schneider, B. A., Trehub, S. E., & Bull, D. (1979). The development of basic auditory processes in infants: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 33(4) Dec 1979, 306-319.
  • Schneider, B. A., Trehub, S. E., Morrongiello, B. A., & Thorpe, L. A. (1989). Developmental changes in masked thresholds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(5) Nov 1989, 1733-1742.
  • Schneider, B. A., & Zurek, P. M. (1989). Lateralization of coherent and incoherent targets added to a diotic background: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(5) Nov 1989, 1756-1763.
  • Schoeny, Z. G., Goetzinger, C. P., & Knox, A. W. (1965). Some effects of bone-conducted masking: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 8(3) 1965, 253-261.
  • Schooneveldt, G. P., & Moore, B. C. (1987). Comodulation masking release (CMR): Effects of signal frequency, flanking-band frequency, masker bandwidth, flanking-band level, and monotic versus dichotic presentation of the flanking band: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(6) Dec 1987, 1944-1956.
  • Schooneveldt, G. P., & Moore, B. C. (1988). Failure to obtain comodulation masking release with frequency-modulated maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(6) Jun 1988, 2290-2292.
  • Schooneveldt, G. P., & Moore, B. C. (1989). Comodulation masking release (CMR) as a function of masker bandwidth, modulator bandwidth, and signal duration: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(1) Jan 1989, 273-281.
  • Schooneveldt, G. P., & Moore, B. C. (1989). Comodulation masking release for various monaural and binaural combinations of the signal, on-frequency, and flanking bands: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 85(1) Jan 1989, 262-272.
  • Schroger, E., Tervaniemi, M., Wolff, C., & Naatanen, R. N. (1996). Preattentive periodicity detection in auditory patterns as governed by time and intensity information: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 4(2) Sep 1996, 145-148.
  • Schwent, V. L., Hillyard, S. A., & Galabos, R. (1976). Selective attention and the auditory vertex potential: II. Effects of signal intensity and masking noise: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 40(6) Jun 1976, 615-622.
  • Seaton, W. H. (1977). The effects of masker intensity level on estimates of critical bandwidth: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Seaton, W. H. (1979). Level dependence of critical bandwidth: Notched-noise masking paradigm: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 19(4) Oct 1979, 267-276.
  • Sek, A., Alcantara, J., Moore, B. C. J., Kluk, K., & Wicher, A. (2005). Development of a fast method for determining psychophysical tuning curves: International Journal of Audiology Vol 44(7) Jul 2005, 408-420.
  • Selters, W. A. (1974). Masked thresholds in normal and impaired ears: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Semela, J. J. (1993). An investigation of wave I' of the brain-stem auditory evoked potential: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Shailer, M. J., Moore, B. C., Glasberg, B. R., Watson, N., & et al. (1990). Auditory filter shapes at 8 and 10 kHz: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(1) Jul 1990, 141-148.
  • Shailer, M. J., & Moore, B. C. J. (1994). Influence of relative level on modulation discrimination interference: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(1) Jul 1994, 583-585.
  • Shakhshaev, S. A. (1991). Binaural release from masking of two-tone complexes: Sensory Systems Vol 5(2) Apr-Jun 1991, 131-133.
  • Shannon, R. V. (1975). Suppression of forward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Shearer, W. M. (1978). Self-masking effects from live and recorded vowels: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 18(3) Jul 1978, 213-219.
  • Sheft, S., & Yost, W. A. (1997). Modulation detection interference with two-component masker modulators: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(2, Pt 1) Aug 1997, 1106-1112.
  • Shepherd, W. T., & Gunn, W. J. (1977). Speech interference assessment: A condensed view of important considerations and a global model of acceptability: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 17(2) Apr 1977, 117-138.
  • Sherrard, C. A. (1975). Stuttering as "false alarm" responding: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 10(2) Oct 1975, 83-91.
  • Shilling, R. D. (1993). Backward detection and discrimination unmasking: Suppression or cueing? : Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Shilling, R. D., & Soderquist, D. R. (1990). Auditory backward recognition masking: Effect of interaural phase on masker efficacy: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 47(4) Apr 1990, 332-336.
  • Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., Schickler, J., Kopco, N., & Litovsky, R. (2001). Spatial unmasking of nearby speech sources in a simulated anechoic environment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 110(2) Aug 2001, 1118-1129.
  • Shuster, L. I. (1990). Motor-motor adaptation to speech: Further investigations: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 71(1) Aug 1990, 275-280.
  • Siegel, G. M., Pick, H. L., Olsen, M. G., & Sawin, L. (1976). Auditory feedback on the regulation of vocal intensity of preschool children: Developmental Psychology Vol 12(3) May 1976, 255-261.
  • Siegel, R. A., & Colburn, H. S. (1989). Binaural processing of noisy stimuli: Internal/external noise ratios for diotic and dichotic stimuli: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(6) Dec 1989, 2122-2128.
  • Silver, J. (1977). The effects of transcendental meditation and masker uncertainty on the detection of a masked psychoacoustic signal: A selective attention theoretical approach: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Silverman, E. B. (1976). The recognition of three critical features of non-speech sounds in a backward masking task: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Silverman, F. H., & Trotter, W. D. (1973). Bibliography: Literature related to use of instrumental aids in stuttering therapy: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 36(1) Feb 1973, 247-251.
  • Silverman, F. H., & Trotter, W. D. (1974). Bibliography related to use of instrumental aids in stuttering therapy: Supplement 2: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 38(3, Pt 2) Jun 1974, 1329-1330.
  • Simmons, A. M. (1988). Masking patterns in the bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana): I. Behavioral effects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(3) Mar 1988, 1087-1092.
  • Simmons, J. A., Freedman, E. G., Stevenson, S. B., Chen, L., & et al. (1989). Clutter interference and the integration time of echoes in the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(4) Oct 1989, 1318-1332.
  • Simmons, J. A., Kick, S. A., Moffat, A. J., Masters, W. M., & et al. (1988). Clutter interference along the target range axis in the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(2) Aug 1988, 551-559.
  • Simon, G. R. (1964). The senorineural acuity level (SAL) technique with narrow-band noise: Journal of Auditory Research 4(3) 1964, 191-194.
  • Sims, N. R. (1982). The curvature problem in intramodality and cross-modality matching functions of loudness and softness: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Sinex, D. G., & Havey, D. C. (1986). Neural mechanisms of tone-on-tone masking: Patterns of discharge rate and discharge synchrony related to rates of spontaneous discharge in the chinchilla auditory nerve: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 56(6) Dec 1986, 1763-1780.
  • Smith, D. W., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. C. (1987). Effects of changes in absolute signal level on psychophysical tuning curves in quiet and noise in patas monkeys: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(1) Jul 1987, 63-68.
  • Smith, D. W., Turner, D. A., & Henson, M. M. (2000). Psychophysical correlates of contralateral efferent suppression: I. The role of the medial olivocochlear system in "central masking" in nonhuman primates: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 107(2) Feb 2000, 933-941.
  • Smith, M. R., Cutler, A., Butterfield, S., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1989). The perception of rhythm and word boundaries in noise-masked speech: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 32(4) Dec 1989, 912-920.
  • Smits, J. T., & Duifhuis, H. (1982). Masking and partial masking in listeners with a high-frequency hearing loss: Audiology Vol 21(4) Jul-Aug 1982, 310-324.
  • Smyth, V. (1985). On the effect of cross-hearing and clinical masking on the auditory brain-stem evoked response: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 61(1) Jul 1985, 26-29.
  • Snashall, S. E. (1974). The effect of contralateral masking on tests of auditory adaptation at threshold: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 3(4) 1974, 159-169.
  • Snell, K. B., & Hu, H.-L. (1999). The effect of temporal placement on gap detectability: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(6) Dec 1999, 3571-3577.
  • Snell, K. B., Mapes, F. M., Hickman, E. D., & Frisina, D. R. (2002). Word recognition in competing babble and the effects of age, temporal processing, and absolute sensitivity: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(2) Aug 2002, 720-727.
  • Sockalingam, R., Dunphy, L., Nam, K.-E., & Gulliver, M. (2007). Effectiveness of frequency-matched masking and residual inhibition in tinnitus therapy: A preliminary study: Audiological Medicine Vol 5(2) 2007, 92-102.
  • Soderquist, D. R., Carstens, A. A., & Frank, G. J. (1981). Backward, simultaneous, and forward masking as a function of signal delay and frequency: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 21(4) Oct 1981, 227-245.
  • Soderquist, D. R., & Lindsey, J. W. (1970). Masking-level differences as a function of noise spectrum level, frequency, and signal duration: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 10(3) Jul 1970, 276-282.
  • Soderquist, D. R., & Shilling, R. D. (1990). Loudness and the binaural masking level difference: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 28(6) Nov 1990, 553-555.
  • Soderquist, D. R., & Wilkes, M. R. (1980). Unmasking in loudness matching and in forward-masking paradigms: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 20(3) Jul 1980, 205-215.
  • Sommers, M. S., & Gehr, S. E. (1998). Auditory suppression and frequency selectivity in older and younger adults: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(2) Feb 1998, 1067-1074.
  • Sonn, M. (1974). Effects of masking-spectrum slope and interaural phase on detection of tones: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 38(3, Pt 1) Jun 1974, 775-784.
  • Sorin, C. (1987). Psychophysical representation of stop consonant and temporal masking in speech. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff Publishing.
  • Sorkin, R. D., Pastore, R. E., & Pohlmann, L. D. (1972). Simultaneous two-channel signal detection: II. Correlated and uncorrelated signals: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1960-1965.
  • Southall, B. L., Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (2000). Masking in three pinnipeds: Underwater, low-frequency critical ratios: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(3,Pt1)) Sep 2000, 1322-1331.
  • Southall, B. L., Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (2003). Auditory masking in three pinnipeds: Aerial critical ratios and direct critical bandwidth measurements: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(3) Sep 2003, 1660-1666.
  • Souza, P. E., Tremblay, K. L., & Boike, K. T. (2003). Effects of Decreased Audibility Produced by High-Pass Maskers in Younger and Older Adults: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 14(8) Oct 2003, 427-433.
  • Souza, P. E., & Turner, C. W. (1994). Masking of speech in young and elderly listeners with hearing loss: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 37(3) Jun 1994, 655-661.
  • Spetner, N. B. (1989). Auditory frequency resolution in human infancy with pulsation threshold masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Spiegel, M. (1988). Speech masking: II. Simultaneous masking thresholds under "naturalistic" listening conditions: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(6) Dec 1988, 2033-2044.
  • Spiegel, M. F. (1987). Speech masking: I. Simultaneous and nonsimultaneous masking within stop /d/ and flap /r/ closures: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1492-1502.
  • Spitzer, J. B., Goldstein, B. A., Salzbrenner, L. G., & Mueller, G. (1983). Effect of tinnitus masker noise on speech discrimination in quiet and two noise backgrounds: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 12(3) 1983, 197-200.
  • Springer, S. P. (1973). Hemispheric specialization for speech opposed by contralateral noise: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 13(3) Jun 1973, 391-393.
  • Stack, J. W. (1995). The perception of stop consonants in silent-center syllables by listeners with high-frequency hearing loss. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Staffel, J. G., Hall, J. W., Grose, J. H., & Pillsbury, H. C. (1990). NoSo and NoS!p detection as a function of masker bandwidth in normal-hearing and cochlear-impaired listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(4) Apr 1990, 1720-1727.
  • Stanny, R. R. (1981). The nonlinear perception of sound intensities: Time-intensity trades and the interactions of forward and backward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Stanny, R. R., & Elfner, L. F. (1980). An "inhibitory" influence on brainstem population responses: Science Vol 208(4442) Apr 1980, 418-419.
  • Stark, E. W., & Borton, T. E. (1975). Noise-excluding enclosure for audiometry: Audiology Vol 14(3) 1975, 232-237.
  • Steiger, H. (1985). Influences of sequential organization processes on the use of binaural cues: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Stephen, S. C., & Haggard, M. P. (1980). Acoustic properties of masking/delayed feedback in the fluency of stutterers and controls: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 23(3) Sep 1980, 527-538.
  • Stephens, S. D. (1975). Contralateral noise and auditory temporal summation: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 4(2) 1975, 113-118.
  • Stevens, S. S. (1966). Power-group transformations under glare, masking, and recruitment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 39(4) 1966, 725-735.
  • Stewart, M. G. (1984). The effects of competing message semantic content on SSI performance: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Stillman, J. A., & Irwin, R. J. (1990). Signal detectability in the presence of monotic or dichotic noise bands of equal or unequal levels: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 47(3) Mar 1990, 281-290.
  • Strickland, E. A., Viemeister, N. F., Van Tasell, D. J., & Preminger, J. E. (1994). Is useful speech information carried by fibers with high characteristic frequencies? : Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(1) Jan 1994, 497-501.
  • Stuart, A., & Phillips, D. P. (1997). Word recognition in continuous noise, interrupted noise, and in quiet by normal-hearing listeners at two sensation levels: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 26(2) 1997, 112-116.
  • Stubblefield, J. H. (1975). A test-retest reliability study on clinical measurement of masking level differences: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Stubblefield, J. H., & Goldstein, D. P. (1977). A test-retest reliability study on clinical measurement of masking level differences: Audiology Vol 16(5) 1977, 419-431.
  • Studebaker, G. A. (1964). Clinical masking of air- and bone-conducted stimuli: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders 29(1) 1964, 23-35.
  • Studebaker, G. A., Gilmore, C., & Sherbecoe, R. L. (1993). Performance-intensity functions at absolute and masked thresholds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 93(6) Jun 1993, 3418-3421.
  • Studebaker, G. A., & Wark, D. J. (1972). An alternative interpretation of the continuous tone masking test: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 12(4) Oct 1972, 297-301.
  • Summers, V. (2000). Effects of hearing impairment and presentation level on masking period patterns for Schroeder-phase harmonic complexes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 108(5,Pt1) Nov 2000, 2307-2317.
  • Summers, V., & Molis, M. R. (2004). Speech Recognition in Fluctuating and Continuous Maskers: Effects of Hearing Loss and Presentation Level: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 47(2) Apr 2004, 245-256.
  • Supin, A. Y. (2005). Rippled-Spectrum Resolution as a Measure of Frequency Resolving Power of Hearing. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co.
  • Sweetow, R. W. (1977). Temporal and spread of masking effects from extended low frequency amplification: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 17(3) Jul 1977, 161-170.
  • Sweitzer, R. S. (1973). Contralateral threshold shifts produced by low level pure tone maskers of varying durations: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Takeuchi, A. H., & Braida, L. D. (1995). Recognition of amplitude-modulation patterns in the presence of a distractor: II. Effects of dichotic presentation and unmodulated distractors: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(1) Jul 1995, 142-147.
  • Tang, Z., & Richards, V. M. (2003). Examination of a linear model in an informational masking study: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 361-367.
  • Tanis, D. C., & Teas, D. C. (1974). Evoked potential correlates of interaural phase reversals: Audiology Vol 13(5) Sep 1974, 357-365.
  • Tavolga, W. N. (1974). Signal/noise ratio and the critical band in fishes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 55(6) Jun 1974, 1323-1333.
  • Taylor, R. (1992). An investigation of the effects of audibility spectrum on speech recognition performance-intensity functions: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Thibodeau, L. M. (1991). Performance of hearing-impaired persons on auditory enhancement tasks: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 89(6) Jun 1991, 2843-2850.
  • Thoms, J. B. (1996). Binaural masking-level difference for detection and discrimination of complex tones. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Tillman, T. W., Carhart, R., & Nicholls, S. (1973). Release from multiple maskers in elderly persons: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 16(1) Mar 1973, 152-160.
  • Tolkmitt, F. J. (1974). Latency of sound localization as a function of azimuth and frequency: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 103(2) Aug 1974, 310-316.
  • Trahiotis, C., & Bernstein, L. R. (1990). Detectability of interaural delays over select spectral regions: Effects of flanking noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(2) Feb 1990, 810-813.
  • Trahiotis, C., Dolan, T. R., & Miller, T. H. (1972). Effect of "backward" masker fringe on the detectability of pulsed diotic and dichotic tonal signals: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 12(4) Oct 1972, 335-338.
  • Trees, D. E., & Turner, C. W. (1986). Spread of masking in normal subjects and in subjects with high-frequency hearing loss: Audiology Vol 25(2) Mar-Apr 1986, 70-83.
  • Trehub, S. E., Bull, D., & Schneider, B. A. (1981). Infants' detection of speech in noise: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 24(2) Jan 1981, 202-206.
  • Tubbs, R. L. (1983). The effects of intense noise and masking on pure-tone detection in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Tun, P. A., O'Kane, G., & Wingfield, A. (2002). Distraction by competing speech in young and older adult listeners: Psychology and Aging Vol 17(3) Sep 2002, 453-467.
  • Tun, P. A., & Wingfield, A. (1999). One voice too many: Adult age differences in language processing with different types of distracting sounds: Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences Vol 54B(5) Sep 1999, P317-P327.
  • Turgeon, M. (2002). Cross-spectral auditory grouping using the paradigm of rhythmic masking release. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Turgeon, M., Bregman, A. S., & Ahad, P. A. (2002). Rhythmic masking release: Contribution of cues for perceptual organization to the cross-spectral fusion of concurrent narrow-band noises: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(4) Apr 2002, 1819-1831.
  • Turgeon, M., Bregman, A. S., & Roberts, B. (2005). Rhythmic Masking Release: Effects of Asynchrony, Temporal Overlap, Harmonic Relations, and Source Separation on Cross-Spectral Grouping: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Vol 31(5) Oct 2005, 939-953.
  • Turl, C. W., Penner, R. H., & Au, W. W. (1987). Comparison of target detection capabilities of the beluga and bottlenose dolphin: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1487-1491.
  • Turner, C. W., & Doherty, K. A. (1997). Temporal masking and the "active process" in normal and hearing-impaired listeners. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Turner, C. W., Horwitz, A. R., & Souza, P. E. (1994). Forward- and backward-masked intensity discrimination measured using forced-choice and adjustment procedures: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(4) Oct 1994, 2121-2126.
  • Turner, C. W., Relkin, E. M., & Doucet, J. (1994). Psychophysical and physiological forward masking studies: Probe duration and rise-time effects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(2, Pt 1) Aug 1994, 795-800.
  • Turner, C. W., Zeng, F.-g., Relkin, E. M., & Horwitz, A. R. (1992). Frequency discrimination in forward and backward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(6) Dec 1992, 3102-3108.
  • Turner, R. G. (2004). Masking Redux I: An Optimized Masking Method: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 15(1) Jan 2004, 17-28.
  • Turner, R. G. (2004). Masking Redux II: A Recommended Masking Protocol: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 15(1) Jan 2004, 29-46.
  • Tyler, R. S. (1979). Unmasking effects produced by combination tones: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Tyler, R. S. (1988). The auditory representation of vowels as inferred from psychoacoustical masking patterns: Journal of Phonetics Vol 16(1) Jan 1988, 125-137.
  • Tyler, R. S., & Conrad-Armes, D. (1983). The determination of tinnitus loudness considering the effects of recruitment: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 26(1) Mar 1983, 59-72.
  • Tyler, R. S., Conrad-Armes, D., & Smith, P. A. (1984). Postmasking effects of sensorineural tinnitus: A preliminary investigation: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 27(3) Sep 1984, 466-474.
  • Tyler, R. S., Fernandes, M. A., & Wood, E. J. (1982). Masking of pure tones by broad-band noise in cochlear-impaired listeners: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(1) Mar 1982, 117-124.
  • Tzuo, P.-f. L. (1981). Effects of duration of fringe of noise and fringe of silence on detection of brief signals masked by noise: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Underwood, G., & Swain, R. A. (1973). Selectivity of attention and the perception of duration: Perception Vol 2(1) 1973, 101-105.
  • Vachon, F., & Tremblay, S. (2006). Delayed Masking and the Auditory Attentional Blink: A Test for Retrieval Competition and Bottleneck Models: Experimental Psychology Vol 53(3) 2006, 182-190.
  • Vaitulevich, S. F., Kal'nitskaya, M. A., & Pak, S. P. (1997). The long-latency auditory potentials in humans evoked by successive paired clicks: Human Physiology Vol 23(5) Sep-Oct 1997, 536-541.
  • Vaitulevich, S. F., & Mal'tseva, N. F. (1987). Reflection of the effect of binaural release from masking in long-latency human auditory evoked potentials: Human Physiology Vol 13(2) Mar-Apr 1987, 86-90.
  • van de Par, S., & Kohlrausch, A. (1997). A new approach to comparing binaural masking level differences at low and high frequencies: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(3) Mar 1997, 1671-1680.
  • van de Par, S., & Kohlrausch, A. (1998). Comparison of monaural (CMR) and binaural (BMLD) masking release: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(3) Mar 1998, 1573-1579.
  • van de Par, S., & Kohlrausch, A. (1998). Diotic and dichotic detection using multiplied-noise maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2100-2110.
  • van de Par, S., & Kohlrausch, A. (1999). Dependence of binaural masking level differences on center frequency, masker bandwidth, and interaural parameters: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 106(4, Pt 1) Oct 1999, 1940-1947.
  • Van Den Brink, G. (1964). Detection of tone pulse of various durations in noise of various bandwidths: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 36(6) 1964, 1206-1211.
  • Van den Brink, W. A., & Houtgast, T. (1990). Efficient across-frequency integration in short-signal detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 87(1) Jan 1990, 284-291.
  • Van den Brink, W. A., Houtgast, T., & Smoorenburg, G. F. (1992). Signal detection in temporally modulated and spectrally shaped maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 267-278.
  • van der Heijden, M., & Kohlrausch, A. (1995). The role of distortion products in masking by single brands of noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(6) Dec 1995, 3125-3134.
  • van der Heijden, M., & Kohlrausch, A. (1995). The role of envelope fluctuations in spectral masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 97(3) Mar 1995, 1800-1807.
  • van der Heijden, M., & Trahiotis, C. (1997). A new way to account for binaural detection as a function of interaural noise correlation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 101(2) Feb 1997, 1019-1022.
  • van der Heijden, M., & Trahiotis, C. (1998). Binaural detection as a function of interaural correlation and bandwidth of masking noise: Implications for estimates of spectral resolution: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(3) Mar 1998, 1609-1614.
  • van der Heijden, M., & Trahiotis, C. (1999). Masking with interaurally delayed stimuli: The use of "internal" delays in binaural detection: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(1) Jan 1999, 388-399.
  • van der Heijden, M., Trahiotis, C., Kohlrausch, A., & van de Par, S. (1997). Binaural detection with spectrally nonoverlapping signals and maskers: Evidence for masking by aural distortion products: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 102(5, Pt 1) Nov 1997, 2966-2972.
  • Van der Laan, K. T., & Wit, H. P. (1983). Predictive value of directional sensitivity diagrams of hearing aids for word discrimination in noise: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 12(1) 1983, 57-61.
  • Van der Vlugt, M. J., & Nooteboom, S. G. (1986). Auditory word recognition is not more sensitive to word-initial than to word-final stimulus information: IPO Annual Progress Report No 21 1986, 41-49.
  • Van Summers, W., Pisoni, D. B., Bernacki, R. H., Pedlow, R. I., & et al. (1988). Effects of noise on speech production: Acoustic and perceptual analyses: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(3) Sep 1988, 917-928.
  • van Wijngaarden, S. J., Steeneken, H. J. M., & Houtgast, T. (2002). Quantifying the intelligibility of speech in noise for non-native talkers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 112(6) Dec 2002, 3004-3013.
  • Van Zanten, G. A., & Brocaar, M. P. (1984). Frequency-specific auditory brainstem responses to clicks masked by notched noise: Audiology Vol 23(3) May 1984, 253-264.
  • Vartanyan, I. A., & Egorova, M. A. (1990). Focused ultrasound perception against noisy background: Sensory Systems Vol 4(2), Trans Apr-Jun 1990, 147-151.
  • Vartanyan, I. A., & Egorova, M. A. (1990). Masking of single and rhythmic acoustic signals with noises of varying spectral composition: Sensory Systems Vol 4(4) Oct-Dec 1990, 329-334.
  • Vartanyan, I. A., & Egorova, M. A. (1992). Noise masking of acoustical impulses with various pathways of signal and masking to the aural receptors: Sensory Systems Vol 6(1) Jan-Mar 1992, 37-41.
  • Velde, T. M. (1992). Auditory filter bandwidth: Comparison of frequency-based and temporal-based estimates: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Veloso, K., Hall, J. W., & Grose, J. H. (1990). Frequency selectivity and comodulation masking release in adults and in 6-year-old children: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 33(1) Mar 1990, 96-102.
  • Verhey, J. L. (1999). Psychoacoustics of spectro-temporal effects in masking and loudness perception. Oldenburg, Germany: Bibliotheks- und Informationssystem der Universitat Oldenburg.
  • Versfeld, N. J., & Dreschler, W. A. (2002). The relationship between the intelligibility of time-compressed speech and speech in noise in young and elderly listeners: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(1,Pt1) Jan 2002, 401-408.
  • Viemeister, N. F. (1983). Auditory intensity discrimination at high frequencies in the presence of noise: Science Vol 221(4616) Sep 1983, 1206-1208.
  • Villeponteaux, L. A. (1972). The effect of auditory occlusion on TAT responses of institutionalized delinquent, adolescent boys: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Voeks, S. K. (1985). Auditory comprehension and backward recognition masking in aphasic adults: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • von Klitzing, R., & Kohlrausch, A. (1994). Effect of masker level on overshoot in running- and frozen-noise maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(4) Apr 1994, 2192-2201.
  • Voroba, B. (1973). Binaural masked thresholds with reproducible noise bursts: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Wakeford, O. S. (1973). Binaural processing in the cat: Lateralization and detection: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Waltzman, S. B., & Levitt, H. (1978). Backward and forward masking with reproducible noise bursts: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 18(4) Oct 1978, 251-264.
  • Wang, M. D., & Bilger, R. C. (1973). Consonant confusions in noise: A study of perceptual features: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 54(5) Nov 1973, 1248-1266.
  • Ward, L. M., & Mori, S. (1996). Attention cueing aids auditory intensity resolution: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(3) Sep 1996, 1722-1727.
  • Warren, R. M. (1984). Perceptual restoration of obliterated sounds: Psychological Bulletin Vol 96(2) Sep 1984, 371-383.
  • Warren, R. M., Wrightson, J. M., & Puretz, J. (1988). Illusory continuity of tonal and infratonal periodic sounds: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 84(4) Oct 1988, 1338-1342.
  • Watson, C. S., Foyle, D. C., & Kidd, G. R. (1990). Limits of auditory pattern discrimination for patterns with various durations and numbers of components: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(6) Dec 1990, 2631-2638.
  • Watson, C. S., Foyle, D. C., & Kidd, G. R. (1992). "Limits of auditory pattern discrimination for patterns with various durations and numbers of components": Erratum: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1168.
  • Watson, C. S., Kellogg, S. C., Kawanishi, D. T., & Lucas, P. A. (1973). The uncertain response in detection-oriented psychophysics: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 99(2) Jul 1973, 180-185.
  • Watts, F. (1973). Mechanisms of fluency control in stutterers: British Journal of Disorders of Communication Vol 8(2) Oct 1973, 131-138.
  • Werner, L. A., & Bargones, J. Y. (1991). Sources of auditory masking in infants: Distraction effects: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 50(5) Nov 1991, 405-412.
  • Werner, L. A., & Boike, K. (2001). Infants' sensitivity to broadband noise: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(5,Pt1) May 2001, 2103-2111.
  • Weston, P. B., Miller, J. D., & Hirsh, I. J. (1965). Release from masking for speech: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38(6) 1965, 1053-1054.
  • Wiegrebe, L., & Krumbholz, K. (1999). Temporal resolution and temporal masking properties of transient stimuli: Data and an auditory model: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(5) May 1999, 2746-2756.
  • Wightman, F. L., Callahan, M. R., Lutfi, R. A., Kistler, D. J., & Oh, E. (2003). Children's detection of pure-tone signals: Informational masking with contracterai maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 113(6) Jun 2003, 3297-3305.
  • Wilbanks, W. A. (1979). Binaural detection as a function of signal frequency and noise level: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 14(6) Dec 1979, 449-452.
  • Wilbanks, W. A. (1983). Effect of signal delay on auditory detection with gated uncorrelated noise: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(1) Jan 1983, 35-36.
  • Wilbanks, W. A. (1983). Masking of the signal for lateralization of tones: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(2) Mar 1983, 138-140.
  • Wilcott, R. C., & Gales, R. S. (1954). Comparison of the masked thresholds of a simulated moving and stationary auditory signal: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(6) Jun 1954, 451-456.
  • Wilson, J. R., & Krishnan, A. (2005). Human Frequency-Following Responses to Binaural Masking Level Difference Stimuli: Journal of the American Academy of Audiology Vol 16(3) Mar 2005, 184-195.
  • Wilson, R. H., Civitello, B. A., & Margolis, R. H. (1985). Influence of interaural level differences on the speech recognition masking level difference: Audiology Vol 24(1) Jan-Feb 1985, 15-24.
  • Wilson, R. H., & Fowler, C. G. (1986). Effects of signal duration on the 500-Hz masking-level difference: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 15(4) 1986, 209-215.
  • Wilson, R. H., & Fugleberg, R. A. (1987). Influence of signal duration on the masking-level difference: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 30(3) Sep 1987, 330-334.
  • Wilson, R. H., Hopkins, J. L., Mance, C. M., & Novak, R. E. (1982). Detection and recognition masking-level differences for the individual CID W-1 spondaic words: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(2) Jun 1982, 235-242.
  • Wilson, R. H., Shanks, J. E., & Koebsell, K. A. (1982). Recognition masking-level differences for 10 CID W-1 spondaic words: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 25(4) Dec 1982, 624-628.
  • Winkler, I., Reinikainen, K., & Naatanen, R. (1993). Event-related brain potentials reflect traces of echoic memory in humans: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 53(4) Apr 1993, 443-449.
  • Wojtczak, M., Donaldson, G. S., & Viemeister, N. F. (2003). Intensity discrimination and increment detection in cochlear-implant users: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 396-407.
  • Wojtczak, M., Schroder, A. C., Kong, Y.-Y., & Nelson, D. A. (2001). The effect of basilar-membrane nonlinearity on the shapes of masking period patterns in normal and impaired hearing: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 109(4) Apr 2001, 1571-1586.
  • Wolf, C. G. (1976). A recognition masking study of consonant processing: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 19(1) Jan 1976, 35-46.
  • Wolpaw, J. R., & Cacace, A. T. (1994). The influence of stimulus intensity, contralateral masking and handedness on the temporal N1 and the T complex components of the auditory N1 wave, by John F. Connolly: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 91(1) Jul 1994, 71-73.
  • Wright, B. A. (1992). The role of envelope fluctuations in an apparent demonstration of suppression in simultaneous masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(6) Jun 1992, 3436-3442.
  • Wright, B. A. (1995). Detectability of simultaneously masked signals as a function of signal bandwidth for different signal delays: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(5, Pt 1) Nov 1995, 2493-2503.
  • Wright, B. A. (1996). Auditory filter asymmetry at 2000 Hz in 80 normal-hearing ears: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(3) Sep 1996, 1717-1721.
  • Wright, B. A. (1996). Correlated individual differences in conditions used to measure psychophysical suppression and signal enhancement: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 100(5, Pt 1) Nov 1996, 3295-3303.
  • Wright, B. A. (1999). Strategies used to detect auditory signals in small sets of random maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 105(3) Mar 1999, 1765-1775.
  • Wright, B. A. (2006). Perceptual Learning of Temporally Based Auditory Skills Thought to Be Deficient in Children With Specific Language Impairment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Wright, B. A., & Dai, H. (1994). Detection of unexpected tones in gated and continuous maskers: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(2) Feb 1994, 939-948.
  • Wright, B. A., & Dai, H. (1994). Detection of unexpected tones with short and long durations: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 95(2) Feb 1994, 931-938.
  • Wright, B. A., Lombardino, L. J., King, W. M., Puranik, C. S., & et al. (1997). Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children: Nature Vol 387(6629) May 1997, 176-178.
  • Wright, B. A., & McFadden, D. (1990). Uncertainty about the correlation among temporal envelopes in two comodulation tasks: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(3) Sep 1990, 1339-1350.
  • Wright, B. A., & McFadden, D. (1992). Evidence that adaptation of suppression cannot account for auditory enhancement or enhanced forward masking. New York, NY: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
  • Wright, B. A., McFadden, D., & Champlin, C. A. (1993). Adaptation of suppression as an explanation of enhancement effects: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 94(1) Jul 1993, 72-82.
  • Yacullo, W. S. (1983). Detection cues in forward masking and their relationship to off-frequency listening: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Yama, M. F. (1992). Effects of temporal separation and masker level on binaural analysis in forward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(1) Jan 1992, 327-335.
  • Yamada, O., Kodera, K., Hink, R. F., & Suzuki, J.-I. (1979). Cochlear distribution of frequency-following response initiation: A high-pass masking noise study: Audiology Vol 18(5) Sep-Oct 1979, 381-387.
  • Yampolsky, S., Waters, G., Caplan, D., Matthies, M., & Chiu, P. (2002). Effects of acoustic degradtion on syntactic processing: Implications for the nature of the sresource system used in language processing: Brain and Cognition Vol 48(2-3) Mar-Apr 2002, 617-625.
  • Yampolsky, S. A. (2004). On-line syntactic processing under noise masking and concurrent memory load. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences.
  • Yanez, E. A., Siegel, G. M., Garber, S. R., & Wellen, C. J. (1982). The effects of different masking noises on children with /s/ or /r/ errors: Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders Vol 47(2) May 1982, 150-153.
  • Yasin, I., & Plack, C. J. (2003). The effects of a high-frequency suppressor on tuning curves and derived basilar-membrane response functions: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 114(1) Jul 2003, 322-332.
  • Yonovitz, A., Thompson, C. L., & Lozar, J. (1979). Masking level differences: Auditory evoked responses with homophasic and antiphasic signal and noise: Journal of Speech & Hearing Research Vol 22(2) Jun 1979, 403-411.
  • Yost, W. A. (1975). Frequency selectivity using tonal-temporal masking: Diotic vs. dichotic masking: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 18(6) Dec 1975, 423-427.
  • Yost, W. A. (1988). The masking-level difference and overall masker level: Restating the internal noise hypothesis: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 83(4) Apr 1988, 1517-1521.
  • Yost, W. A., & Klein, A. J. (1979). Thresholds of filtered transients: Audiology Vol 18(1) Jan-Feb 1979, 17-23.
  • Yost, W. A., Nielsen, D. W., Tanis, D. C., & Bergert, B. (1974). Tone-on-tone binaural masking with an antiphasic masker: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 15(2) Apr 1974, 233-237.
  • Yost, W. A., Penner, M. J., & Feth, L. L. (1972). Signal detection as a function of contralateral sinusoid-to-noise ratio: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 51(6, Pt 2) Jun 1972, 1966-1970.
  • Yost, W. A., Sheft, S., & Opie, J. (1989). Modulation interference in detection and discrimination of amplitude modulation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 86(6) Dec 1989, 2138-2147.
  • You, C. H., Koh, S. N., & Rahardja, S. (2006). Masking-based beta -order MMSE speech enhancement: Speech Communication Vol 48(1) Jan 2006, 57-70.
  • You, C. H., Koh, S. N., & Rahardja, S. (2007). Subband Kalman filtering incorporating masking properties for noisy speech signal: Speech Communication Vol 49(7-8) Jul 2007, 558-573.
  • Young, L. L., Goodman, J. T., & Carhart, R. (1979). Effects of whitening and peak-clipping on speech intelligibility in the presence of a competing message: Audiology Vol 18(1) Jan-Feb 1979, 72-79.
  • Youngblood, J., & Martin, F. N. (1981). On the relationship of stapedial contraction to central masking: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 21(1) Jan 1981, 45-49.
  • Zee, E. (1981). Effect of vowel quality on perception of post-vocalic nasal consonants in noise: Journal of Phonetics Vol 9(1) Jan 1981, 35-48.
  • Zelniker, T., Rattok, J., & Medem, A. (1974). Selective listening and threshold for tones appearing on a relevant and on an irrelevant input channel: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 16(1) Aug 1974, 50-52.
  • Zeng, F.-g. (1992). Intensity discrimination in the presence of forward masking: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Zeng, F.-G. (1994). Loudness growth in forward masking: Relation to intensity discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 96(4) Oct 1994, 2127-2132.
  • Zeng, F.-G. (1998). Interactions of forward and simultaneous masking in intensity discrimination: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 103(4) Apr 1998, 2021-2030.
  • Zeng, F.-g., & Turner, C. W. (1992). Intensity discrimination in forward masking: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 92(2, Pt 1) Aug 1992, 782-787.
  • Zhang, T., & Formby, C. (2007). Effects of cueing and auditory temporal masking: Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol 50(3) Jun 2007, 564-575.
  • Zheng, L., Early, S. J., Mason, C. R., Idrobo, F., Harrison, J. M., & Carney, L. H. (2002). Binaural detection with narrowband and wideband reproducible noise maskers: II. Results for rabbit: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 111(1,Pt1) Jan 2002, 346-356.
  • Zhou, B. (1995). Auditory filter shapes at high frequencies: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 98(4) Oct 1995, 1935-1955.
  • Zurek, P. M., & Delhorne, L. A. (1987). Consonant reception in noise by listeners with mild and moderate sensorineural hearing impairment: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 82(5) Nov 1987, 1548-1559.
  • Zwicker, E. (1965). Temporal effects in simultaneous masking and loudness: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38(1) 1965, 132-141.
  • Zwicker, E. (1965). Temporal effects in simultaneous masking by white-noise bursts: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 37(4) 1965, 653-663.
  • Zwicker, E. (1980). Reversed behaviour of masking at low levels: Audiology Vol 19(4) Jul-Aug 1980, 330-334.
  • Zwicker, E., & Martner, O. (1990). On the dependence of (f-sub-2-f-sub-1) difference tones on subject and on additional masker: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 88(3) Sep 1990, 1351-1358.
  • Zwicker, E., & Scharf, B. (1965). A model of loudness summation: Psychological Review Vol 72(1) Jan 1965, 3-26.
  • Zwicker, E., & Schorn, K. (1978). Psychoacoustical tuning curves in audiology: Audiology Vol 17(2) 1978, 120-140.
  • Zwicker, E., & Schorn, K. (1982). Temporal resolution in hard-of-hearing patients: Audiology Vol 21(6) Nov-Dec 1982, 474-492.
  • Zwicker, U. T. (1984). Auditory recognition of diotic and dichotic vowel pairs: Speech Communication Vol 3(4) Dec 1984, 265-277.
  • Zwislocki, J. J. (1972). A theory of central auditory masking and its partial validation: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 52(2, Pt 2) Aug 1972, 644-659.
  • Hall, J W, Grose, J H & Mendoza, L (1995) "Across-channel processes in masking" in Moore, B C J (ed) "Handbook of perception and cognition, 2nd Edition, Vol 4, Hearing" Academic, New York.
  • Moore, B C J (1995) "Frequency analysis and masking" in Moore, B C J (ed) "Handbook of perception and cognition, 2nd Edition, Vol 4, Hearing" Academic, New York.
  • Oxenham, A.J. Plack, C.J. Suppression and the upward spread of masking, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104 (6) pp.3500-3510


Additional material

Books

Papers

External links

This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).
  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 1.6 1.7 1.8 Gelfand, S.A. (2004) Hearing- An Introduction to Psychological and Physiological Acoustics 4th Ed. New York, Marcel Dekker
  2. Moore, B.C.J. (2004) An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th Ed. London, Elsevier Academic Press
  3. 3.0 3.1 Moore, B.C.J. (1986) Frequency Selectivity in Hearing, London, Academic Press
  4. Moore, B.C.J. (1995) Perceptual Consequences of Cochlear Damage, Oxford, Oxford University Press
  5. 5.00 5.01 5.02 5.03 5.04 5.05 5.06 5.07 5.08 5.09 5.10 5.11 Moore, B.C.J. (1998) Cochlear Hearing Loss, London, Whurr Publishers Ltd
  6. Sellars, P. (2000), Behind the Mask, Cambridge: Sound on Sound, http://www.soundonsound.com/sos/may00/articles/mp3.htm, retrieved on 26 February 2007 
  7. Oxenham, A.J. Plack, C.J. Suppression and the upward spread of masking, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104 (6) pp.3500–3510
Advertisement